Animals respond to sudden challenges with a coordinated set of physiological and behavioral responses that enhance the ability to cope with stressors. While general characteristics of the vertebrate stress response are well described, it is not as clear how individual components covary between or within individuals. A rapid increase in glucocorticoids coordinates the stress response and one of the primary downstream results is an increase in glucose availability via reduced glucose utilization. Here, we asked whether between- and within-individual variation in corticosterone directly predict variation in glucose. We collected 2673 paired glucose and corticosterone measures from 776 tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) from four populations spanning the species range. In adults, glucose and corticosterone both increased during a standardized restraint protocol in all four populations. Moreover, in one population experimentally increasing a precursor that stimulates corticosterone release resulted in a further increase in both measures. In contrast, nestlings did not show a robust glucose response to handling or manipulation. Despite this group-level variation, there was very little evidence in any population that between-individual variation in corticosterone predicted between-individual variation in glucose regulation. Glucose was moderately repeatable within individuals, but within-individual variation in glucose and corticosterone were unrelated. Our results highlight the fact that a strong response in one aspect of the coordinated acute stress response (corticosterone) does not necessarily indicate that specific downstream components, such as glucose, will show similarly strong responses. These results have implications for understanding the evolution of integrated stress response systems.

Wild animals live in capricious environments where sudden challenges are encountered regularly. Successfully navigating these challenges requires an integrated physiological and behavioral response (Wingfield et al., 1998). In vertebrates, acute challenges trigger both the catecholamine regulated ‘fight-or-flight’ response and the glucocorticoid mediated acute stress response (Sapolsky et al., 2000). When rapidly elevated, glucocorticoids organize a variety of physiological and behavioral changes that facilitate the successful response to an acute challenge along with recovery and preparation for subsequent challenges (Romero et al., 2009; Sapolsky et al., 2000). In the past 15 years, attention has shifted from describing the general response to investigating how and why individuals differ and what consequences this variation may have for fitness and the evolution of physiological systems (Bonier et al., 2009; Breuner et al., 2008). While some patterns have emerged (Schoenle et al., 2021), the lack of consistent links between fitness and glucocorticoids, coupled with substantial within-individual variation in components of the stress response across time, contexts or even different tissues in one animal has led to some debate about how to interpret studies that seek to link fitness with glucocorticoids without measuring other aspects of the multifaceted stress response (Gormally et al., 2020; Lattin et al., 2015; Romero and Gormally, 2019). Addressing these critiques will require more studies of how different aspects of the stress response system covary within and between individuals.

One of the actions of glucocorticoids during an acute stress response is to alter glucose homeostasis and increase the availability of glucose (Kuo et al., 2015). Regulation is accomplished through multiple routes; first, glucocorticoids act antagonistically to insulin in peripheral tissue and decrease glucose uptake across cell membranes by removing glucose transporters from cell surfaces, thereby increasing circulating glucose availability (Horner et al., 1987; Remage-Healey and Romero, 2001; Romero and Beattie, 2021). At the same time, glucocorticoids promote gluconeogenesis in the liver and alter glucose homeostasis through a variety of other indirect pathways (Kuo et al., 2015). In the context of the acute vertebrate stress response, glucocorticoids are thought to increase the availability of glucose for use by the brain and to allow animals to cope with and recover from an acute challenge (Kuo et al., 2015; Remage-Healey and Romero, 2001). These changes in glucose availability are often interpreted as mobilizing energy to promote short-term survival, but the biggest increase in circulating glucose actually results from a decrease in glucose utilization, rather than increased production (Romero and Beattie, 2021).

Although the general pattern of acute challenges increasing glucose availability is often taken as a fundamental component of the stress response (McEwen and Wingfield, 2003; Sapolsky et al., 2000), there is a great deal of variation and context dependence in this response both within and between species (Remage-Healey and Romero, 2001; Romero et al., 2009). For example, in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris), handling stress increases both corticosterone – the primary glucocorticoid in birds – and glucose, but only in samples taken during or shortly after night-time (Remage-Healey and Romero, 2000). In a wild population of Rufous-winged sparrows (Peucaea capalis), glucose increases, decreases or stays the same in response to handling stress, depending on the stage in the annual breeding cycle (Deviche et al., 2016b). Thus, the magnitude of the glucose response to a stressor – and whether a response occurs at all – may differ with season, time of day and nutrition. The reactive scope model specifically predicts that increased glucose may only be pronounced in animals that are tested during a fasted state, as was the case for many early lab based studies (Romero et al., 2009). A small number of studies published to date demonstrate that baseline glucose can be moderately repeatable and associated with individual performance (Montoya et al., 2018, 2020). Across many more species, baseline and stress-induced glucocorticoid levels are also moderately repeatable within individuals (Taff et al., 2018a), but repeatability of each trait does not necessarily imply covariation within or between individuals.

Despite the mechanistic link between glucocorticoids and glucose, relatively few studies have investigated changes in the two measures simultaneously in wild animals (but see Deviche et al., 2016a,b). Even fewer studies have investigated whether between-individual variation in the absolute value or magnitude of change in glucocorticoids during an acute stress response predicts the degree of change in glucose availability. In a recent study of wandering gartersnakes (Thamnophis elegans vagrans), Neuman-Lee et al. (2020) used both observational and experimental data to show that while glucose did increase with an acute stress response, individual differences in the amount of glucocorticoid change did not predict the magnitude of the change in circulating glucose (see also Gangloff et al., 2017). The generality of this lack of relationship has important implications for understanding how selection operates on variation in the physiological stress response. There is an assumption – often implicit – in evolutionary physiology that a stronger glucocorticoid response to a stressor will result in a similarly stronger change in downstream regulation, including a larger increase in glucose (Romero and Gormally, 2019). However, this assumption is rarely directly tested and if substantial regulation occurs in other components of the system (e.g. in tissue-specific receptor density; Lattin et al., 2015) then producing a similar change in glucose might require different levels of glucocorticoids in two different animals. Moreover, individuals may differ in their degree of within-individual covariation (how tightly linked glucocorticoids and glucose are within the same individual when measured multiple times). Even if glucocorticoids and glucose are tightly coupled within individuals, there is no guarantee that within-individual trait correlations will scale to between-individual covariation (Agrawal, 2020).

Here, we studied between- and within-individual covariation in glucose and corticosterone regulation during an acute stress response in the breeding season of wild tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor). We measured glucose and corticosterone repeatedly at baseline and stress-induced levels for breeding adults from four populations with different climate variability along with nestlings from one population. In the main population, we also coupled observational data with experimental manipulations that directly increased circulating corticosterone in adults and nestlings by injection with synthetic adrenocorticotropic hormone to determine whether there was a causal effect of additional corticosterone on subsequent glucose levels.

We first predicted that glucose and corticosterone would be relatively low in baseline samples, be elevated in stress-induced samples, and reach their highest values in samples where corticosterone was experimentally elevated. Next, we predicted that if variation in corticosterone levels is the direct cause of glucose elevation, then individual variation in circulating corticosterone and in the magnitude of the natural or experimental increase in corticosterone would be correlated with glucose levels or the increase in glucose levels over the same time period. Alternatively, if glucose and corticosterone regulation do not covary, it would suggest that these two aspects of the acute stress response can be regulated relatively independently or that their link depends critically on additional traits (e.g. nutritional state). To test the prediction of the reactive scope model that glucose only increases robustly after fasting, we included interactions between corticosterone and mass (as a proxy for fasted state) in these models. Finally, using a subset of individuals that were measured multiple times, we assessed the degree to which corticosterone and glucose covaried within individuals.

General field methods

We studied tree swallows Tachycineta bicolor (Vieillot 1808) breeding near Ithaca, NY, USA (42.4°N, 76.5°W) from 2016 to 2019. In each year, we monitored nest boxes using established protocols for this population (Winkler et al., 2020). Adult females were captured at the nest box up to 3 times each breeding season (1–2 times during incubation and 1–2 times after nestlings had hatched). Adult males were typically captured once per year, 3–8 days after nestlings had hatched. Nestlings were sampled on days 12 and 15 after hatching. All adults were captured between 06:00 h and 10:00 h, and nestlings were sampled between 12:00 h and 15:00 h to limit variation in physiological measurements associated with circadian patterns. Adults and nestlings were sampled during different time windows because it is not logistically possible to sample them in the same period.

At each capture, we took a measurement of individual mass to the nearest 0.25 g and added a unique USGS aluminium band to any individual that was not previously banded. For most captures, we took a series of blood samples by brachial venipuncture to measure corticosterone and glucose. First, we collected a baseline sample within 2 min of disturbance (<70 µl). Birds were then held in a closed bag for 30 min, after which we collected a stress-induced blood sample (<30 µl). For a subset of adults, immediately after the stress-induced sample, we manipulated the course of the stress response by injecting Cortrosyn (a synthetic version of adrenocorticotropic hormone, ACTH) and collected a final blood sample 30 min later (<30 µl; see details below). We performed the same manipulation on nestlings, except in this case the Cortrosyn sample was collected on a separate day from the baseline and stress-induced sample (see below).

We measured glucose from baseline, stress-induced and post-Cortrosyn blood samples at the time of collection using a handheld glucose meter and test strips (FreeStyle, Abbott Diabetes Care, Alameda, CA, USA). Similar devices have been used in previous studies of wild birds (Clinchy et al., 2004; Malisch et al., 2018), and this device was previously validated to provide repeatable measures of glucose in this population of tree swallows (Taff et al., 2021). The remaining blood sample was stored on ice in the field for <3 h. Plasma was then separated by centrifugation and stored frozen until corticosterone was measured with an ELISA kit that was previously validated in this population (Arbor Assays K014-H5; Taff et al., 2019b).

During 2016–2019, adult females in the population were subjected to a variety of manipulative experiments (Taff et al., 2019a, 2018b; Zimmer et al., 2019). In this study, we combined samples from several prior studies with samples that had not previously been included in published studies as long as we had measures of both corticosterone and glucose. We excluded samples from individuals that had been subject to direct manipulations of corticosterone from multi-day dosing because we were interested in the immediate effects of acute corticosterone increases on glucose (Vitousek et al., 2018b), but we included samples from treatments that manipulated flight costs, perceived predator presence or plumage coloration. Males did not receive any direct manipulation and we include all male samples. In the one year that nestling glucose was measured (2019), adult females had received plumage dulling or simulated predation treatments prior to nestling sampling. However, these treatments only targeted adult females and we included all nestling samples. As part of the adult experiments during this year, eggs at most nests were cross-fostered prior to the onset of incubation so that each nest typically included eggs from multiple females; we included nest identity as a random effect in all of our models but did not specifically investigate any effects of cross-fostering.

Comparative population study

In parallel with the study described above in Ithaca, NY, USA, we collected similar data from adult tree swallows breeding in McCarthy, AK, USA (2016–2017; 61.4°N, 143.3°W), Burgess Junction, WY, USA (2018; 44.5°N, 107.3°W), and Chattanooga, TN, USA (2018; 35.1°N, 85.2°W). Sampling schedules and details of sampling in each location were identical to those described above for the NY population. Full descriptions of these study locations can be found in Zimmer et al. (2020). Inclusion criteria were the same as described above for the NY population. Corticosterone measurements from the other populations were not available for most adult males, so analyses comparing the four populations are restricted only to adult females. We did not collect glucose measurements from nestlings in these populations and we do not report data from any post-injection measures in these additional populations. Baseline and stress-induced samples were collected exactly as described above.

Manipulating the corticosterone response

In the NY population only, we used manipulations to artificially increase the magnitude of the corticosterone response to handling stress in 2019 to determine whether there was a direct causal effect of continued corticosterone release on circulating glucose levels. Most adults and nestlings in 2019 received an injection of Cortrosyn (synthetic ACTH).

For nestlings, the time course of sampling differed slightly from that for adults. For all nestlings, we collected a series of samples on day 12 that included a baseline and stress-induced sample as described above for adults. On day 15, we returned to each nestling and collected a post-Cortrosyn sample at a single time point. For this last sample, nestlings were injected immediately after removal from the nest and then a single blood sample was collected 30 min later.

Validation of Cortrosyn injection effects

To ensure that Cortrosyn had the desired effect in elevating the corticosterone response, we conducted two validation studies on a separate set of nestlings and adults that were not part of the main study presented here. Once reconstituted, Cortrosyn is not stable at room temperature. Therefore, rather than delivering exact doses based on individual mass, we reconstituted vials of lyophilized Cortrosyn (Amphastar Pharmaceutical Incorporated, item #054881) and prepared aliquots pre-measured into syringes based on the average mass in our population (on day 15, nestlings weigh approximately the same amount as full-grown adults and the same dose was used for both adults and nestlings). Aliquoted Cortrosyn doses were stored frozen at −20°C for <2 weeks and thawed immediately before injection.

In 2018, we carried out a validation experiment on 15 day old nestlings from 9 nests. At each nest, individual nestlings were alternately assigned to a Cortrosyn injection group (n=23 nestlings) or a control group that received a saline injection (n=20 nestlings). For all nestlings, a baseline blood sample (<30 µl) was collected within 3 min of disturbance and then nestlings were immediately injected with either 50 µl of saline or 50 µl of 0.1 μg μl−1 freshly thawed Cortrosyn. Following injection, two additional blood samples (<30 µl) were collected, 15 and 30 min after injection.

In 2019, we carried out a separate validation experiment on adult females captured during incubation that were not part of the main study presented here. For each female, we collected a baseline blood sample (<70 µl) within 3 min of disturbance. All females received a 50 µl saline injection immediately after this baseline sample was collected and then had a second blood sample (<30 µl) taken 30 min later. Immediately after this second sample, females were injected with either an additional saline dose of the same volume (n=9) or a dose of Cortrosyn (n=9; 50 µl at 0.1 μg μl−1). Thirty minutes after this second injection, a final blood sample (<30 µl) was collected. For both adults and nestlings in these validation experiments, blood samples were processed and corticosterone was measured exactly as described for the main experiment.

To compare corticosterone between treatment groups, we fitted a single model for each dataset (adults and nestlings). For nestlings, we fitted a linear mixed model with corticosterone measurement as the response, an interaction between treatment and sampling time point (fitted as a factor) as predictors, and individual identity as a random effect. Because multiple nestlings were sampled from the same nest, this model included a random effect for nest of origin. For adults, we fitted a similar model, except that there was no need for a random effect for nest as only one female was sampled at each nest. We used the full models to compare circulating corticosterone in saline-versus Cortrosyn-injected birds at each of the three time points. Significance in these mixed models was assessed with P-values based on Satterthwaite's Method implemented by the ‘lmerTest’ package in R (Kuznetsova et al., 2017).

Data analysis

Using a subset of adult samples from all populations where multiple measures were available, we first evaluated overall unadjusted repeatability in baseline, stress-induced and stress-induced minus baseline glucose and corticosterone in a linear mixed model fitted with the ‘rptR’ package in R (Stoffel et al., 2017). We next sought to determine whether glucose and corticosterone differed at a group level for the three different sample types using data from the NY population (baseline, stress-induced and post-Cortrosyn). We fitted a single linear mixed model separately for adults and nestlings to address this question, with glucose or corticosterone as the response variable and sample type as a categorical predictor. The adult model included an additional fixed effect for sex and a random effect of bird identity to account for repeated sampling from the same individual. The nestling model included random effects for individual identity and for nest identity to account for the fact that nestlings sampled from the same nest are not independent.

We next asked whether between-individual variation in circulating corticosterone predicted variation in glucose levels. For these analyses, we fitted a set of three models separately for adults and nestlings with baseline glucose or change in glucose (baseline to stress-induced or stress-induced to post-Cortrosyn) as the response variable. Predictor variables included baseline corticosterone or the change in corticosterone over the same sampling interval. The adult model included a random effect for individual identity to account for repeated sampling, and the nestling model included a random effect for nest identity to account for non-independence. The adult post-Cortrosyn model did not include any repeat sampling, so it was fitted as a simple linear model with no random effects. In these models, we also included mass and an interaction between mass and the corticosterone predictor to test whether corticosterone and glucose were more tightly linked under conditions of food limitation. If there was no support for the mass by corticosterone interaction, we removed this effect from the final model for simplicity.

To determine whether there were population differences in glucose regulation, we fitted three linear mixed models with baseline, stress-induced or stress-induced minus baseline glucose as the response variable and population as a categorical predictor. These analyses included only baseline and stress-induced sample types in adult females. Next, we fitted models similar to those described above with either baseline or stress-induced minus baseline glucose as the response and with corticosterone over the same interval, mass, and a corticosterone by mass interaction as predictors. These models were fitted separately for each population and included female identity as a random effect.

Finally, we assessed whether there was any evidence for within-individual covariation between glucose and corticosterone using a subset of individuals that had four or more sampling events. This level of repeated sampling was only available from the NY population. Using this subset, we centered and standardized both glucose and corticosterone within each individual and then fitted models with baseline, stress-induced or the change in glucose from baseline to stress-induced as the response variable and with corticosterone over the same time period as the predictor. By centering within individuals, we used these models to ask whether an increase in corticosterone – relative to an individual's own mean corticosterone level over all observations – was associated with an increase in glucose relative to their own mean glucose level across observations (see discussion of within-individual mean centering in Westneat et al., 2020).

All linear mixed models except for repeatability estimates were fitted using the ‘lme4’ package version 1.1-26 (Bates et al., 2015). We assessed model fits by inspection of scaled residuals using the ‘DHARMa’ package in R (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=DHARMa). For all linear mixed models with categorical comparisons, we extracted means and 95% confidence intervals for each group using the ‘emmeans’ package in R with the Satterthwaite approximation (Lenth, 2020). We interpreted groups whose confidence intervals do not overlap to be significantly different. In cases where an interaction was supported, we illustrated the interaction by calculating confidence intervals across a range of predictor values by drawing 1,000,000 samples from the multivariate normal distribution of the fitted model using the ‘mvrnorm’ function in R package MASS version 7.5-53 (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MASS) and then calculating the highest posterior density interval with default settings (‘HPDI’ function) using package ‘rethinking’ version 2.01 in R (McElreath, 2020). All figures and analyses were produced in R version 4.0.2 (http://www.R-project.org/).

Ethical note

All work described here was approved by the Cornell University Institutional Animal Care & Use Board (IACUC protocol numbers 2001-0051 and 2019-0023). Capture and sampling of wild birds was approved by appropriate federal and state agencies (including federal banding permits # 24129 and 20576, and state permits NY-215, NY-2350, WY-1163 and TN-1425)).

In total, our NY analyses included corticosterone and glucose samples from 331 adults with 776 baseline, 586 stress-induced and 45 post-Cortrosyn samples. Of these 331 adults, 215 had repeated captures and these individuals were sampled 3.1±1.4 (mean±s.d.) times. For nestlings, we included samples from 187 nestlings in 43 nests. The population comparison also included baseline and stress-induced corticosterone and glucose measurements from 71, 75 and 112 adult females from AK, TN and WY, respectively. Of these 258 females, 176 had repeated captures and these individuals were sampled 2.9±0.8 times. A full table of sample sizes by age, location, year and sample type is given in Table S1.

Baseline, stress-induced and change in glucose levels had low, but significant repeatability (baseline r=0.11, 95% CI=0.02 to 0.20, likelihood ratio test P=0.02; stress-induced r=0.21, 95% CI=0.12 to 0.30, P<0.001; glucose response r=0.11, 95% CI=0.02 to 0.20, P=0.01). Baseline corticosterone was not repeatable in this sample (baseline corticosterone r=0.01, 95% CI=0 to 0.10, P=0.42). Stress-induced corticosterone and the change in corticosterone had the highest repeatability (stress-induced corticosterone r=0.32, 95% CI=0.23 to 0.41, P<0.001; corticosterone response r=0.25, 95% CI=0.16 to 0.35, P<0.001). Note that repeatability estimates for corticosterone, but not glucose, in a subset of these birds were previously reported with similar effect sizes (see table 5 in Vitousek et al., 2018a). For both glucose and corticosterone, there was a moderate correlation between baseline and stress-induced levels, while the change in levels was, unsurprisingly, correlated with raw values (full correlation matrix in Table S2).

Validation of Cortrosyn injection effects

Injection with Cortrosyn led to a clear increase in circulating corticosterone in both adults (Fig. 1A) and nestlings (Fig. 1B). For adults, circulating corticosterone increased from baseline to 30 min after capture. The two treatment groups did not differ at the first or second time point (before Cortrosyn was injected). However, by the final time point (30 min after injection), the Cortrosyn-injected group had significantly higher circulating corticosterone. This difference was driven by a continued increase in corticosterone from 30 to 60 min in the Cortrosyn group coupled with a stable circulating level from 30 to 60 min in the saline-injected group (linear mixed model n=54 measurements from 18 individuals; Cortrosyn compared with control at time point 3, β=30.28, 95% CI=19.75 to 40.81, P<0.001).

Fig. 1.

Circulating corticosterone levels in tree swallows before and after Cortrosyn or saline injection. Data are for adult females (A) and 15 day old nestlings (B) at the indicated times after capture, and following Cortrosyn (synthetic adrenocorticotropic hormone) or saline injection. Boxes and whiskers show the median, interquartile range (IQR) and largest value within 1.5 times the IQR.

Fig. 1.

Circulating corticosterone levels in tree swallows before and after Cortrosyn or saline injection. Data are for adult females (A) and 15 day old nestlings (B) at the indicated times after capture, and following Cortrosyn (synthetic adrenocorticotropic hormone) or saline injection. Boxes and whiskers show the median, interquartile range (IQR) and largest value within 1.5 times the IQR.

For nestlings, corticosterone increased in both groups from baseline to 15 min after capture but increased significantly more in the Cortrosyn-injected group than in the saline-injected group. From 15 to 30 min, saline-injected nestlings declined in circulating corticosterone, but Cotrosyn-injected nestlings continued to show a rise, resulting in an even larger difference in circulating corticosterone between the two groups at 30 min after capture (linear mixed model n=122 measurements, 43 individuals, 9 nests; Cortrosyn compared with control at time point 2, β=12.92, 95% CI=4.75 to 21.09, P=0.002; time point 3, β=27.28, 95% CI=19.18 to 35.39, P<0.001).

Overall changes in glucose and corticosterone

In NY adults, mean corticosterone levels differed significantly for all three sample types, although there was substantial overlap in the distribution of individual corticosterone measures (Fig. 2A; Table S3). Corticosterone was lowest in the baseline sample (emmeans estimated mean from mixed model 6.4 ng ml−1; 95% CI=4.9 to 7.8 ng ml−1) and increased substantially in the stress-induced sample (32.1 ng ml−1; 95% CI=30.6 to 33.6 ng ml−1). Injection with Cortrosyn resulted in a further increase of corticosterone (43.9 ng ml−1; 95% CI=39.7 to 48.2 ng ml−1).

Fig. 2.

Corticosterone and glucose levels of adult tree swallows in the NY population by sample type. (A) Corticosterone, (B) glucose. Boxes and whiskers show the median, IQR and largest value within 1.5 times the IQR. Small points show raw data. Solid diamonds and solid black lines show the point estimate and 95% confidence interval for each group mean as calculated by the emmeans package and the linear mixed model described in Materials and Methods.

Fig. 2.

Corticosterone and glucose levels of adult tree swallows in the NY population by sample type. (A) Corticosterone, (B) glucose. Boxes and whiskers show the median, IQR and largest value within 1.5 times the IQR. Small points show raw data. Solid diamonds and solid black lines show the point estimate and 95% confidence interval for each group mean as calculated by the emmeans package and the linear mixed model described in Materials and Methods.

Overall, glucose concentrations showed a similar pattern of variation between sample types to that for corticosterone (Fig. 2B; Table S3). Glucose was lowest in the initial sample (mean 209.3 mg dl−1; 95% CI=205.7 to 212.8 mg dl−1) and increased in the stress-induced sample (240.0 mg dl−1; 95% CI=236.3 to 243.7 mg dl−1). Post-Cortrosyn injection samples had the highest circulating glucose levels (270.0 mg dl−1; 95% CI=259.8 to 280.2 mg dl−1).

In NY nestlings, corticosterone increased from the initial to the stress-induced sample (Fig. 3A; initial mean 6.3 ng ml−1; 95% CI=3.1 to 9.4 ng ml−1; stress-induced=25.0 ng ml−1; 95% CI=21.8 to 28.1 ng ml−1). Cortrosyn injection resulted in the highest corticosterone levels, although the confidence interval for post-Cortrosyn samples overlapped that for stress-induced samples (29.2 ng ml−1; 95% CI=26.0 to 32.5 ng ml−1).

Fig. 3.

Corticosterone and glucose levels of nestling tree swallows in the NY population by sample type. (A) Corticosterone, (B) glucose. Boxes and whiskers show the median, IQR range and largest value within 1.5 times the IQR. Small points show raw data. Solid diamonds and solid black lines show the point estimate and 95% confidence interval for each group mean as calculated by the emmeans package and the linear mixed model described in Materials and Methods.

Fig. 3.

Corticosterone and glucose levels of nestling tree swallows in the NY population by sample type. (A) Corticosterone, (B) glucose. Boxes and whiskers show the median, IQR range and largest value within 1.5 times the IQR. Small points show raw data. Solid diamonds and solid black lines show the point estimate and 95% confidence interval for each group mean as calculated by the emmeans package and the linear mixed model described in Materials and Methods.

In contrast to adults and despite clear variation in corticosterone levels, the confidence intervals for glucose concentration overlapped across all three sample types (Fig. 3B). Although they did not differ significantly, glucose levels were lowest in the initial sample (mean=204.2 mg dl−1; 95% CI=194.6 to 213.8 mg dl−1) and increased in the stress-induced sample (222.2 mg dl−1; 95% CI=212.5 to 231.9 mg dl−1). Glucose did not increase further after Cortrosyn injection (219.0 mg dl−1; 95% CI=209.1 to 228.9 mg dl−1).

Between-individual covariation in glucose and corticosterone

For adults and nestlings in NY, there was no significant relationship between baseline corticosterone and baseline glucose, although nestlings with higher baseline corticosterone tended to have lower glucose levels (Fig. 4A; Table S4; adult β=1.7; 95% CI=−0.4 to 3.7; nestling β=−3.6; 95% CI=−10.7 to 3.5). Among nestlings, a greater increase in corticosterone from baseline to the stress-induced sample was associated with a smaller increase in glucose during the same period (Fig. 4B; β=−5.5; 95% CI=−11.0 to −0.1).

Fig. 4.

Covariation in between-individual variation in corticosterone and glucose for adult and nestling tree swallows in the NY population. (A) Baseline glucose versus baseline corticosterone (note, baseline corticosterone levels above 20 ng ml−1 are not shown but were included in analyses). (B) The change in glucose from baseline to stress-induced samples versus the change in corticosterone over the same period. (C) The change in both measures from stress-induced to post-Cortrosyn samples. In B and C, dashed lines indicate positive changes in corticosterone (right of the vertical line) and glucose (above the horizontal line).

Fig. 4.

Covariation in between-individual variation in corticosterone and glucose for adult and nestling tree swallows in the NY population. (A) Baseline glucose versus baseline corticosterone (note, baseline corticosterone levels above 20 ng ml−1 are not shown but were included in analyses). (B) The change in glucose from baseline to stress-induced samples versus the change in corticosterone over the same period. (C) The change in both measures from stress-induced to post-Cortrosyn samples. In B and C, dashed lines indicate positive changes in corticosterone (right of the vertical line) and glucose (above the horizontal line).

In contrast, among adults there was no overall relationship between the stress-induced increase in corticosterone and glucose (Fig. 4B), but there was an interaction between mass and the change in corticosterone from baseline to the stress-induced sample (Table S4; corticosterone by mass interaction β=4.3; 95% CI=0.7 to 8.0). Adults that were below average mass had a positive relationship between corticosterone increase and glucose increase during this period, while adults that were above average mass had a negative relationship (Fig. S1). However, this model only explained a small amount of variation in the glucose response (full model marginal R2=0.03; Table S4). Neither mass nor the change in corticosterone from stress-induced to post-Cortrosyn measurements was related to the change in glucose in adults or nestlings (Fig. 4C; Table S4).

Population comparison

When comparing population baseline glucose levels, the NY population had a higher circulating level than the AK and WY populations, but the confidence intervals for all other two-way comparisons overlapped (Fig. 5A; emmeans estimate for AK 206.4, 95% CI=202.6 to 210.2; NY 212.6, 95% CI=210.4 to 214.8; TN 208.2, 95% CI=204.3 to 212.1; WY 198.9, 95% CI=195.1 to 202.6). For stress-induced glucose, the NY population had higher levels than the AK and TN populations, but the other two-way comparisons had overlapping confidence intervals (Fig. 5B; AK 230.8, 95% CI=224.4 to 237.3; NY 242.1, 95% CI=238.4 to 245.8; TN 228.1, 95% CI=221.6 to 234.6; WY 239.3, 95% CI=232.8 to 245.8).

Fig. 5.

Population comparison of variation in glucose among adult female tree swallows. (A) Baseline, (B) stress-induced and (C) baseline versus stress-induced glucose levels. Boxes and whiskers show the median, IQR and largest value within 1.5 times the IQR. Small points show raw data. Solid diamonds and black lines show the point estimate and 95% confidence interval for each group mean as calculated by the emmeans package.

Fig. 5.

Population comparison of variation in glucose among adult female tree swallows. (A) Baseline, (B) stress-induced and (C) baseline versus stress-induced glucose levels. Boxes and whiskers show the median, IQR and largest value within 1.5 times the IQR. Small points show raw data. Solid diamonds and black lines show the point estimate and 95% confidence interval for each group mean as calculated by the emmeans package.

All four populations showed an increase in mean glucose levels between baseline and stress-induced samples (Fig. 5C). However, the WY population showed a greater increase over this period than the AK or TN population, while the other two-way comparisons did not differ (mean change in glucose AK 28.0, 95% CI=21.8 to 34.2; NY 32.4, 95% CI=28.7 to 36.0; TN 23.1, 95% CI=16.8 to 29.5; WY 42.2, 95% CI=35.8 to 48.6).

For baseline glucose, there was no evidence that between-individual variation in adult female corticosterone was related to variation in circulating glucose in any of the four populations (Table S5; coefficient estimate for baseline corticosterone AK β=1.5, 95% CI=−2.4 to 5.5; NY β=1.5, 95% CI=−0.7 to 3.7; TN β=−2.2, 95% CI=−6.6 to 2.3; WY β=−2.2, 95% CI=−7.3 to 2.9). Mass was negatively associated with baseline glucose levels in all four populations, although the confidence interval crossed zero in the TN population (Table S5; estimate for mass AK β=−4.1, 95% CI=−8.0 to −0.1; NY β=−3.0, 95% CI=−5.2 to −0.8, TN β=−3.2, 95% CI=−7.2 to 0.8; WY β=−4.8, 95% CI=−9.1 to −0.4). There was no support for an interaction between mass and baseline corticosterone on baseline glucose in any population (Table S5). For the change in glucose from baseline to stress-induced measures, there was no support for either a direct effect of the change in corticosterone, for mass alone, or for an interaction between corticosterone and mass in any population other than NY (Table S5). The NY population had a similar effect size for the corticosterone by mass interaction to that described above, although the confidence interval was wider in this subset of data with males excluded (see above).

Within-individual glucose and corticosterone covariation

For both baseline and stress-induced glucose, there was no evidence that deviations from the observed within-individual average corticosterone level were associated with similar deviations in the within-individual average glucose level (Fig. 6A,B; overall baseline β=0.1, 95% CI=−0.1 to 0.3; overall stress-induced β=−0.01, 95% CI=−0.2 to 0.2). For the stress-induced minus baseline measures, there was a tendency for higher within-individual corticosterone stress responses to be associated with stronger within-individual glucose responses, though the confidence interval for this effect included zero (Fig. 6C; overall response β=0.2, 95% CI=0.0 to 0.4).

Fig. 6.

Covariation in within-individual variation in corticosterone and glucose for tree swallows in the NY population. (A) Baseline glucose versus baseline corticosterone. (B) Stress-induced glucose versus stress-induced corticosterone. (C) The change in both measures from baseline to stress-induced levels. Data were standardized within individual from the NY population. Points show raw data; blue lines are regressions for each individual sampled at least 4 separate times; red line and confidence interval show the overall relationship across all birds. The black dashed line is included as a reference of no effect. By definition, each individual's regression passes through the origin where both corticosterone and glucose are at the mean value observed for that bird.

Fig. 6.

Covariation in within-individual variation in corticosterone and glucose for tree swallows in the NY population. (A) Baseline glucose versus baseline corticosterone. (B) Stress-induced glucose versus stress-induced corticosterone. (C) The change in both measures from baseline to stress-induced levels. Data were standardized within individual from the NY population. Points show raw data; blue lines are regressions for each individual sampled at least 4 separate times; red line and confidence interval show the overall relationship across all birds. The black dashed line is included as a reference of no effect. By definition, each individual's regression passes through the origin where both corticosterone and glucose are at the mean value observed for that bird.

At the group level, our results fit well with the canonical stress response. In adults, corticosterone and glucose increased from baseline to stress-induced samples and this result was robust across four widely spread populations. Furthermore, injection with Cortrosyn in the NY population resulted in an additional increase in both measures in adults, though not in nestlings. However, these group-level patterns largely failed to scale down to the between- and within-individual level. Between-individual variation in corticosterone was unrelated to variation in glucose at any time scale that we measured, except in one case when considered as an interaction with mass. This interaction supported a key prediction of the reactive scope model: that corticosterone and glucose should be more closely linked in fasted animals (Romero et al., 2009). However, even in that instance, the corticosterone response explained a very small amount of variation in the glucose response, suggesting that it is not safe to assume that strong responders in one aspect of the acute stress response will also have strong changes in other facets of the response. Furthermore, even at the within-individual level, there was no clear evidence that modulation of the corticosterone response was associated with similar modulation of the glucose response.

This lack of between- and within-individual covariation between corticosterone and glucose has important implications for evolutionary physiologists interested in understanding variation in the acute stress response. Many studies make an implicit assumption that strong corticosterone responders will be strong responders in other downstream components of the stress response (see critique in Romero and Gormally, 2019). However, much of the basis for this assumption comes from group-level comparisons (e.g. overall comparisons of baseline versus stress-induced samples) and recent papers have challenged the degree to which different components of the stress response system are correlated within or between individuals (Lattin et al., 2015; Neuman-Lee et al., 2020). Making inferences about individuals based on group-level differences is a classic logical error known as the ecological fallacy (Piantadosi et al., 1988). To varying degrees, evolutionary physiologists fall into this trap when trying to understand the fitness consequences of variation in the acute stress response from studies that measure only a single component (e.g. corticosterone). Understanding how and when different components of the stress response system covary – and when those correlations do or do not transfer across the within-individual, between-individual and between-population or between-species level – will require both better empirical data and a more robust scale-dependent framework for understanding trait correlations (Agrawal, 2020).

To a large extent, the limitation inherent in studies of the stress response that only measure corticosterone has long been recognized and the major barrier to progress is more logistical than conceptual. Studies in wild animals are still limited to disruptive sampling at a limited number of single time points. Recent conceptual papers have sought to explain the variation across these different time points by incorporating the time course of a stress response with systems biology (Del Giudice, 2012; Del Giudice et al., 2011) and within-individual reaction norms (Hau et al., 2016; Lema, 2014; Taff and Vitousek, 2016; Wada and Sewall, 2014). These approaches are powerful because they allow for a full description of the hierarchical nature of trait variation from within-individual levels up to population and species levels in a single coherent framework. However, fully embracing these approaches in empirical studies – particularly for multiple simultaneous physiological measures – requires the ability to repeatedly sample individual animals many times across different contexts. Despite a large number of samples over many years in our study, we only had 24 individuals that were sampled 4 or more times. Using those samples, we found no evidence for within-individual covariation in glucose and corticosterone, but this result would be much more robust if we were able to sample the same individual continuously (see discussion of power for within-individual reaction norms in van de Pol, 2012). Bio-logging devices capable of recording key physiological parameters continuously may revolutionize this field, though they are not yet capable of measuring many of the parameters that are most relevant for stress physiology (see discussion in Romero et al., 2015). In the meantime, an increased focus on theory and on empirical work that targets key assumptions (e.g. within- or between-individual covariation), rather than just documenting between-individual variation, will be useful.

Despite the fact that there was no between- or within-individual covariation in glucose and corticosterone, we found that both of these parameters were somewhat consistent within individuals. For corticosterone, we previously reported negligible repeatability of baseline values and moderate repeatability of stress-induced and stress-induced minus baseline values (Vitousek et al., 2018a) and we report similar patterns here. For glucose, all three measures were repeatable, but identity explained only a fairly small amount of variation in glucose levels (11–21%). These repeatability estimates are somewhat lower than those found in recent studies of zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), where both baseline and stress-induced glucose were found to be individually repeatable and associated with early life conditions and lifespan (multiple repeatability estimates reported ranging from 30% to 50%; Montoya et al., 2018; Montoya et al., 2020). There are few published estimates of glucose repeatability and it is not clear what caused our lower estimates, but one obvious difference is that our measurements were taken under more variable natural conditions rather than in captivity. As with corticosterone, baseline glucose is likely to be more influenced by current conditions – such as time since last feeding – than stress-induced samples are, and this might explain the lower repeatability for baseline measures.

It is also unclear what drove the difference that we observed between adults and nestlings. In general, nestling glucose was less responsive to both corticosterone elevation after handling and experimental increases in corticosterone. In chickens, several components of the glucose regulation system in chicks differ from those of adults (reviewed in Braun and Sweazea, 2008). It is possible that the 12–15 day old nestlings that we sampled had not yet fully developed the physiological systems that would result in a robust glucose response or that the time required to adjust glucose differs in nestlings. However, nestlings at this age do show a fully developed corticosterone response to experimental challenges. Alternatively, it is possible that the difference between adults and nestlings is an artefact of our sampling design. Nestlings were only sampled in a single year and the amount of variation in ecological conditions was narrower than for adults that were sampled over several years and populations. Nestlings may also be buffered from changes in ecological conditions if parents compensate for changes behaviorally (i.e. changing brooding and provisioning). Moreover, we sampled nestlings only in the afternoon, while adults were only sampled in the morning. Previous work in captive adult European starlings found that handling resulted in a glucose response during the night and early morning, but that this response disappeared during the daytime hours (Remage-Healey and Romero, 2000, 2001). Because our samples were restricted to narrow times of day for each age class, we could not evaluate whether the glucose response differed by time of day in this study.

When comparing different populations, we found that two general patterns – a negative association between mass and baseline glucose and a stress-induced increase in glucose – were robust across all populations. Despite large differences in ecological conditions and environmental predictability across these locations (Zimmer et al., 2020), there was little evidence for between-individual covariation between corticosterone and glucose in any population. It is interesting to note, however, that both the absolute levels of glucose and the magnitude of glucose increase differed in some cases. With only four populations to compare, it is unclear whether these differences are related to the different ecological challenges experienced by each population or whether they result from phenotypic plasticity versus local adaptation. While several comparative studies have investigated differences in baseline glucose in relation to mass and life history characteristics (Braun and Sweazea, 2008; Tomasek et al., 2019), we are not aware of any comparative data on differences in the magnitude of the change in glucose during an acute stress response between species. Large-scale comparative studies of baseline and stress-induced corticosterone have recently begun to illuminate how hormonal regulation covaries with life history differences (Vitousek et al., 2019). Using this approach to understand variation in the patterns of covariation between different physiological components of the acute stress response is a promising direction as multifaceted physiological responses are measured in more species. It is increasingly possible to measure a panel of physiological parameters in the field for many species with point-of-care devices (Morales et al., 2020). Moreover, this comparative approach is less constrained by the logistical challenges described above at the within- and between-individual level.

Taken together, our results demonstrate that physiological or behavioral changes that co-occur as part of the canonical stress response at a population or species level do not necessarily covary at the between- or within-individual level. While this presents major challenges for linking fitness to variation in the stress response at the individual level (Romero and Gormally, 2019), it also adds to a growing number of studies calling for a diversification of measures used to assess variation in the vertebrate stress response (Gormally et al., 2020). In particular, we advocate for integrating multiple measures with flexible analytical frameworks that are able to formally consider the hierarchical nature of variation between different components of the stress response system, as has been promoted recently for understanding behavioral variation (Allegue et al., 2016; Araya-Ajoy et al., 2015; Westneat et al., 2014). Moving forward, careful and clear thinking will be needed to ensure that studies intending to answer a question at one level (e.g. between individuals) have a study design and sampling regime that will work at that level and to ensure that implicit assumptions are tested empirically.

We would like to thank the many students and collaborators who helped with field season data collection and logistics over the years and locations described here. We also thank members of the Vitousek Lab group for feedback and discussion of this project.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: C.T., C.Z., T.A.R., D.A.A., D.R.A., S.J., A.P.R., M.N.V.; Methodology: C.T., C.Z.; Software: C.T.; Validation: C.T.; Formal analysis: C.T.; Investigation: C.T., C.Z., T.A.R., D.C.v.O., D.R.A., M.N.V.; Resources: M.N.V.; Data curation: C.T., C.Z., T.A.R., D.C.v.O.; Writing - original draft: C.T.; Writing - review & editing: C.Z., T.A.R., D.C.v.O., D.A.A., D.R.A., S.J., A.P.R., M.N.V.; Visualization: C.T.; Supervision: C.T., M.N.V.; Project administration: C.T., C.Z., M.N.V.

Funding

Research funding was provided by National Science Foundation IOS 1457251, U.S. Department of Agriculture NIFA Hatch 1017321, and Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency YFA D17AP00033 (to M.N.V.).

Data availability

Data and code required to reproduce all analyses, results and figures in this paper are available from Zenodo: doi:10.5281/zenodo.6011738; and GitHub: https://github.com/cct663/glucose_cort

Agrawal
,
A. A.
(
2020
).
A scale–dependent framework for trade–offs, syndromes, and specialization in organismal biology
.
Ecology
101
,
e02924
.
Allegue
,
H.
,
Araya-Ajoy
,
Y. G.
,
Dingemanse
,
N. J.
,
Dochtermann
,
N. A.
,
Garamszegi
,
L. Z.
,
Nakagawa
,
S.
,
Réale
,
D.
,
Schielzeth
,
H.
,
Westneat
,
D. F.
and
Hadfield
,
J.
(
2016
).
Statistical Quantification of Individual Differences (SQuID): An educational and statistical tool for understanding multilevel phenotypic data in linear mixed models
.
Method. Ecol. Evol.
8
,
257
-
267
.
Araya-Ajoy
,
Y. G.
,
Mathot
,
K. J.
and
Dingemanse
,
N. J.
(
2015
).
An approach to estimate short-term, long-term, and reaction norm repeatability
.
Method. Ecol. Evol.
6
,
1462
-
1473
.
Bates
,
D.
,
Maechler
,
M.
,
Bolker
,
B.
and
Walker
,
S.
(
2015
).
Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4
.
J. Stat. Softw.
67
,
1
-
48
.
Bonier
,
F.
,
Martin
,
P. R.
,
Moore
,
I. T.
and
Wingfield
,
J. C.
(
2009
).
Do baseline glucocorticoids predict fitness?
Trends Ecol. Evol.
24
,
634
-
642
.
Braun
,
E. J.
and
Sweazea
,
K. L.
(
2008
).
Glucose regulation in birds
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. B Biochem. Mol. Biol.
151
,
1
-
9
.
Breuner
,
C. W.
,
Patterson
,
S. H.
and
Hahn
,
T. P.
(
2008
).
In search of relationships between the acute adrenocortical response and fitness
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
157
,
288
-
295
.
Clinchy
,
M.
,
Zanette
,
L.
,
Boonstra
,
R.
,
Wingfield
,
J. C.
and
Smith
,
J. N.
(
2004
).
Balancing food and predator pressure induces chronic stress in songbirds
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
271
,
2473
-
2479
.
Del Giudice
,
M.
(
2012
).
Fetal programming by maternal stress: Insights from a conflict perspective
.
Psychoneuroendocrinology
37
,
1614
-
1629
.
Del Giudice
,
M.
,
Ellis
,
B. J.
and
Shirtcliff
,
E. A.
(
2011
).
The adaptive calibration model of stress responsivity
.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
35
,
1562
-
1592
.
Deviche
,
P.
,
Bittner
,
S.
,
Davies
,
S.
,
Valle
,
S.
,
Gao
,
S.
and
Carpentier
,
E.
(
2016a
).
Endocrine, metabolic, and behavioral effects of and recovery from acute stress in a free-ranging bird
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
234
,
95
-
102
.
Deviche
,
P.
,
Valle
,
S.
,
Gao
,
S.
,
Davies
,
S.
,
Bittner
,
S.
and
Carpentier
,
E.
(
2016b
).
The seasonal glucocorticoid response of male Rufous-winged Sparrows to acute stress correlates with changes in plasma uric acid, but neither glucose nor testosterone
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
235
,
78
-
88
.
Gangloff
,
E. J.
,
Sparkman
,
A. M.
,
Holden
,
K. G.
,
Corwin
,
C. J.
,
Topf
,
M.
and
Bronikowski
,
A. M.
(
2017
).
Geographic variation and within-individual correlations of physiological stress markers in a widespread reptile, the common garter snake (Thamnophis sirtalis)
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol.
205
,
68
-
76
.
Gormally
,
B. M. G.
,
Estrada
,
R.
,
McVey
,
M.
and
Romero
,
L. M.
(
2020
).
Beyond corticosterone: the acute stress response increases DNA damage in house sparrows
.
J. Exp. Zool. A Ecol. Integr. Physiol.
333
,
595
-
606
.
Hau
,
M.
,
Casagrande
,
S.
,
Ouyang
,
J.
and
Baugh
,
A.
(
2016
).
Glucocorticoid-mediated phenotypes in vertebrates: Multilevel variation and evolution
.
Adv. Study Behav.
48
,
41
-
115
.
Horner
,
H. C.
,
Munck
,
A.
and
Lienhard
,
G. E.
(
1987
).
Dexamethasone causes translocation of glucose transporters from the plasma membrane to an intracellular site in human fibroblasts
.
J. Biol. Chem.
262
,
17696
-
17702
.
Kuo
,
T.
,
McQueen
,
A.
,
Chen
,
T.-C.
and
Wang
,
J.-C.
(
2015
).
Regulation of glucose homeostasis by glucocorticoids
.
Adv. Exp. Med. Biol.
872
,
99
-
126
.
Kuznetsova
,
A.
,
Brockhoff
,
P. B.
and
Christensen
,
R. H. B.
(
2017
).
“lmerTest” Package: tests in linear mixed effects models
.
J. Stat. Soft.
82
,
0
-
32
.
Lattin
,
C. R.
,
Keniston
,
D. E.
,
Reed
,
J. M.
and
Romero
,
L. M.
(
2015
).
Are receptor concentrations correlated across tissues within individuals? a case study examining glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptor binding
.
Endocrinology
156
,
1354
-
1361
.
Lema
,
S. C.
(
2014
).
Hormones and phenotypic plasticity in an ecological context: Linking physiological mechanisms to evolutionary processes
.
Integr. Comp. Biol.
54
,
850
-
863
.
Lenth
,
R. V.
(
2020
).
emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R Package Version 1.5.3
.
Malisch
,
J. L.
,
Bennett
,
D. J.
,
Davidson
,
B. A.
,
Wenker
,
E. E.
,
Suzich
,
R. N.
and
Johnson
,
E. E.
(
2018
).
Stress-induced hyperglycemia in white-throated and white-crowned sparrows: a new technique for rapid glucose measurement in the field
.
Physiol. Biochem. Zool.
91
,
943
-
949
.
McElreath
,
R.
(
2020
).
Statistical Rethinking: A Bayesian Course with Examples in R and STAN
.
CRC Press
.
McEwen
,
B. S.
and
Wingfield
,
J. C.
(
2003
).
The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine
.
Horm. Behav.
43
,
2
-
15
.
Montoya
,
B.
,
Briga
,
M.
,
Jimeno
,
B.
,
Moonen
,
S.
and
Verhulst
,
S.
(
2018
).
Baseline glucose level is an individual trait that is negatively associated with lifespan and increases due to adverse environmental conditions during development and adulthood
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
188
,
517
-
526
.
Montoya
,
B.
,
Briga
,
M.
,
Jimeno
,
B.
and
Verhulst
,
S.
(
2020
).
Glucose regulation is a repeatable trait affected by successive handling in zebra finches
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
190
,
455
-
464
.
Morales
,
A.
,
Frei
,
B.
,
Leung
,
C.
,
Titman
,
R.
,
Whelan
,
S.
,
Benowitz-Fredericks
,
Z. M.
and
Elliott
,
K. H.
(
2020
).
Point-of-care blood analyzers measure the nutritional state of eighteen free-living bird species
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Mol. Integr. Physiol.
240
,
110594
.
Neuman-Lee
,
L. A.
,
Hudson
,
S. B.
,
Webb
,
A. C.
and
French
,
S. S.
(
2020
).
Investigating the relationship between corticosterone and glucose in a reptile
.
J. Exp. Biol.
223
,
jeb203885
.
Piantadosi
,
S.
,
Byar
,
D. P.
and
Green
,
S. B.
(
1988
).
The ecological fallacy
.
Am. J. Epidemiol.
127
,
893
-
904
.
Remage-Healey
,
L.
and
Romero
,
L. M.
(
2000
).
Daily and Seasonal Variation in Response to Stress in Captive Starlings (Sturnus Vulgaris): Glucose
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
119
,
60
-
68
.
Remage-Healey
,
L.
and
Romero
,
L. M.
(
2001
).
Corticosterone and insulin interact to regulate glucose and triglyceride levels during stress in a bird
.
Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol.
281
,
994
-
1003
.
Romero
,
L. M.
and
Beattie
,
U. K.
(
2021
).
Common myths of glucocorticoid function in ecology and conservation
.
J. Exp. Zool. A Ecol. Integr. Physiol.
337
,
7
-
14
.
Romero
,
L. M.
,
Dickens
,
M. J.
and
Cyr
,
N. E.
(
2009
).
The reactive scope model—A new model integrating homeostasis, allostasis, and stress
.
Horm. Behav.
55
,
375
-
389
.
Romero
,
L. M.
and
Gormally
,
B. M. G.
(
2019
).
How truly conserved is the “well-conserved” vertebrate stress response?
Integr. Comp. Biol.
59
,
273
-
281
.
Romero
,
L. M.
,
Platts
,
S. H.
,
Schoech
,
S. J.
,
Wada
,
H.
,
Crespi
,
E.
,
Martin
,
L. B.
and
Buck
,
C. L.
(
2015
).
Understanding stress in the healthy animal – potential paths for progress
.
Stress
18
,
491
-
497
.
Sapolsky
,
R.
,
Romero
,
L. M.
and
Munck
,
A. U.
(
2000
).
How to glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions
.
Endocr. Rev.
21
,
55
-
89
.
Schoenle
,
L. A.
,
Zimmer
,
C.
,
Miller
,
E. T.
and
Vitousek
,
M. N.
(
2021
).
Does variation in glucocorticoid concentrations predict fitness? A phylogenetic meta-analysis
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
300
,
113611
.
Stoffel
,
M. A.
,
Nakagawa
,
S.
and
Schielzeth
,
H.
(
2017
).
RptR: Repeatability estimation and variance decomposition by generalized linear mixed-effects models
.
Method. Ecol. Evol.
8
,
1639
-
1644
.
Taff
,
C. C.
and
Vitousek
,
M. N.
(
2016
).
Endocrine flexibility: optimizing phenotypes in a dynamic world?
Trends Ecol. Evol.
31
,
476
-
488
.
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Schoenle
,
L. A.
and
Vitousek
,
M. N.
(
2018a
).
The repeatability of glucocorticoids: a review and meta-analysis
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
260
,
136
-
145
.
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Zimmer
,
C.
and
Vitousek
,
M. N.
(
2018b
).
Efficacy of negative feedback in the HPA axis predicts recovery from acute challenges
.
Biol. Lett.
14
,
20180131
.
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Campagna
,
L.
and
Vitousek
,
M. N.
(
2019a
).
Genome-wide variation in DNA methylation is associated with stress resilience and plumage brightness in a wild bird
.
Mol. Ecol.
28
,
3722
-
3737
.
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Zimmer
,
C.
and
Vitousek
,
M. N.
(
2019b
).
Achromatic plumage brightness predicts stress resilience and social interactions in tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor)
.
Behav. Ecol.
30
,
733
-
745
.
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Zimmer
,
C.
,
Scheck
,
D.
,
Ryan
,
T. A.
,
Houtz
,
J. L.
,
Smee
,
M. R.
,
Hendry
,
T. A.
and
Vitousek
,
M. N.
(
2021
).
Plumage manipulation alters the integration of social behavior, physiology, internal microbiome, and fitness
.
Anim. Behav.
178
,
11
-
36
.
Tomasek
,
O.
,
Bobek
,
L.
,
Kralova
,
T.
,
Adamkova
,
M.
and
Albrecht
,
T.
(
2019
).
Fuel for the pace of life: baseline blood glucose concentration coevolves with life history traits in songbirds
.
Funct. Ecol.
33
,
239
-
249
.
van de Pol
,
M.
(
2012
).
Quantifying individual variation in reaction norms: how study design affects the accuracy, precision and power of random regression models
.
Method. Ecol. Evol.
3
,
268
-
280
.
Vitousek
,
M. N.
,
Johnson
,
M. A.
,
Downs
,
C. J.
,
Miller
,
E. T.
,
Martin
,
L. B.
,
Francis
,
C. D.
,
Donald
,
J. W.
,
Fuxjager
,
M. J.
,
Goymann
,
W.
,
Hau
,
M.
et al. 
(
2019
).
Macroevolutionary Patterning in Glucocorticoids Suggests Different Selective Pressures Shape Baseline and Stress-Induced Levels
.
Am. Nat.
193
,
866
-
880
.
Vitousek
,
M. N.
,
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Hallinger
,
K. K.
,
Zimmer
,
C.
,
Winkler
,
D. W.
(
2018a
).
Hormones and fitness: Evidence for trade-offs in glucocorticoid regulation across contexts
.
Front. Ecol. Evol.
6
,
1
-
14
.
Vitousek
,
M. N.
,
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Stedman
,
J.
,
Zimmer
,
C.
,
Ardia
,
D. R.
,
Salzman
,
T. C.
and
Winkler
,
D. W.
(
2018b
).
The lingering impact of stress: Brief acute glucocorticoid exposure has sustained, dose-dependent effects on reproduction
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
285
,
20180722
.
Wada
,
H.
and
Sewall
,
K. B.
(
2014
).
Introduction to the symposium—Uniting evolutionary and physiological approaches to understanding phenotypic plasticity
.
Integr. Comp. Biol.
54
,
774
-
782
.
Westneat
,
D. F.
,
Wright
,
J.
and
Dingemanse
,
N. J.
(
2014
).
The biology hidden inside residual within-individual phenotypic variation
.
Biol. Rev.
90
,
729
-
743
.
Westneat
,
D. F.
,
Araya–Ajoy
,
Y. G.
,
Allegue
,
H.
,
Class
,
B.
,
Dingemanse
,
N.
,
Dochtermann
,
N. A.
,
Garamszegi
,
L. Z.
,
Martin
,
J. G. A.
,
Nakagawa
,
S.
,
Réale
,
D.
et al. 
(
2020
).
Collision between biological process and statistical analysis revealed by mean centering
.
J. Anim. Ecol.
89
,
2813
-
2824
.
Wingfield
,
J. C.
,
Maney
,
D. L.
,
Breuner
,
C. W.
,
Jacobs
,
J. D.
,
Lynn
,
S.
,
Ramenofsky
,
M.
and
Richardson
,
R. D.
(
1998
).
Ecological bases of hormone-behavior interactions: the “emergency life history stage”
.
Am. Zool.
38
,
191
-
206
.
Winkler
,
D. W.
,
Hallinger
,
K. K.
,
Pegan
,
T. M.
,
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Verhoeven
,
M. A.
,
Oordt
,
D. C. van
,
Stager
,
M.
,
Uehling
,
J. J.
,
Vitousek
,
M. N.
et al. 
(
2020
).
Full lifetime perspectives on the costs and benefits of lay-date variation in tree swallows
.
Ecology
,
101
,
e03109
.
Zimmer
,
C.
,
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Ardia
,
D. R.
,
Winkler
,
D. W.
and
Vitousek
,
M. N.
(
2019
).
On again, off again: acute stress response and negative feedback together predict resilience to experimental stressors
.
Funct. Ecol.
33
,
619
-
628
.
Zimmer
,
C.
,
Taff
,
C. C.
,
Ardia
,
D. R.
,
Rose
,
A. P.
,
Aborn
,
D. A.
,
Johnson
,
L. S.
and
Vitousek
,
M. N.
(
2020
).
Environmental unpredictability shapes glucocorticoid regulation across populations of tree swallows
.
Sci. Rep.
10
,
13682
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information