Optical detection of horizontally polarized light is widespread among aquatic insects. This process usually occurs in the UV or blue spectral ranges. Recently, it was demonstrated that at least one collembolan species, the water springtail (Podura aquatica) also possesses positive polarotaxis to horizontally polarized light. These hexapods are positively phototactic, live on the surface of calm waters and usually accumulate close to the riparian vegetation. In laboratory experiments, we measured the wavelength dependence of phototaxis and polarotaxis of P. aquatica in the 346–744 nm and 421–744 nm ranges, respectively. According to our results, the action spectrum of phototaxis is bimodal with two peaks in the blue (λ1=484 nm) and green-yellow (λ2=570 nm) ranges, while polarotaxis operates in the blue spectral range. For the first time, we show that collembolan polarotaxis functions in the same spectral range as the polarotaxis of many aquatic insects. We present our experiments and discuss the possible ecological significance of our findings.

Springtails (Collembola) are tiny arthropods that play essential roles in terrestrial ecosystems by contributing to the decomposition of organic matter. Currently, approximately 7000 species are known, but at least 50,000 species are estimated to be present on our planet (Hopkin, 1997). They inhabit all types of terrestrial habitats, including the most extreme places like deserts, polar regions and the highest mountains (Hopkin, 1997). Nearly all of the springtails feed on decaying plant material and fungi (Rusek, 1998), although a few species have been reported to feed on live plants (Hopkin, 1997; Bishop et al., 1998). Collembola species are usually negatively phototactic (Salmon and Ponge, 1998; Fox et al., 2007) and live in the uppermost layer of the soil. In contrast to the majority of springtails, the water-surface-inhabiting Podura aquatica Linnaeus 1758 is positively phototactic, like other species living on plants or on the surface of water (Shaller, 1972). Unlike desert-inhabiting springtails, P. aquatica is extremely sensitive to desiccation; thus, water intake must happen almost constantly, which is usually performed via a specialized organ called the ventral tube (Noble-Nesbitt, 1963). When undisturbed, these dark-bodied springtails congregate at the surface of ponds or very slowly moving waters close to the shore and the riparian vegetation (Verheijen and Brouwer, 1971; Childs, 1915) (Fig. 1). In the case of an approaching object, they perform several-centimeter jumps with the help of their fork-shaped locomotory organ, the furcula. Although most springtails possess eyes, they have no more than eight ommatidia per eye, which is also the case for P. aquatica. Moreover, due to the high acceptance angles of these ommatidia (up to 80 deg), their fields of view usually overlap meaning that the resolution of collembolan eyes is very low (Shaller, 1972).

Fig. 1.

Habitat of Podura aquatica. (A) One of the ponds near Budapest, Hungary, where springtails were collected during spring. (B,C) Podura aquatica resting on decaying plant material.

Fig. 1.

Habitat of Podura aquatica. (A) One of the ponds near Budapest, Hungary, where springtails were collected during spring. (B,C) Podura aquatica resting on decaying plant material.

Numerous aquatic insects recognize water surfaces optically by means of the water-reflected horizontally polarized light (Schwind, 1983, 1984, 1989, 1991, 1995; Wildermuth, 1998; Horváth and Varjú, 2004; Csabai et al., 2006; Horváth et al., 2008). Recently, as the first example for a Collembola species, we have demonstrated that P. aquatica also possess positive polarotaxis to horizontally polarized light (Egri et al., 2016). Now, our primary aim was to study the wavelength dependence of the photo- and polarotaxis of P. aquatica. In this study, we present the results of laboratory choice experiments and we discuss how the spectral and reflection polarization characteristics of the environment may synergistically determine the habitat selection of P. aquatica.

Springtails

Female and male adult P. aquatica were collected from the surface of ponds near Budapest (47° 36′ 37.29″ N, 19° 7′ 24.24″ E, Fig. 1) and were kept at 13°C under 12 h:12 h dark:light conditions in plastic containers with pond water, duckweed and other plant leaves collected from the same site. Diffuse light of a cool white LED lamp was used for lighting the containers. The emission spectrum of this illumination was measured with a radiometrically calibrated, cosine corrector (CC-3-UV-S) equipped Ocean Optics STS-VIS spectrometer (Ocean Optics, Largo, USA) (Fig. S1A). Based on this measurement, the total photon flux of this illumination was 1.47×1014 photons cm−2 s−1. Animals were kept in captivity for no more than 5 days before experiments were carried out.

Light source

In the experiments, a custom built, Arduino UNO controlled light source was used which contained 14 power LEDs covering the 346–744 nm spectral range (Fig. 2). A Moritex SOHC4S3.5-1500S four-branch quartz light guide was used to deliver the light stimuli to the springtails. The LEDs were fixed on a two-dimensional motorized stage in a box. To emit light of the desired LED into the light guide, the stage was moved to the proper position. The intensity of light at the light guide outputs was controlled electronically and also with neutral density filters: (i) the currently selected LED light intensity was controlled with a pulse width modulation (PWM; Barr, 2001) signal with a frequency of fPWM=980 Hz; (ii) a filter wheel was applied between the light guide and the LEDs mounted on the motorized stage. The filter wheel had 4 windows: The first window was empty, the second, third and fourth contained one, two and three pairs of Lee 210 and Lee 211 (LEE Filters, Andover, UK) neutral density filters, respectively (Fig. S1B). Using a combination of these two intensity control methods, the light intensity of the different monochromatic stimuli could be varied through several log intensity units depending on the wavelength. The light intensity of each LED in the light source was calibrated using an Ocean Optics STS-VIS spectrometer through empty space, and the transmission spectra of the neutral density filters in the filter wheel were also measured (Fig. S1B). Thus, the light intensity of any stimulus could be calculated for a given PWM signal. The measurement range of the spectrometer allowed us to calibrate only 13 out of the 14 monochromatic LEDs. The shortest wavelength LED (CUD4AF1B, λ=346 nm, Seoul Viosys, Seoul, Republic of Korea) was calibrated with an OPT101 photodiode based on the photodiode's spectral sensitivity curve, and the shape of the LED's emission spectrum both available in the corresponding datasheets. The exact types of the other power LEDs were unknown, but their emission spectra were measured. Fig. 2 shows the normalized emission spectrum of each LED measured at the light-guide outputs. Throughout this study, light intensities of stimuli in the experiments and measured emission spectra were calibrated for photon numbers.

Fig. 2.

Emission spectra of the monochromatic light stimuli in case of equal photon flux for each LED. Areas under curves are equal. Numbers show the peak wavelength of each LED.

Fig. 2.

Emission spectra of the monochromatic light stimuli in case of equal photon flux for each LED. Areas under curves are equal. Numbers show the peak wavelength of each LED.

In addition to the calibration of the light source, we checked whether the angular distribution of light exiting the light guide outputs were similar. We aimed one light guide output to a perpendicular white paper at 14 cm distance. For each wavelength, the other side of the paper was photographed in RAW image format with a NIKON D3200 DSLR camera. The intensity along the diameter of the light spot was plotted, a Gaussian function was fitted on each measurement and full-width half-maximums (FWHM) were calculated (Fig. S2). The high noise in Fig. S2N occurred because the light of the longest wavelength LED (744 nm) was almost invisible for the camera. The FWHM for the light guide outputs were α1=21.63±1.35 deg, α2=22.91±1.43 deg, α3=22.86±1.32 deg, and α4=22.02±1.55 deg (mean±s.d.); thus, the angular distribution of the stimuli of each output was quite similar.

Experiment 1. Action spectrum of phototaxis

The wavelength dependence of positive phototaxis of P. aquatica was measured in the 346–744 nm range at 14 different wavelengths in a darkened chest lined with black material. Springtails were tested in an 86×26×1 mm (length×width×height) rectangular arena made of black cardboard (Fig. 3). Three pieces of 1 mm thick 30×30 mm quartz glasses were used for covering the arena to prevent the escape of the springtails and to allow their observation and stimulation with light. Stimulation was performed with the previously described light source using all 4 outputs of the light guide in parallel: light guide outputs were directed onto one end of the arena from 60 deg measured from the horizontal; thus, when a light stimulus was delivered to the springtails, they could move in a light gradient. A 3 mm sandblasted glass was placed between the light guide and the arena to act as a diffuser to ensure a smooth light gradient in the arena. The transmission spectrum of the quartz and sandblasted glass were taken into account when a light stimulus was applied (Fig. S1B). The movement of springtails in the arena was recorded by a Genius WideCam F100 USB webcam modified for infrared sensitivity. The arena was illuminated from above by an SMD5050-150-IR infrared LED strip (length 30 cm) with an emission peak at 940 nm. Springtails were detected with an algorithm adapted from Egri et al. (2016).

During trials, the relative humidity, which was measured with a HIH-4000 Series humidity sensor, varied between 50% and 55%. Since P. aquatica is extremely sensitive to low humidity like other Collembola species, too much time in the arena would have been lethal for the tested springtails and would have produced false results. Based on preliminary tests we found that the first 15–20 min were not stressful for them and they were moving constantly, but thereafter we observed decrease in their activity: after 50 min, only 20% of them were moving. For this reason, we could not allow very long dark adaptation times before each light stimulus.

In each trial, one wavelength was tested as follows: 20–35 specimens were placed in the arena where they could freely walk, but could not jump. After 3 min of dark adaptation, the test started by applying 30 s light stimuli separated with 60 s dark periods. Except for the 744 nm stimulus, 6 light stimuli were applied in a given trial with logarithmically increasing light intensity. This was due to the increased transmission of the neutral density filters above 700 nm (Fig. S1B), which did not allow us to set the initial light intensity as low as for the other wavelengths. The purpose of the dark periods was twofold. Besides the need for dark adaptation, the springtails had to be given the opportunity to distribute randomly before the next stimulus. For measuring the springtail attraction to light for a given light intensity and wavelength, we calculated their relative centroid shift Δx towards the illuminated side of the arena at the end of the stimulus measured from the arena center (see Fig. 3B). Here, we call this shift relative because it was measured in arena length units. The implementation of the trials and the evaluation process is demonstrated by Movie 1. In this experiment, n=10 trials were performed for each wavelength and a total of 3613 P. aquatica were tested. To test if a given light stimulus elicited a significant response, we applied a Mann–Whitney U-test for comparing the distribution of responses (Δx at the end of the stimulus) with the distribution of Δx values at the beginning of the given stimulus.

The action spectrum of phototaxis was obtained very similarly to the method described in Mazza et al. (2010). Mean responses (Δx) of springtails were plotted as a function of the logarithm of stimulus intensity J=log(Istimulus) for all wavelengths the following sigmoid dose–response curve was fitted on these mean responses with Gnuplot v5.2 (http://www.gnuplot.info):

formula
(1)

where A1 and A2 are the two extreme values of the curve, J50% is the logarithm of light intensity I50% corresponds to 50% response and s is related to the steepness of the curve. We applied A2=0 for fitting because in the case of Istimulus=0 the expected Δx value was zero. Thus for each wavelength I50% is the light intensity required for eliciting half of the maximal response. The action spectrum was calculated by obtaining the reciprocals of these intensities as a function of wavelength, then normalizing the curve with the maximal value. The uncertainty of the points of the action spectrum was obtained from the asymptotic standard error of the fitted J50% parameters. Cubic spline interpolation was used to locate the maxima of the action spectrum.

Experiment 2. Action spectrum of polarotaxis I

We performed choice experiments with P. aquatica to test their preference for horizontally polarized light over vertically and unpolarized light as a function of wavelength in the 421–744 nm range, including 11 different wavelengths. The choice arena was a 30×10×6.5 cm (length×width×height) two-window arena where the springtails could move towards the preferred light stimulus from the center (Fig. 4). The stimulus windows were composed of 7.1×5.7 cm sandblasted glass (thickness 3 mm) and the inner surface of the arena was covered with matte white paper for minimizing polarization-related disturbances. Before each test, a new piece of white paper was used at the bottom to minimize odor clues from the previously tested springtails. The light stimuli were created with the same light source as described above, but by using 2 outputs of the light guide per stimulus window of the arena. The light from the light guide outputs was diffused by a creased aluminium foil reflector symmetrically on each side of the arena. With this experimental arrangement, equal intensity stimulus pairs could be delivered into the arena across the two windows. The tests were performed in the same darkened chest as experiment 1, and the same infrared camera method was used for recording the springtails (Fig. 4).

Fig. 3.

Measurement of the action spectrum of phototaxis. (A) Experimental setup, showing the arena used for experiment 1. (B) Example image of the camera during a light stimulus of λstimulus=442 nm. The dashed and solid lines represent the horizontal position of the arena center and the centroid of springtails needed for the calculation of Δx.

Fig. 3.

Measurement of the action spectrum of phototaxis. (A) Experimental setup, showing the arena used for experiment 1. (B) Example image of the camera during a light stimulus of λstimulus=442 nm. The dashed and solid lines represent the horizontal position of the arena center and the centroid of springtails needed for the calculation of Δx.

Fig. 4.

Measurement of the wavelength dependence of polarotaxis. (A) Photograph of the experimental setup used for experiments 2 and 3. (B) Setup with a horizontally polarized blue stimulus (λstimulus=442 nm). (C) Aluminium reflector used to diffusely reflect light from the light guide outputs. (D) Arena in position in front of the reflector. (E) Example of the detection of springtails for a horizontally/vertically (left/right) polarized stimulus pair in experiment 2 (t=70 s, λstimulus=496 nm). Relative centroid shift of springtail Δx towards horizontally polarized stimulus calculated as in Fig. 3B.

Fig. 4.

Measurement of the wavelength dependence of polarotaxis. (A) Photograph of the experimental setup used for experiments 2 and 3. (B) Setup with a horizontally polarized blue stimulus (λstimulus=442 nm). (C) Aluminium reflector used to diffusely reflect light from the light guide outputs. (D) Arena in position in front of the reflector. (E) Example of the detection of springtails for a horizontally/vertically (left/right) polarized stimulus pair in experiment 2 (t=70 s, λstimulus=496 nm). Relative centroid shift of springtail Δx towards horizontally polarized stimulus calculated as in Fig. 3B.

Before each trial, the desired stimulus pair was set, then 80–150 P. aquatica specimens were placed in a standing plastic releaser tube (diameter 28 cm) at the center of the arena. After 30 s, the releaser was removed and the chest was quickly closed. During the following 110 s, images were saved by the camera every second. Later, all of the photographs were evaluated with the method of Egri et al. (2016), thus the number of springtails at the left, middle and right third of the arena and the relative centroid shift Δx were obtained as a function of time. For each trial, these values were averaged in the 80–110 s interval. For example, regarding the relative centroid shift of the springtails, Δx was considered as the mean of the measured Δx values during the last 30 s of the given trial. The same principle was applied for the determination of the springtail numbers in the left, middle and right thirds of the arena. Numbers of choices were considered as numbers of springtails in the left or right third of the arena, while numbers of specimens in the middle third of the arena were treated as numbers of inactive individuals that did not choose stimulus. With this method, the track of animals could not be determined because the springtails usually performed jumps during the tests. The animals could not jump out from the arena, but at the sides they could get under the paper lining on the bottom, thus a slight decrease in their numbers may have occurred, but it was negligible. Movie 2 summarizes the evaluation method of the trials of experiments 2 and 3.

In experiment 2, we tested the preference of P. aquatica for horizontally polarized versus unpolarized stimuli. In the case of real positive polarotaxis a highly polarized light stimulus is more attractive than an unpolarized stimulus with the same emission spectrum even if the polarized light's intensity is two or more times lower (Egri et al., 2016). Since P. aquatica is positively polarotactic to horizontally polarized white light (Egri et al., 2016), one of the best strategies to test the wavelength dependence of polarotaxis was to use horizontally polarized and unpolarized stimulus pairs with at least two-times dimmer light stimulus on the polarized side. Hence, polarized stimuli were created by inserting a visual range linear polarizer (XP42-18, ITOS, Mainz, Germany) between the aluminium reflector and the window of the arena with horizontal transmission axis (Fig. 4D). This arrangement inherently produced an approximately 60% dimmer polarized stimulus than the unpolarized stimulus with no polarizer. The photon flux of the unpolarized stimuli was 8.6×1012 photons cm−2 s−1. Table 1 summarizes the relative light intensities of polarized stimuli compared with the corresponding unpolarized stimulus (no polarizer) as a function of wavelength calculated from the emission spectra of the unpolarized stimuli and the transmission spectrum of the polarizer. In case of visible wavelengths, the degree of polarization d of stimuli was measured by imaging polarimetry (Horváth and Varjú, 1997, 2004; Egri et al., 2016), and the results showed that d was practically constant across the applied wavelengths (Table 1). Because the emission spectrum of the stimuli were narrow peaks (Fig. 2), the polarized and unpolarized stimuli had practically the same emission spectrum.

Table 1.

Optical characteristics of polarized stimuli in experiment 2 as a function of wavelength

Optical characteristics of polarized stimuli in experiment 2 as a function of wavelength
Optical characteristics of polarized stimuli in experiment 2 as a function of wavelength

For each stimulus pair, 4 trials were performed, 2 with swapped stimuli in order to compensate for incidental asymmetries of the setup. We plotted the total numbers of choices and mean centroid shifts towards horizontally polarized stimulus (Δx) as a function of wavelength. For estimating the wavelength of maximal polarotaxis, we fitted a Gaussian function on the Δx points corresponding to the positive polarotaxis reactions. To check the significance of total choice numbers for a given wavelength, χ2 tests were applied; in the corresponding 4 trials, the total number of springtails choosing the horizontally polarized stimulus were compared with the total number of specimens choosing the unpolarized side.

Experiment 3. Action spectrum of polarotaxis II

According to Egri et al. (2016), for a given emission spectrum, the most attractive, moderately attractive and least attractive light for P. aquatica are horizontally polarized, unpolarized and vertically polarized, respectively. Thus, for a given wavelength, the most striking reaction could be achieved by a horizontally/vertically polarized stimulus pair. For this reason, in experiment 3, we also performed trials with horizontally and vertically polarized stimuli by applying polarizers at both sides of the arena. Consequently, in these trials, the light intensities of the stimuli were equal. In this experiment, the total number of springtails choosing horizontally and vertically polarized stimulus were compared with χ2 tests for all tested wavelengths (Table 1).

Spectral characteristics of the habitats of P. aquatica

At our springtail collecting site, several small lakes and ponds are located where P. aquatica is abundant. At one of these water bodies, we measured the spectral characteristics of the environment from the aspect of a springtail standing on the water in the vicinity of the riparian vegetation. Spectral acquisitions were performed with the same Ocean Optics STS-VIS spectrometer. With the help of a small spirit level, the cosine corrector (acceptance angle 180 deg) of the spectrometer was positioned 4 cm above the water surface with a horizontal optical axis. Measurements were performed from a fishing pier (i) towards the open water and (ii) towards the bank. During the measurements, the spectrometer was 80 cm from the vegetation (reed) at the bank of the small lake. Measurements in both directions were performed 4 times each under sunny and totally overcast conditions during moderate solar elevation. The aim of these measurements was to get spectral information about the summed, global light coming from the side of open water and the riparian zone at the position of a hypothetical springtail standing on the water in the vicinity of the shore.

Fig. 5 demonstrates the average responses of springtails to increasing light intensity for all 14 wavelengths, with asterisks indicating if the given stimulus had a significant effect (P<0.05), according to the Mann–Whitney U-test. Fig. 6 shows the action spectrum of phototaxis of P. aquatica as a function of wavelength in the 346–744 nm spectral range calculated from the sigmoid dose-response curves fitted on the points of Fig. 5. The action spectrum of phototaxis is mostly restricted to the visible range, and is bimodal with peaks at wavelengths λ1=484 nm and λ2=570 nm. In other words, the phototaxis of P. aquatica has two peaks, one in the blue and one in the yellowish-green ranges of human vision (Fig. 6). This is the reason why a visible range polarizer was sufficient for the polarotaxis experiments (experiments 2 and 3); however, there was a small but significant phototaxis below the λ1 peak.

Fig. 5.

Phototactic responses of P. aquatica as a function of photon flux for 14 different wavelengths. Points represent the means of 10 trials ±1 s.e., while solid lines are the fitted sigmoid dose-response curves (Eqn 1). Asterisks display the significance in Mann–Whitney U-tests (P<0.05).

Fig. 5.

Phototactic responses of P. aquatica as a function of photon flux for 14 different wavelengths. Points represent the means of 10 trials ±1 s.e., while solid lines are the fitted sigmoid dose-response curves (Eqn 1). Asterisks display the significance in Mann–Whitney U-tests (P<0.05).

Fig. 6.

Action spectrum of phototaxis of P. aquatica. Vertical bars were calculated from the asymptotic standard errors of log photon flux (J50%) needed for eliciting 50% response, and the dashed line shows cubic spline interpolation. λ1=484 nm and λ2=570 nm are the positions of the two maxima.

Fig. 6.

Action spectrum of phototaxis of P. aquatica. Vertical bars were calculated from the asymptotic standard errors of log photon flux (J50%) needed for eliciting 50% response, and the dashed line shows cubic spline interpolation. λ1=484 nm and λ2=570 nm are the positions of the two maxima.

Fig. 7 demonstrates the responses of springtails to horizontally polarized/unpolarized stimulus pairs as a function of wavelength in the spectral range of 421–744 nm. In Fig. 7A, the proportion of total springtail choices are shown for each wavelength. Black and white bars represent the proportion of choices in the horizontally polarized and unpolarized side of the arena, respectively, while gray bars indicate the proportion of inactive specimens that stayed in the middle third of the arena. For all wavelengths, the total number of tested springtails (Npolarized+Nmiddle+Nunpolarized) is shown by numbers in the gray bars. Asterisks show the significance of χ2 tests calculated from the total number of choices obtained in the two sides of the arena (Npolarized versus Nunpolarized). The means of relative centroid shifts (Δx) of the 4 trials are plotted in Fig. 7B as a function of wavelength with vertical bars denoting standard errors.

Fig. 7.

Response of springtails to horizontally polarized light against unpolarized stimulus as a function of wavelength (experiment 2). (A) Proportion of total springtails in the arena terminals corresponding to the horizontally polarized stimuli (black bars), unpolarized stimuli (white bars) and the inactive specimens in the middle of the arena (gray bars). Total numbers of tested springtails are shown in the gray bars. *P<0.05, ***P<0.0001, in χ2 tests. (B) Mean relative centroid shifts Δx of springtails towards horizontally polarized stimulus. Vertical bars represent the s.e. of the 4 trials. Gaussian function fitted on the 421–552 nm region is shown by solid line. Wavelength of Gaussian maximum is λexp2=491 nm.

Fig. 7.

Response of springtails to horizontally polarized light against unpolarized stimulus as a function of wavelength (experiment 2). (A) Proportion of total springtails in the arena terminals corresponding to the horizontally polarized stimuli (black bars), unpolarized stimuli (white bars) and the inactive specimens in the middle of the arena (gray bars). Total numbers of tested springtails are shown in the gray bars. *P<0.05, ***P<0.0001, in χ2 tests. (B) Mean relative centroid shifts Δx of springtails towards horizontally polarized stimulus. Vertical bars represent the s.e. of the 4 trials. Gaussian function fitted on the 421–552 nm region is shown by solid line. Wavelength of Gaussian maximum is λexp2=491 nm.

According to Fig. 7, in experiment 2 the dimmer horizontally polarized light (Table 1) was more attractive than the unpolarized stimulus at three neighboring wavelengths between 467 nm and 516 nm. Thus, this was the range of positive polarotaxis for horizontally polarized light. Gaussian fitting revealed that the maximal polarotactic reaction corresponds to λexp2=491 nm. On both sides of this peak, phototaxis overwhelmed polarotaxis and the unpolarized stimulus was more attractive than the horizontally polarized one. Regarding the wavelengths over 641 nm, no significant preference could be observed.

Fig. 8 shows the responses of P. aquatica in experiment 3 for equal-intensity stimulus pairs of horizontally and vertically polarized light with the same concept as Fig. 7. The shape of the bar plot and the curve in Fig. 8 is qualitatively similar to those in Fig. 7. Horizontally polarized light was significantly more attractive for the springtails than the vertically polarized stimulus again in the range of 467–516 nm. It is also clear that the magnitude of polarotactic reactions was higher in experiment 3 (Fig. 8) than in experiment 2 (Fig. 7), which is not surprising because the horizontally/vertically polarized stimulus pair elicits the most striking reactions (Egri et al., 2016). In the case of experiment 3, the wavelength of maximal attraction to horizontally polarized light was λexp3=492 nm.

Fig. 8.

Response of springtails to equal-intensity stimulus pairs of horizontally and vertically polarized light (experiment 3). (A) Proportion of total springtails in the arena terminals corresponding to the horizontally polarized stimuli (black bars), vertically polarized stimuli (white bars) and the inactive specimens in the middle of the arena (gray bars). Total numbers of tested springtails are shown in the gray bars. **P<0.001, ***P<0.0001, in χ2 tests. (B) Mean relative centroid shifts Δx of springtails towards horizontally polarized stimulus. Vertical bars represent the s.e. of the 4 trials. Gaussian function fitted on the 421–552 nm region is shown by solid line. Wavelength of Gaussian maximum is λexp3=492 nm.

Fig. 8.

Response of springtails to equal-intensity stimulus pairs of horizontally and vertically polarized light (experiment 3). (A) Proportion of total springtails in the arena terminals corresponding to the horizontally polarized stimuli (black bars), vertically polarized stimuli (white bars) and the inactive specimens in the middle of the arena (gray bars). Total numbers of tested springtails are shown in the gray bars. **P<0.001, ***P<0.0001, in χ2 tests. (B) Mean relative centroid shifts Δx of springtails towards horizontally polarized stimulus. Vertical bars represent the s.e. of the 4 trials. Gaussian function fitted on the 421–552 nm region is shown by solid line. Wavelength of Gaussian maximum is λexp3=492 nm.

The results of spectral measurements of the habitat of P. aquatica are shown in Fig. 9. Fig. 9A demonstrates a panorama photograph of the scene where the spectral measurements were performed. Thick blue and green arrows represent the horizontal direction of the optical axis of the spectrometer's cosine corrector in the case of the measurements towards the open water and bank side, respectively. Fig. 9B and C indicate the mean measured spectra when the weather was sunny and overcast, respectively, thus each curve is an average of 4 measurements. We worked with spectra measured in photon units rather than power units, because biological processes depend on the number of absorbed photons (Johnsen, 2012). In Fig. 9B and C, the FWHM of the two peaks of the action spectrum of phototaxis (Fig. 6) are shown as gray shaded regions. To estimate the ratio of phototaxis-eliciting short (∼λ1) and long (∼λ2) wavelength photons, we integrated the spectra in the FWHM regions of the action spectrum of phototaxis and divided the second by the first (Fig. 9B,C). It is clear from these graphs, that independent of the weather conditions, in this model the behavior-influencing light intensity around λ1 was relatively higher than around λ2 in the case of the open water side (blue curve) than in the case of the bank side (green curve). In other words, from the aspect of a P. aquatica specimen, the bank side had more long wavelength components than the side of open water independent of the cloud coverage. It is also clear that the light intensities were lower in most wavelengths towards the bank, because towards the open water the brighter skylight was more dominant.

Fig. 9.

Spectral characteristics of a natural habitat of P. aquatica. (A) Panorama photo of a small lake from a fishing pier. The arrows represent horizontal measuring directions (blue: towards open water; green: towards bank). (B) Measured spectrum of the sunny environment with spectrometer directed towards the open water (blue) and the bank (green). Gray regions show the FWHM of the two peaks of the action spectrum of phototaxis (Fig. 6). Mean±s.d. ratios of integrated photon counts (I2/I1) in the regions around λ1 and λ2 are shown with blue (open water) and green (bank) formulas. (C) Measured spectrum under totally overcast conditions.

Fig. 9.

Spectral characteristics of a natural habitat of P. aquatica. (A) Panorama photo of a small lake from a fishing pier. The arrows represent horizontal measuring directions (blue: towards open water; green: towards bank). (B) Measured spectrum of the sunny environment with spectrometer directed towards the open water (blue) and the bank (green). Gray regions show the FWHM of the two peaks of the action spectrum of phototaxis (Fig. 6). Mean±s.d. ratios of integrated photon counts (I2/I1) in the regions around λ1 and λ2 are shown with blue (open water) and green (bank) formulas. (C) Measured spectrum under totally overcast conditions.

Spectral sensitivity of insects usually ranges from the UV to the green regions of the spectrum, although some butterfly species possess receptors with absorption maximum in the red channel (Menzel and Backhaus, 1991; Briscoe and Chittka, 2001). As Fig. 6 shows, the phototaxis of P. aquatica is restricted to the blue-green-yellow range with two peaks (λ1=484 nm and λ2=570 nm). According to the results of experiments 2 and 3 (Figs 7,8), there were spectral regions where (i) polarotaxis dominated, (ii) simply phototaxis occurred or (iii) no preference was recorded. Polarotaxis was observed in a narrow spectral region in the blue range. In Figs 7 and 8 it is also shown that spectral regions where phototaxis overwhelmed the attraction to horizontally polarized light occurred on both sides of the shorter wavelength peak (λ1) of the action spectrum of phototaxis (Fig. 6). In Fig. 6, a slight increase can be seen towards the UV range, but our measurement range does not allow us to make a judgment if there is really another peak of phototaxis in the UV range. Light stimuli with wavelengths above 641 nm did not elicit any significant reactions in the experiments. This is also shown by the low choice numbers for these long wavelengths in Fig. 7A and Fig. 8A. Low choice numbers compared with the numbers of inactive individuals indicate that the springtails did not perceive the red/infrared stimuli well. Although the polarization characteristics of the stimuli at the arena terminals were different, the majority of the springtails stayed in the middle third of the arena, because such long wavelengths were sensed as darkness. This is not surprising, because insects usually lack red-sensitive photoreceptors (Menzel and Backhaus, 1991).

Based on these results, we suggest that P. aquatica possess at least two different visual pigments; however, it should be tested with absorption spectroscopy or electroretinography, but the latter is an extremely difficult task for such tiny and vulnerable animals. What we can unambiguously conclude is that polarotaxis of P. aquatica functions in the blue spectral range peaking between 484 nm and 492 nm, and longer wavelengths, particularly around λ2=570 nm, are also perceived well by this species but this range plays a role only in phototactic behavior. In nature, there is no situation when the global illumination is monochromatic, because the Sun is a black body radiator, thus it has a broad emission spectrum (Cronin et al., 2014). Consequently, in a terrestrial natural habitat the light scattered from the sky or returned from field objects (e.g. water, plants, rocks, soil) will usually contain most wavelengths of the Sun's global illumination, but the spectral composition can change depending on the absorption and reflection spectrum of these objects. Our spectral measurements of the habitats of P. aquatica (Fig. 9) were performed using a cosine corrector with a hemispherical field of view. This method was suitable only for collectively measuring the spectrum of all lights coming from the direction of the shore or the open water. Although we do not know exactly what kind of image forms in the visual system of P. aquatica, it is clear that the measured irradiances (Fig. 9) towards the shore contain relatively more long-wavelength (∼λ2) components compared with the short-wavelength components (∼λ1). However, as every amateur and professional naturalist knows, field objects in nature are commonly greenish, yellowish or brownish, but towards the open waters the spectral composition of the blue or cloudy sky dominates. It is a common observation that P. aquatica usually avoid open waters and accumulates in the intimate vicinity of the shore, where decaying and living plant materials are usually present (Verheijen and Brouwer, 1971; Childs, 1915) (Fig. 1). We suggest that the second peak of the action spectrum (λ2 in Fig. 6) allows the discrimination of the riparian zone from the open water; however, this should be tested in further experiments.

In aquatic insects, polarization sensitivity usually operates in the short-wavelength spectral range, mainly in the UV or blue (Schwind, 1991, 1995). It has been explained as an adaptation to the optical environment since in nature the degree of polarization of water-reflected light increases with decreasing wavelength (Schwind, 1995; Horváth and Varjú, 1997). Based on our results P. aquatica possesses the same ability with its blue-sensitive positive polarotaxis. As another example for spectral adaptation of polarotaxis, Flamarique and Browman (2000) demonstrated that two Daphnia species utilize middle-wavelength pigments for polarization vision, a wavelength range that is more common underwater in lakes and ponds, which are the habitats of these planktonic crustaceans. For P. aquatic, the ability of polarization vision may not only be important for maintaining position on the water surface or in the intimate vicinity of water bodies. According to Childs (1915), this species burrows into the mud close to the shores when winter is approaching and returns to the water surface at the beginning of spring. Thus polarization vision may also play a role in water seeking for P. aquatica right after the dormant state; however, it has been also claimed that P. aquatica overwinters in the mud at the bottom of ponds (Heckman, 2018).

The main message of our study is that similarly to a variety of aquatic insects, the polarotaxis of P. aquatica functions in the blue spectral range, but longer wavelength light is also important for this species. Polarotaxis allows water detection and phototaxis may play a role in recognition of the riparian zone. With this strategy, water springtail with their low-resolution eyes can easily visually find the safest and food-rich habitats, which are water surfaces surrounded by vegetation. In the present paper, we suggest that the optical habitat selection of P. aquatica relies on both the reflection-polarization and spectral characteristics of their visual environment.

We thank Antal Bocsi, Gábor Bocsi and Miklós Egri for their help in the design of our light source. We are grateful for the constructive advice of the reviewers.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: A.E., G.K.; Methodology: A.E., G.K.; Software: A.E.; Validation: A.E.; Formal analysis: A.E.; Investigation: A.E.; Resources: A.E., G.K.; Data curation: A.E.; Writing - original draft: A.E., G.K.; Writing - review & editing: A.E., G.K.; Visualization: A.E.; Supervision: A.E., G.K.; Project administration: A.E.; Funding acquisition: A.E.

Funding

This work was supported by the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office (NKFIH PD-115451 to A.E.) and the Economic Development and Innovation Operational Programme (GINOP-2.3.2-15-2016-00057). The project was also supported by the János Bolyai Research Scholarship of the Hungarian Academy of Sciences.

Barr
,
M.
(
2001
).
Pulse width modulation
.
Embedded Syst. Program.
14
,
103
-
104
.
Bishop
,
A. L. M.
,
McKenzie
,
H. J.
,
Barchia
,
I. M.
and
Spohr
,
L. J.
(
1998
).
Efficacy of insecticides against the lucerne flea, Sminthurus viridis (L.) (Collembola: Sminthuridae), and other arthropods in lucerne
.
Aust. J. Entomol.
37
,
40
-
48
.
Briscoe
,
A. D.
and
Chittka
,
L.
(
2001
).
The evolution of color vision in insects
.
Annu. Rev. Entomol.
46
,
471
-
510
.
Childs
,
G. H.
(
1915
).
Some observations on the life history of the water springtail (Podura aquatica - 1758)
.
MSc thesis, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis
.
Cronin
,
T. W.
,
Johnsen
,
S.
,
Marshall
,
N. J.
and
Warrant
,
E. J.
(
2014
).
Visual Ecology
.
Princeton
:
Princeton University Press
.
Csabai
,
Z.
,
Boda
,
P.
,
Bernáth
,
B.
,
Kriska
,
G.
and
Horváth
,
G.
(
2006
).
A ‘polarisation sun-dial’ dictates the optimal time of day for dispersal by flying aquatic insects
.
Freshwater Biol.
51
,
1341
-
1350
.
Egri
,
Á.
,
Farkas
,
A.
,
Kriska
,
G.
and
Horváth
,
G.
(
2016
).
Polarization sensitivity in Collembola: an experimental study of polarotaxis in the water-surface-inhabiting springtail Podura aquatica
.
J. Exp. Biol.
219
,
2567
-
2576
.
Flamarique
,
I.
and
Browman
,
H. I.
(
2000
).
Wavelength-dependent polarization orientation in Daphnia
.
J. Comp. Physiol A
186
,
1073
-
1087
.
Fox
,
G. L.
,
Coyle-Thompson
,
C. A.
,
Bellinger
,
P. F.
and
Cohen
,
R. W.
(
2007
).
Phototactic responses to ultraviolet and white light in various species of collembola, including the eyeless species, Folsomia candida
.
J. Insect Sci.
7
,
1
-
12
.
Heckman
,
C. W.
(
2018
).
Ecological Strategies of Aquatic Insects
.
Boca Raton
:
CRC Press, Taylor & Francis Group
.
Hopkin
,
S. P.
(
1997
).
Biology of Springtails (Insecta: Collembola)
.
Oxford; New York; Tokyo
:
Oxford University Press
.
Horváth
,
G.
and
Varjú
,
D.
(
1997
).
Polarization pattern of freshwater habitats recorded by video polarimetry in red, green and blue spectral ranges and its relevance for water detection by aquatic insects
.
J. Exp. Biol.
200
,
1155
-
1163
.
Horváth
,
G.
and
Varjú
,
D.
(
2004
).
Polarized Light in Animal Vision–Polarization Patterns in Nature
.
Heidelberg; Berlin; New York
:
Springer
.
Horváth
,
G.
,
Majer
,
J.
,
Horváth
,
L.
,
Szivák
,
I.
and
Kriska
,
G.
(
2008
).
Ventral polarization vision in tabanids: horseflies and deerflies (Diptera: Tabanidae) are attracted to horizontally polarized light
.
Naturwissenschaften
95
,
1093
-
1100
.
Johnsen
,
S.
(
2012
).
The Optics of Life
.
Princeton, NJ
:
Princeton University Press
.
Mazza
,
C. A.
,
Izaguirre
,
M. M.
,
Curiale
,
J.
and
Ballaré
,
C. L.
(
2010
).
A look into the invisible: ultraviolet-B sensitivity in an insect (Caliothrips phaseoli) revealed through a behavioural action spectrum
.
Proc. R. Soc. Biol. Sci. Ser. B
277
,
367
-
373
.
Menzel
,
R.
and
Backhaus
,
W.
(
1991
).
Colour vision in insects
. In
The Perception of Colour
, Vol.
6
,
vision and visual dysfunction
(ed.
P.
Gouras
), pp.
262
-
293
.
London
:
Macmillan
.
Noble-Nesbitt
,
J.
(
1963
).
A site of water and ionic exchange with the medium in Podura aquatica L. (Collembola, Isotomidae)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
40
,
701
-
711
.
Rusek
,
J.
(
1998
).
Biodiversity of Collembola and their functional role in the ecosystem
.
Biodiv. Conserv.
7
,
1207
-
1219
.
Salmon
,
S.
and
Ponge
,
J.-F.
(
1998
).
Responses to light in a soil-dwelling springtail
.
Eur. J. Soil Biol.
34
,
199
-
201
.
Schwind
,
R.
(
1983
).
A polarization-sensitive response of the flying water bug Notonecta glauca to UV light
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
150
,
87
-
91
.
Schwind
,
R.
(
1984
).
Evidence for true polarization vision based on a two-channel analyzer system in the eye of the water bug, Notonecta glauca
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
154
,
53
-
57
.
Schwind
,
R.
(
1989
).
A variety of insects are attracted to water by reflected polarized light
.
Naturwissenschaften
76
,
377
-
378
.
Schwind
,
R.
(
1991
).
Polarization vision in water insects and insects living on a moist substrate
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
169
,
531
-
540
.
Schwind
,
R.
(
1995
).
Spectral regions in which aquatic insects see reflected polarized light
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
177
,
439
-
448
.
Shaller
,
F.
(
1972
).
Observations on the visual reactions of Collembola
. In
Information Processing in the Visual Systems of Arthopods
(ed.
R.
Wehner
), pp.
249
-
253
.
Heidelberg; Berlin; New York
:
Springer
.
Verheijen
,
F. J.
and
Brouwer
,
J. M. M.
(
1971
).
Orientation of Podura aquatica (L.) (Collembola, Insecta) in a natural angular radiance distribution
.
Neth. J. Zool.
22
,
72
-
80
.
Wildermuth
,
H.
(
1998
).
Dragonflies recognize the water of rendezvous andoviposition sites by horizontally polarized light: a behavioural field test
.
Naturwissenschaften
85
,
297
-
302
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information