Following their emergence on land, sea turtle hatchlings need to travel through the open ocean. Whether hatchlings can detect ecologically and functionally relevant chemical cues released in the marine habitat is unknown. We collected seawater at 6 and 27 km off the Brazilian coast, i.e. within and beyond the continental shelf. In a two-choice flume, we exposed post-emergent (<24 h old) loggerhead (Caretta caretta) turtles to these seawaters. Based on their life history, we posited that if hatchlings could distinguish between the seawater from these regions, they should prefer the oceanic seawater and/or avoid the coastal seawater. Hatchlings were tested singly and could access any parts of the flume. We recorded the seawater plume first visited and the time spent in each plume. Of all the first choices and time spent in a plume, nearly 70% involved the oceanic seawater. The ability of hatchlings to distinguish between seawaters could provide goal-recognition information.

Long-distance migrations impose tough demands on the sensory guidance of animals (Akesson and Hedenstrom, 2007; Bauer et al., 2011). This reality may be particularly acute for marine organisms that journey in a seemingly featureless environment (i.e. without landmarks or beacons), often for hundreds or thousands of kilometres (Block et al., 2011; Costa et al., 2012; Luschi, 2013). Given their precarious conservation status (Anonymous, 2018) and their necessity to travel great distances to reach precise locations to complete essential parts of their life cycle, the mechanisms by which sea turtles accomplish this feat have been of great interest to ecologists and conservationists (Koch et al., 1969; Lohmann et al., 2008a; Mazor et al., 2016).

Following their emergence on land, sea turtle hatchlings migrate offshore and into the open ocean using a suite of guidance mechanisms that primarily rely on wave direction (Salmon and Lohmann, 1989; Lohmann and Lohmann, 1992; Goff et al., 1998; Wang et al., 1998) and Earth's magnetic field (Brothers and Lohmann, 2018; Lohmann and Lohmann, 2019; Lohmann et al., 2001, 2008a,b; Luschi et al., 2003; Southwood and Avens, 2010). However, how the movement decisions of sea turtle hatchlings may be influenced by their detection of chemical cues is comparatively far less understood. Although geomagnetic and hydrodynamic cues provide broad-scale guidance, chemosensory cues emanating from, for example, native beaches, landmasses and feeding ‘hotspots’ may need to be detected at a closer range. If detected, these cues could confirm the arrival at an intended destination or the function of goal recognition (sensuEndres et al., 2009, 2016; Grassman et al., 1984; Kedzuf and Salmon, 2016).

Although sea turtles can detect several chemosensory cues, determining the functional significance of this process requires that ecologically relevant substances (in composition and concentration) be used in controlled experiments. Most experiments on sea turtle olfaction have been conducted under laboratory conditions and provided compelling evidence that specific water-borne (Grassman and Owens, 1987; Manton et al., 1972; Owens et al., 1982) and/or air-borne cues (Endres and Lohmann, 2012, 2013; Endres et al., 2009; Kedzuf and Salmon, 2016) may be detected. However, these experiments typically relied on substances that were either artificial and/or with their concentration unknown relative to what sea turtles may encounter in situ. Whether animals may detect a given chemical cue (e.g. odour) enabling a response is contingent on the nature (Brown et al., 2010) and the intensity of that cue (its concentration), the latter requiring being above a minimum threshold (Brown et al., 2006; Mirza et al., 2006). Thus, a better understanding of the functional significance of the chemosensory abilities of sea turtles could be enhanced by testing ecologically relevant substances, such as those found in their habitat.

Here, we tested whether loggerhead (Caretta caretta Linnaeus 1758) sea turtle hatchlings can distinguish between seawater types from two regions of their natural habitat. Specifically, we used seawater collected from within and beyond the continental shelf (thereafter ‘coastal’ and ‘oceanic’, respectively), and experimentally exposed hatchlings to these. We attempted to enhance the ecological relevance of our methodology by testing the ability of these post-emergent hatchlings to distinguish among seawater types without providing prior exposure to these seawater types. As reaching the open ocean (oceanic province) is thought to be the goal of loggerhead hatchlings (Bolten, 2003), we posited that if hatchlings were able to distinguish between these seawater types, hatchlings would be attracted by the oceanic seawater and/or avoid the coastal seawater. Regardless, this possibility would imply that hatchlings may distinguish between the coastal and oceanic regions based on ecologically relevant concentrations of chemosensory cues present in seawater. Such an ability could imply the occurrence of a goal-recognition function, informing hatchlings of having reached their destination.

Hatchling collection and maintenance

Thirty loggerhead (C. caretta) hatchlings, less than 24 h old, were obtained from the Tamar Project (www.tamar.org.br), a non-governmental Brazilian organization involved in sea turtle conservation. Specifically, eggs were directly collected by field technicians of the Tamar Project from ovipositing females at beaches near Praia do Forte, Bahia State, Brazil. These eggs were incubated until hatching at the Tamar Project's nursery beach. On 2 and 28 June and 17 July 2019, we collected recently hatched turtles; each of these three groups contained 10 hatchlings that were the offspring of three females (hereafter groups 1–3). After their transport to the laboratory in a 20×15×20 cm icebox overlaid with sand from their native beach, hatchlings were held in a 60×25×25 cm aquarium, also overlaid with this sand and kept at 30°C, following recommendations by Mrosovky (1980). Hatchlings remained in the laboratory for a maximum of 36 h before their release at their native beach. All collections and handling procedures were authorized by the national Brazilian agency involved in the conservation of biota (Sisbio License 66006-1). Furthermore, all experimental activities (detailed below) were authorized and complied with the requirements of the Ethics Committee on the Use of Animals from the Federal University of Bahia (project number 08/2019).

Seawater collection

We collected seawater from two locations offshore from Praia do Forte (coordinates: 12°31′44.2″S, 37°49′30.1″W and 12°35'57.7″S, 37°53′39.0″W). We used a 10 m motorized vessel to access these two regions, specifically within and beyond the continental shelf, i.e. at 6 km (depth of ∼40 m) and 27 km (depth of ∼1000 m) from shore, respectively (Fig. 1). Seawater sampling occurred on the same days as obtaining hatchlings, thus on three occasions. We used 20-liter plastic containers to collect and transport this water to the laboratory. Prior to usage, these containers were cleaned following the procedure detailed in Lewin and Chen (1973). Specifically, we soaked these containers for 5 days with chlorine-diluted tap water (concentration of 2 ml l−1). We subsequently rinsed and soaked these containers for an additional 5 days with an anti-chlorine-based solvent (i.e. sodium thiosulfate 100 mg l−1). Following this period, we performed a second rinsing and soaking process for a period of 2 days, this time with dechlorinated tap water mixed with 10 ml of a neutral detergent [i.e. sodium dodecylbenzenesulfonate (5.0–15.0%), sodium lauryl ether sulphate (1.0–10.0%) and bronopol (<0.2%)]. Finally, we rinsed each container five times with local surface seawater before collecting the seawater used in the present experiment. Water collection was done by filling the containers with surface seawater from the bow of the vessel, with the latter being adrift and its motor stopped. We used this procedure to reduce potential chemical interferences that could emanate from the vessel. Throughout the experiment, each container was used for only one type of seawater (i.e. coastal or oceanic). At the laboratory, each seawater type was transferred into one of two 100-liter reservoirs, which, along with its tubing system (e.g. hoses and valves), had also been washed as described above.

Fig. 1.

Map of Brazil and the seawater collection points. The location of the seawater collection points, within (C, coastal) and beyond (O, oceanic) the continental shelf, are indicated by the red circles (inset map, right). The continental shelf is represented by the light-grey region that is contiguous to the coast. The coastal and oceanic sampling points were each distanced by 6 and 27 km from the coast, and their approximate depths were 40 and 1000 m, respectively. P, the beach of Praia do Forte (State of Bahia) from where hatchlings originated. The star on the main map (left) indicates the location of Brasilia, Brazil's capital. Source: Google Earth Pro 2021.

Fig. 1.

Map of Brazil and the seawater collection points. The location of the seawater collection points, within (C, coastal) and beyond (O, oceanic) the continental shelf, are indicated by the red circles (inset map, right). The continental shelf is represented by the light-grey region that is contiguous to the coast. The coastal and oceanic sampling points were each distanced by 6 and 27 km from the coast, and their approximate depths were 40 and 1000 m, respectively. P, the beach of Praia do Forte (State of Bahia) from where hatchlings originated. The star on the main map (left) indicates the location of Brasilia, Brazil's capital. Source: Google Earth Pro 2021.

Experimental apparatus and procedures

We used a two-choice flume, which allows testing for a preference (or avoidance) between one of two water plumes (sensuJutfelt et al., 2017). Our glass-made flume was 200 cm in length and its sides (i.e. 30 cm in height) were covered from the outside by opaque dark-blue paper (Fig. S1). The base of the flume (i.e. the outflow) was equipped with a 30×45 cm shelf positioned a few centimeters above the water flow and covered with sand from the hatchlings' native beach. This miniature ‘beach’ was set to reduce the stress inherent to manipulating hatchlings and served for acclimation, whereby each test subject could move freely from the terrestrial to the aquatic environment. This beach gently sloped toward the flowing water, with this characteristic known to provide hatchlings with a means of orientating toward the sea (Lohmann and Lohmann, 1996; Salmon et al., 1992). Upstream (i.e. 20 cm) from the forward edge of the beach, the flume's outflow channel splits into two inflow channels (left and right; each 45×135 cm; Fig. 1), in each of which flowed one of the two seawater types described above.

Each of the two seawater storage reservoirs was equipped with a dual outflow system (i.e. valves and tubing) that could supply each of the two channels on demand. Flow resulted from gravity and was regulated by predetermining the aperture of valves and by keeping the reservoirs at least half full. We conducted dye tests to assess flow speed and linearity in each of the two channels, with this procedure allowing us to measure the time required for the dye to reach the base of each channel (Fig. S2). After determining the proper valve aperture to obtain the desired flow, we repeated the dye test at the onset of each trial session; this allowed us to ensure that flow was comparable among channels and through repetitions. In each channel, the water current was set at 16 ml min−1, which was sufficient to create a directional flow while allowing hatchlings to swim freely. To reduce potential interference of aerial chemical cues (i.e. odours; Endres et al., 2009) emanating from the laboratory, we covered the channels with a removable glass plate, positioned at 15 cm above the water flow. No other activities (experimental or other) took place in the lab during the experiment.

To reduce potential interference from extraneous light sources that might affect the behaviour of the sea turtles (Witherington and Martin, 2005), we conducted this experiment under low light conditions (sensuLeduc et al., 2010). Specifically, under otherwise dark conditions, we relied on two lights equipped with a 5 W red lamp, each placed diagonally at a distance of 1.2 m from the upstream end of each channel (Fig. S3). Such dimly lit conditions appeared to be adequate for turtles to behave ‘normally’ (i.e. readily swimming in the flume) and allowed us to document the behaviour of the hatchlings, as detailed below.

The experimental testing occurred on three dates (i.e. the day that followed the collection of hatchlings and seawater, as detailed above), each with 10 hatchlings. We used this number of hatchlings per testing session given the logistical constraints of keeping a greater number of individuals in captivity at a given time. Furthermore, to provide greater ecological relevance, we considered that trials needed to occur within 24 h after the hatchlings' emergence. During this period of hyperactivity (so-called ‘frenzy’; Wyneken, 1997), hatchlings are characterized by their continuous swimming behaviour (Musick and Limpus, 1997).

A testing session started by filling each of the flume's channels with one of two seawater types (randomly chosen) to a depth of ∼15 cm and letting this water flow for 5 min. Each hatchling was placed in an identified container, which allowed us to randomize the testing order while knowing the identity of each individual. To initiate a trial, we placed an individual on the sand-covered beach at the starting point (Fig. S3). Specifically, hatchlings were placed ∼10 cm ahead of the back wall of the flume, at the mid-point of the beach's width, and at a 90 deg angle (i.e. hatchling's head-to-tail body plan) to the flume's back wall. After entering the water, hatchlings were allowed to freely explore the flume for a period of 10 min and were video-recorded with a GoPro Hero 4 camera (24 frames s−1 and 1080 dpi; San Mateo, CA, USA) placed overhead. After all 10 hatchlings were tested, we drained the flume, reversed the flow of each seawater type into each of the two channels (i.e. left versus right) and repeated the procedures presented above. In other words, each hatchling was tested twice, each time with each of the two seawater plumes on a different side of the flume. Afterward, the remaining seawater was discarded and hatchlings were returned to their native beach and released at the water's edge.

Data extraction and statistical analysis

After the completion of all trials, an observer (G.S.) analysed the videos in random order, thus without knowledge of the treatment. Videos were watched on a 31.0×17.5 cm computer screen, from which we extracted: (1) the time required to leave the beach and enter the water, (2) the time in the water before entering a channel, (3) whether a hatchling first entered the left or right channel, and (4) the time spent in each channel. A choice was defined when a hatchling had entirely passed beyond the channels' bifurcation. We also noted (5) the number of times a hatchling switched sides (e.g. leaving the left to enter the right channel of the flume), indicating whether it had been in direct contact with both seawater plumes.

As only one experimenter conducted the trials, this person was required to know the order of the treatments and thus could not conduct blind tests. If unconscious biases by the experimenter occurred relative to the initial position and orientation of hatchlings when introduced into the flume, such biases could potentially influence their initial direction and affect the channel in which they subsequently entered. To control for this possibility, a second observer (A.O.H.C.L., blind to the treatments) verified the position and orientation of hatchlings upon being introduced onto the flume's beach. This was done by visualizing the video records and measuring the initial position of the hatchlings and the direction they faced using a ruler and a protractor, respectively, placed against the computer screen. Specifically, the starting position of hatchlings was measured in relation to the midpoint (i.e. of the width) of the flume's beach (e.g. 25 out of 50 cm, or 50%), whereas the starting angle involved measuring the deviation of hatchlings' longitudinal body plan (i.e. from head to tail) relative to the flume's lengthwise direction (e.g. 0 deg, or no deviation). These data then were used in our statistical model to control for any deviation from neutral starting conditions.

To determine whether hatchlings preferred a seawater plume (i.e. coastal or oceanic) or one side of the tank (i.e. left or right), we compared the percentage of time they passed in each of the plumes and sides, using separate generalized linear models (GLMs) with a linear error distribution. The first and second analyses considered the fixed factors plume and side, respectively, both with the fixed factor repetition, and an interaction between the two terms. We also considered the fixed factor group, as testing was done with three groups of 10 individuals, each of a different female. As covariates, we used the variables position, orientation, time to channel and the number of side switches. We also tested whether the number of the first choices toward the oceanic plume differed among repetitions, using a GLM with a binary logistic error distribution, whereby the number of the first choice (whether between plumes or sides) could change or remain constant between the first and second repetitions. A preference (for a plume or side) would be indicated by a lack of significant change between the first and second repetitions, thus being maintained across repetitions. Statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS v. 21.

The starting conditions of hatchlings on the flume's beach were neutral/unbiased (i.e. at the midpoint, and 0 deg of deviation) in all but 23% of trials (14/60), with these deviations being minute. Specifically, in six trials, hatchling position deviated by up to ±2% from the 50% midpoint (±1 cm; mean±s.e.m.=50.1±0.08%; means are presented ±s.e.m. throughout). In eight other trials, hatchling orientation deviated by up to ±3 deg from the 0 deg starting angle (mean 0.07±0.06 deg). No statistical differences between groups or repetitions existed for these two variables. When the oceanic plume was presented on the left and right sides of the flume, the mean initial position and orientation of hatchlings were 50.1±0.08% with 0.1±0.11 deg, and 49.9±0.09% with <0.1±0.13 deg, respectively. On average, hatchlings took 21.4±1.29 s following their release onto the flume beach to enter a channel with no statistical effect of groups or repetitions. In most trials, hatchlings swam in both seawater plumes (mean number of side switches=2.1±0.16), with no difference between groups or repetitions. To further verify whether the minute deviations from neutrality at the onset of these 14 trials could have affected our overall conclusions, we verified the side in which these hatchlings subsequently entered. For example, orientating or placing a hatchling toward the right side of the flume suggests a higher propensity to enter the right-side channel, and vice versa for left-side placements. However, such outcomes unfolded in less than 43% of these cases (6 out of 14; Table S1).

Hatchlings initially chose the oceanic plume in 66.7% and 70.0% of the trials for the first and second repetitions, respectively (mean=68.3%; Fig. 2), with no statistical difference in the number of the first choice for a seawater plume between repetitions (Wald Chi-square=0.077, d.f.=1, P=0.781). Thus, the hatchlings' first choice was maintained among repetitions. By contrast, hatchlings initially chose the left side of the flume in 66.7% and 30.0% of the first and second repetitions, respectively, with this choice statistically differing between repetitions (Wald Chi-square=7.858, d.f.=1, P=0.005).

Fig. 2.

Mean percentage of time spent in each of the two seawater plumes and sides of the flume, and the first choices made by hatchlings. The percentage of time spent in each of the two seawater plumes (dark- and light-grey boxplots corresponding to oceanic and coastal seawater, respectively) and sides of the flume during the first (1) and second (2) repetitions for each of the three groups of hatchlings (N=10, top to bottom panels, respectively). L and R stand for the left and right channels of the flume, respectively, in which the seawater of the two sampled regions flowed sequentially. The boxplot lines are the mean for the measured parameters, the boxes range from the 25th to the 75th percentile, whiskers are 1.5×inter-quartile range and circles are outliers. The blue diamonds indicate the percentage of hatchlings that initially entered the left or right channel of the flume. For the first and second groups, the oceanic seawater flowed in the left channel (randomly determined) during the first repetition. For the third group, this seawater initially flowed in the right channel. During the second repetition, we reversed the flow of the seawater among channels to control for a potential side bias. In each panel, the grey horizontal line represents the 50% mark, which indicates no preference for a side and/or a plume.

Fig. 2.

Mean percentage of time spent in each of the two seawater plumes and sides of the flume, and the first choices made by hatchlings. The percentage of time spent in each of the two seawater plumes (dark- and light-grey boxplots corresponding to oceanic and coastal seawater, respectively) and sides of the flume during the first (1) and second (2) repetitions for each of the three groups of hatchlings (N=10, top to bottom panels, respectively). L and R stand for the left and right channels of the flume, respectively, in which the seawater of the two sampled regions flowed sequentially. The boxplot lines are the mean for the measured parameters, the boxes range from the 25th to the 75th percentile, whiskers are 1.5×inter-quartile range and circles are outliers. The blue diamonds indicate the percentage of hatchlings that initially entered the left or right channel of the flume. For the first and second groups, the oceanic seawater flowed in the left channel (randomly determined) during the first repetition. For the third group, this seawater initially flowed in the right channel. During the second repetition, we reversed the flow of the seawater among channels to control for a potential side bias. In each panel, the grey horizontal line represents the 50% mark, which indicates no preference for a side and/or a plume.

The mean percentage of time that hatchlings spent in the oceanic seawater was 69.3% and 68.1% for the first and second repetitions, respectively (mean=68.8±2.22%) (Fig. 2). Alternatively, the percentage of the time spent on the left side of the flume was 55.6% and 44.9% for the first and second repetitions, respectively (mean=50.3±3.20%). We observed a significant difference in the percentage of time spent between plumes, but not between sides (Table 1). When comparing the interaction terms plume×repetition and side×repetition, the former yielded no significant difference, suggesting that no change occurred among repetitions, whereas the latter yielded a significant difference. Thus, hatchlings did not appear to prefer a distinct side of the flume but rather a plume type. The variable group had no statistically significant effect on these outcomes. The position and orientation of hatchlings upon introduction onto the beach, the time to reach the channels, and the number of times hatchlings switched sides had no effects on these results (Table 1).

Table 1.

Results of generalized linear models, with a linear error distribution that yielded no over-dispersion of residues

Results of generalized linear models, with a linear error distribution that yielded no over-dispersion of residues
Results of generalized linear models, with a linear error distribution that yielded no over-dispersion of residues

A functional role for hatchlings' chemosensory abilities

Here, we tested whether post-emergent loggerheads could distinguish between seawater types collected within and beyond the continental shelf. Testing for the ability to distinguish between distinct water masses provides crucial information on the abilities of sea turtles to sense the components of their habitat. Furthermore, using ecologically relevant substances may further our understanding of the functional role such abilities may serve at sea. Based on this, our experiment's rationale hinged on the fact that post-emergent loggerheads migrate to the oceanic (offshore) province (Type 2 life history; Bolten, 2003), presumably to avoid predators that could abound coastward (Carr, 1987). Thus, if hatchlings could distinguish between these naturally occurring seawater types, we expected to observe a preference for the oceanic seawater and/or avoidance of the coastal one. In agreement with our prediction, in nearly 70% of trials, hatchlings initially chose the oceanic plume and spent significantly more time in that plume.

Sea turtles can detect chemicals emanating from various substances, including food (Endres et al., 2009; Kedzuf and Salmon, 2016), plastic debris (Pfaller et al., 2020), dimethyl sulfide (Endres and Lohmann, 2012), mud (Endres and Lohmann, 2013; Endres et al., 2009) and sand (Grassman et al., 1984). Investigations into these substances have provided insights into the chemosensory abilities of turtles. However, the functional significance of such sensory ability has remained rather elusive, i.e. whether turtles use chemosensory abilities for orientation, navigation and/or goal recognition. At least partly, this may be the result of the nature and/or concentration of the substances tested, which may not represent what turtles encounter in their habitat. Uncovering the functional role of the chemosensory functions of sea turtles may be furthered by testing substances that occur in the wild, and at their ecologically relevant concentrations. For example, when considering feeding at sea, seawater collected from an oceanic productive ‘hotspot’ would represent a more ecologically relevant cue compared with arbitrary use of seafood fragments. Although collecting such seawater for its subsequent laboratory use is logistically challenging, the results obtained should further our understanding of the chemosensory functions of sea turtles in the open ocean.

Laboratory experiments using ecologically relevant cues paired with in situ monitoring may help to understand the role of chemosensory functions in sea turtle navigation and orientation. Throughout their ontogeny, sea turtles may rely on active-oriented swimming to reach and remain in favourable habitats (Hays et al., 2020; Luschi, 2013; Luschi et al., 1998; Putman and Mansfield, 2015; Putman et al., 2012a,b). Orientating at sea is based on wave direction and a geomagnetic compass and map (Bauer et al., 2011; Goff et al., 1998; Lohmann et al., 2008c, 2012, 2022; McNamara et al., 2011). However, in situ investigations of sea turtles equipped with satellite transmitters allowed tracking their movements relative to an oceanic island goal (e.g. Ascension Island). Contrary to upwind sea turtles, those located downwind from this island were able to reach it, suggesting that olfaction could play a role in orientation and goal recognition (Hays et al., 2003, 2020; Luschi et al., 2001; but see Girard et al., 2006). Although an island constitutes a tangible landmark, reaching a precise marine destination (e.g. gyre), by contrast, lacks such a feature. Reaching and remaining in such a destination may require relying on more locally restricted cues, such as chemicals emanating from that location. In their experiment, Polovina et al. (2006) monitored loggerheads equipped with satellite tags. These sea turtles remained in a pelagic hotspot (i.e. Kuroshio Extension Bifurcation Region) when the surface water contained a high concentration of chlorophyll. However, when these waters became vertically stratified and the chlorophyll concentration dropped, these sea turtles moved to a distinct location with high surface chlorophyll concentration. If local chemical cues from productive oceanic regions are detected, that may indicate that a specific region has been reached and trigger context-appropriate behaviour (e.g. feeding, sensuKezduf and Salmon, 2016). Paired with our experiment, this evidence underscores the possibility that sea turtles may distinguish between different seawater masses, which could provide goal-recognition information.

Our oceanic sampling was 15 km from the edge of the continental shelf, 21 km from the coastal sampling point and 27 km from the coast. Although the distance separating these points is relatively short considering the size of the marine realm, their respective depth drastically differed, specifically from ∼40 to ∼1000 m. This region is located within the bifurcation zone of the South Equatorial Current, which gives origin to two western boundary currents: the southbound Brazil Current and the northbound North Brazil Undercurrent, which partly flow above the continental shelf (Peterson and Stramma, 1991; Pereira et al., 2014; Rodrigues et al., 2007). Monitoring in the region has revealed that shelf break upwellings, mainly driven by wind processes, also occur (Thévenin et al., 2019). Paired with vertical and horizontal water movements, the abrupt change in depth could result in distinct concentrations of suspended sediments among adjacent regions. Thus, distinct concentrations of suspended sediments could provide, at least partly, a mechanism that enabled hatchlings to distinguish between the seawater plumes presented here.

How marine organisms may use ecologically relevant chemosensory cues is not always well understood because their precise composition and/or concentrations that may be detected to elicit a response are often poorly known (Costa et al., 2012). Nonetheless, while the ability to detect odours may be important, it may also provide an activational role, whereby organisms become more attuned to other sensory cues and display the most context-appropriate behaviour (Jorge et al., 2009). Here, we have provided evidence that sea turtle hatchlings may distinguish between distinct seawater masses that they are likely to encounter during their seaward migration. This detection, along with other sensory mechanisms, may allow these organisms to determine their arrival at favourable destinations when at sea.

We thank José Amorim Reis-Filho for his help with the experimental setup, Guilherme Lessa for edits on the regional physical oceanography, and two anonymous reviewers for their contributions that led to improving this paper. We are also indebted to the TAMAR Project for kindly providing us with the hatchlings used in the present study, and for letting us on board their vessel to sample seawater. G.S. is grateful to Universidade Federal da Bahia for making available the facilities used herein.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: G.S., A.O.H.C.L.; Methodology: G.S.; Formal analysis: A.O.H.C.L.; Investigation: G.S.; Resources: E.M.d.S.; Data curation: G.S.; Writing - original draft: A.O.H.C.L.; Writing - review & editing: G.S., E.M.d.S., A.O.H.C.L.; Visualization: G.S.; Supervision: A.O.H.C.L.; Project administration: E.M.d.S.

Funding

A.O.H.C.L. received a Programa Nacional de Pós Doutorado post-doctoral fellowship granted by Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES) (88882.305953/2018-01). G.S. received a doctoral fellowship granted by CAPES.

Akesson
,
S.
and
Hedenstrom
,
A.
(
2007
).
How migrants get there: migratory performance and orientation
.
Bioscience
57
,
123
-
133
.
Anonymous
(
2018
).
Livro vermelho da fauna Brasileira ameaçada de extinção
.
ICMBio Brasilia
1
,
1
-
495
.
Bauer
,
S.
,
Nolet
,
B. A.
,
Giske
,
J.
,
Chapman
,
J. W.
,
Akesson
,
S.
,
Hedenstrom
,
A.
and
Fryxell
,
J. M.
(
2011
).
Cues and Decision Rules in Animal Migration. Animal Migration – A Synthesis
. In
Animal Migration – A Synthesis
(ed.
E. J.
Milner-Gulland
,
J. M.
Fryxell
and
A. R. E.
Sinclair
), pp.
68
-
87
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.
Block
,
B. A.
,
Jonsen
,
I. D.
,
Jorgensen
,
S. J.
,
Winship
,
A. J.
,
Shaffer
,
S. A.
,
Bograd
,
S. J.
,
Hazen
,
E. L.
,
Foley
,
D. G.
,
Breed
,
G. A.
,
Harrison
,
A. L.
et al. 
(
2011
).
Tracking apex marine predator movements in a dynamic ocean
.
Nature
475
,
86
-
90
.
Bolten
,
A. B.
(
2003
).
Variation in sea turtle life history patterns: neritic vs. oceanic developmental stages
. In
The Biology of Sea Turtles
, Vol.
2
(ed.
P. L.
Lutz
,
J. A.
Musick
and
J.
Wyneken
), pp.
243
-
257
.
Boca Raton
:
CRC Press
.
Brothers
,
J. R.
,
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
2018
).
Evidence that magnetic navigation and geomagnetic imprinting shape spatial genetic variation in sea turtles
.
Curr. Biol.
28
,
1325
-
1329.e2
.
Brown
,
G. E.
,
DiCapua
,
D. M.
,
Ivan
,
L. I.
and
Roh
,
E.
(
2006
).
Effects of group size on the threat-sensitive response to varying concentrations of chemical alarm cues by juvenile convict cichlids
.
Can. J. Zool.
84
,
1
-
8
.
Brown
,
G. E.
,
Elvidge
,
C. K.
,
Macnaughton
,
C. J.
,
Ramnarine
,
I.
and
Godin
,
J.-G.
(
2010
).
Cross-population responses to conspecific chemical alarm cues in wild Trinidadian guppies, Poecilia reticulata: evidence for local conservation of cue production
.
Can. J. Zool.
88
,
139
-
147
.
Carr
,
A. F.
(
1987
).
New perspectives on the pelagic stage of sea turtle development
.
Conserv. Biol.
1
,
103
-
121
.
Costa
,
D. P.
,
Breed
,
G. A.
and
Robinson
,
P. W.
(
2012
).
New insights into pelagic migrations: implications for ecology and conservation
.
Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst.
43
,
73
-
96
.
Endres
,
C. S.
and
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
2012
).
Perception of dimethyl sulfide (DMS) by loggerhead sea turtles: a possible mechanism for locating high-productivity oceanic regions for foraging
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
3535
-
3538
.
Endres
,
C. S.
and
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
2013
).
Detection of coastal mud odors by loggerhead sea turtles: a possible mechanism for sensing nearby land
.
Marine Biology
160
,
2951
-
2956
.
Endres
,
C. S.
,
Putman
,
N. F.
and
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
2009
).
Perception of airborne odors by loggerhead sea turtles
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
3823
-
3827
.
Endres
,
C. S.
,
Putman
,
N. F.
,
Ernst
,
D. A.
,
Kurth
,
J. A.
,
Lohmann
,
C. M. F.
and
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
2016
).
Multi-modal homing in sea turtles: modeling dual use of geomagnetic and chemical cues in island-finding
.
Front. Behav. Neurosci.
10
,
363
.
Girard
,
C.
,
Sudre
,
J.
,
Benhamou
,
S.
,
Roos
,
D.
and
Luschi
,
P.
(
2006
).
Homing in green turtles Chelonia mydas: oceanic currents act as a constraint rather than as an information source
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
322
,
281
-
289
.
Goff
,
M.
,
Salmon
,
M.
and
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
1998
).
Hatchling sea turtles use surface waves to establish a magnetic compass direction
.
Anim. Behav.
55
,
69
-
77
.
Grassman
,
M.
and
Owens
,
D.
(
1987
).
Chemosensory imprinting in juvenile green sea turtles, Chelonia mydas
.
Anim. Behav.
35
,
929
-
931
.
Grassman
,
M. A.
,
Owens
,
D. W.
,
McVey
,
J. P.
and
M
,
R. M.
(
1984
).
Olfactory-based orientation in artificially imprinted sea turtles
.
Science
224
,
83
-
84
.
Hays
,
G. C.
,
Akesson
,
S.
,
Broderick
,
A. C.
,
Glen
,
F.
,
Godley
,
B. J.
,
Papi
,
F.
and
Luschi
,
P.
(
2003
).
Island-finding ability of marine turtles
.
Proc. Biol. Sci. R. Soc.
270
Suppl. 1,
S5
-
S7
.
Hays
,
G. C.
,
Cerritelli
,
G.
,
Esteban
,
N.
,
Rattray
,
A.
and
Luschi
,
P.
(
2020
).
Open ocean reorientation and challenges of island finding by sea turtles during long-distance migration
.
Curr. Biol.
30
,
3236
-
3242.e3
.
Jorge
,
P. E.
,
Marques
,
A. E.
and
Phillips
,
J. B.
(
2009
).
Activational rather than navigational effects of odors on homing of young pigeons
.
Curr. Biol.
19
,
650
-
654
.
Jutfelt
,
F.
,
Sundin
,
J.
,
Raby
,
G. D.
,
Krång
,
A.-S.
and
Clark
,
T. D.
(
2017
).
Two-current choice flumes for testing avoidance and preference in aquatic animals
.
Method. Ecol. Evol.
8
,
379
-
390
.
Kedzuf
,
S.
and
Salmon
,
M.
(
2016
).
Do marine turtles use odors to locate foraging hotspots in the open ocean?
Chelonian Conserv. Biol.
15
,
90
-
101
.
Koch
,
A. L.
,
Carr
,
A.
and
Ehrenfeld
,
D. W.
(
1969
).
The problem of open-sea navigation: The migration of the green turtle to Ascension Island
.
J. Theor. Biol.
22
,
136
-
179
.
Leduc
,
A. O. H. C.
,
Roh
,
E.
,
Macnaughton
,
C.
,
Benz
,
F.
,
Rosenfeld
,
J.
and
Brown
,
G. E.
(
2010
).
Ambient pH and the response to chemical alarm cues in juvenile Atlantic salmon: mechanisms of reduced behavioral responses
.
Trans. Am. Fish. Soc.
139
,
117
-
128
.
Lewin
,
J.
and
Chen
,
C.-H.
(
1973
).
Changes in the concentration of soluble and particulate iron in seawater enclosed in containers
.
Limnol. Oceanogr.
18
,
590
-
596
.
Lohmann
,
K. J.
and
Lohmann
,
C. M. F.
(
1992
).
Orientation to oceanic waves by green turtle hatchlings
.
J. Exp. Biol.
171
,
1
-
13
.
Lohmann
,
K.
and
Lohmann
,
C.
(
1996
).
Orientation and open-sea navigation in sea turtles
.
J. Exp. Biol.
199
,
73
-
81
.
Lohmann
,
K. J.
and
Lohmann
,
M. F.
(
2019
).
There and back again: natal homing by magnetic navigation in sea turtles and salmon
.
J. Exp. Biol.
222
,
1
-
10
.
Lohmann
,
K. J.
,
Cain
,
S. D.
,
Dodge
,
S. A.
and
Lohmann
,
C. M.
(
2001
).
Regional magnetic fields as navigational markers for sea turtles
.
Science
294
,
364
-
366
.
Lohmann
,
K. J.
,
Luschi
,
P.
and
Hays
,
G. C.
(
2008a
).
Goal navigation and island-finding in sea turtles
.
J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol.
356
,
83
-
95
.
Lohmann
,
K. J.
,
Lohmann
,
C. M. F.
and
Endres
,
C. S.
(
2008b
).
The sensory ecology of ocean navigation
.
J. Exp. Biol.
211
,
1719
-
1728
.
Lohmann
,
K. J.
,
Putman
,
N. F.
and
Lohmann
,
C. M. F.
(
2008c
).
Geomagnetic imprinting: a unifying hypothesis of long-distance natal homing in salmon and sea turtles
.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA
105
,
19096
-
19101
.
Lohmann
,
K. J.
,
Putman
,
N. F.
and
Lohmann
,
C. M. F.
(
2012
).
The magnetic map of hatchling loggerhead sea turtles
.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.
22
,
336
-
342
.
Lohmann
,
K. J.
,
Goforth
,
K. M.
,
Mackiewicz
,
A. G.
,
Lim
,
D. S.
and
Lohmann
,
C. M.
(
2022
).
Magnetic maps in animal navigation
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
208
,
41
-
67
.
Luschi
,
P.
(
2013
).
Long-distance animal migrations in the oceanic environment: orientation and navigation correlates
.
ISRN Zoology
2013
,
1
-
23
.
Luschi
,
P.
,
Hays
,
G. C.
and
Papi
,
F.
(
2003
).
A review of long–distance movements by marine turtles, and the possible role of ocean currents
.
Oikos
103
,
1
-
10
.
Luschi
,
P.
,
Hays
,
G. C.
,
Del Seppia
,
C.
,
Marsh
,
R.
and
Papi
,
F.
(
1998
).
The navigational feats of green sea turtles migrating from Ascension Island investigated by satellite telemetry
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
265
,
2279
-
2284
.
Luschi
,
P.
,
Akesson
,
S.
,
Broderick
,
A. C.
,
Glen
,
F.
,
Godley
,
B. J.
,
Papi
,
F.
and
Hays
,
G. C.
(
2001
).
Testing the navigational abilities of ocean migrants: displacement experiments on green sea turtles (Chelonia mydas)
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
50
,
528
-
534
.
Manton
,
M.
,
Karr
,
A.
and
Ehrenfeld
,
D. W.
(
1972
).
Chemoreception in the migratory sea turtle, Chelonia mydas
.
Biological Bulletin
143
,
184
-
195
.
Mazor
,
T.
,
Beger
,
M.
,
McGowan
,
J.
,
Possingham
,
H. P.
and
Kark
,
S.
(
2016
).
The value of migration information for conservation prioritization of sea turtles in the Mediterranean
.
Glob. Ecol. Biogeogr.
25
,
540
-
552
.
McNamara
,
J. M.
,
Barta
,
Z.
,
Klaassen
,
M.
and
Bauer
,
S.
(
2011
).
Cues and the optimal timing of activities under environmental changes
.
Ecol. Lett.
14
,
1183
-
1190
.
Mirza
,
R.
,
Ferrari
,
M. C. O.
,
Kiesecker
,
J. M.
and
Chivers
,
D. P.
(
2006
).
Responses of American toad tadpoles to predation cues: behavioural response thresholds, threat-sensitivity and acquired predation recognition
.
Behavior
143
,
877
-
889
.
Mrosovky
,
N.
(
1980
).
Thermal biology of sea turtles
.
Am. Zool.
20
,
531
-
547
.
Musick
,
J. A.
and
Limpus
,
C. J.
(
1997
).
Habitat utilization and migration in juvenile sea turtles
.
Biol. Sea Turtles
1
,
137
-
163
.
Owens
,
D. W.
,
Grassman
,
M. A.
and
Hendrickson
,
J. R.
(
1982
).
The imprinting hypothesis and sea turtle reproduction
.
Herpetologica
38
,
124
-
135
.
Pereira
,
J.
,
Gabioux
,
M.
,
Marta-Almeida
,
M.
,
Cirano
,
M.
,
Paiva
,
A. M.
and
Aguiar
,
A. L.
(
2014
).
The bifurcation of the western boundary current system of the South Atlantic Ocean
.
Rev. Bras. Geofísica
32
,
241
-
257
.
Peterson
,
R. G.
and
Stramma
,
L.
(
1991
).
Upper-level circulation in the South Atlantic Ocean
.
Prog. Oceanogr.
26
,
1
-
73
.
Pfaller
,
J. B.
,
Goforth
,
K. M.
,
Gil
,
M. A.
,
Savoca
,
M. S.
and
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
2020
).
Odors from marine plastic debris elicit foraging behavior in sea turtles
.
Curr. Biol.
30
,
R213
-
R214
.
Polovina
,
J.
,
Uchida
,
I.
,
Balazs
,
G.
,
Howell
,
E. A.
,
Parker
,
D.
and
Dutton
,
P.
(
2006
).
The Kuroshio Extension Bifurcation Region: a pelagic hotspot for juvenile loggerhead sea turtles
.
Deep Sea Res. II Top. Stud. Oceanogr.
53
,
326
-
339
.
Putman
,
N. F.
and
Mansfield
,
K. L.
(
2015
).
Direct evidence of swimming demonstrates active dispersal in the sea turtle ‘‘Lost Years
’’.
Curr. Biol.
25
,
1221
-
1227
.
Putman
,
N. F.
,
Scott
,
R.
,
Verley
,
P.
,
Marsh
,
R.
and
Hays
,
G. C.
(
2012a
).
Natal site and offshore swimming influence fitness and long-distance ocean transport in young sea turtles
.
Mar. Biol.
159
,
2117
-
2126
.
Putman
,
N. F.
,
Verley
,
P.
,
Shay
,
T. J.
and
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
2012b
).
Simulating transoceanic migrations of young loggerhead sea turtles: merging magnetic navigation behavior with an ocean circulation model
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
1863
-
1870
.
Rodrigues
,
R. R.
,
Rothstein
,
L. M.
and
Wimbush
,
M.
(
2007
).
Seasonal variability of the south equatorial current bifurcation in the Atlantic Ocean: a numerical study
.
J. Phys. Oceanogr.
37
,
16
-
30
.
Salmon
,
M.
and
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
1989
).
Orientation cues used by hatchling loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta L.) during their offshore migration
.
Ethology
83
,
215
-
228
.
Salmon
,
M.
,
Wyneken
,
J.
,
Fritz
,
E.
and
Lucas
,
M.
(
1992
).
Seafinding by hatchling sea turtles: role of brightness, silhouette and beach slope as orientation cues
.
Behaviour
122
,
56
-
77
.
Southwood
,
A.
and
Avens
,
L.
(
2010
).
Physiological, behavioral, and ecological aspects of migration in reptiles
.
J. Compa. Physiol. B Biochem. Syst. Envir. Physiol.
180
,
1
-
23
.
Thévenin
,
M. R.
,
Pereira
,
J.
and
Lessa
,
G. C.
(
2019
).
Shelf-break upwelling on a very narrow continental shelf adjacent to a western boundary current formation zone
.
J. Mar. Syst.
194
,
52
-
65
.
Wang
,
J. H.
,
Jackson
,
J. K.
and
Lohmann
,
K. J.
(
1998
).
Perception of wave surge motion by hatchling sea turtles
.
J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol.
229
,
177
-
186
.
Witherington
,
B. E.
and
Martin
,
E. R.
(
2005
).
Understanding, assessing, and resolving light pollution problems on sea turtle nesting beaches
.
Florida Mar. Res. Inst. Tech. Rep. TR-2
1
-
84
.
Wyneken
,
J.
(
1997
). Sea turtle locomotion: mechanisms, behavior, and energetics. In
The Biology of Sea Turtles
(ed.
P. L.
Lutz
,
J. A.
Musick
), pp.
165
-
198
.
Boca Raton
:
CRC Press
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information