Insect societies are often composed of many individuals, achieving collective decisions that depend on environmental and colonial characteristics. For example, ants are able to focus their foraging effort on the most rewarding food source. While this phenomenon is well known, the link between the food source quality and the intranidal food dissemination networks and its dynamics has been neglected. Here, we analysed the global dynamics of food dissemination in Camponotus cruentatus workers, after feeding on a low (0.1 mol l−1) or on a high (1 mol l−1) sucrose concentration food source. We also analysed the trophallaxis activity at the individual level and built the complete network of trophallaxis. The results reveal that the dynamics of food dissemination and the structure of the trophallaxis network are robust and independent of the food concentration. We discuss these results in the light of recent advances in the study of efficiency in food management in ants.

Social insects, and ants in particular, live in large and complex societies. Information sharing and the presence of feedback loops between workers (Schafer et al., 2006) allow for a collective exploitation of the best quality food source (e.g. Beckers et al., 1993) while coping with the colony needs (Portha et al., 2002; Sorensen et al., 1985). The individual and collective feeding behaviour in ants is influenced by the food source characteristics such as the sucrose concentration (Beckers et al., 1992; Cassill, 2003; Josens et al., 1998; Price et al., 2016; Reid et al., 2012) and the nature of the food (Markin, 1970; Sorenşen et al., 1981; Sorensen and Vinson, 1981), as well as by the level of starvation of the colony (Josens and Roces, 2000). However, the way in which these parameters impact food dissemination inside the colony and, in particular, the individual patterns of trophallaxis exchange, their dynamics and the structure of the associated networks, have been largely overlooked (Cassill, 2003; Greenwald et al., 2015, 2018; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010). Yet, different parameters, such as starvation, affect these dynamics and networks, facilitating food recruitment, which speeds up the dynamics of food accumulation and dissemination at the intranidal level (Buffin et al., 2012; Mailleux et al., 2010; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010). At low or intermediate starvation level, the crop is still partially laden (Cassill and Tschinkel, 1999b) and the propensity of ants to recruit and to exchange food increases with food concentration (Cassill, 2003). Moreover, this propensity also increases with starvation level (Cassill, 2003; Mailleux et al., 2011) and leads to a large total number of trophallactic events and to a fast food dissemination through the nest (Buffin et al., 2012; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010). This increase in speed is partially due to reorganization of the intranidal process of food spreading, such as a higher proportion of ants that are both donors and receivers when highly starved compared with satiated. In addition, fluid intake rate and crop filling increase with starvation (Josens and Roces, 2000) and food of a lower quality is enough to stimulate the foragers to collect food (Mayor et al., 1987; Mc Cabe et al., 2006). Some nutrients that are ignored by foragers at low levels of starvation are collected and distributed within colonies after a lengthy period of starvation (Chong et al., 2002). This reveals a lower food quality acceptability threshold of highly starved ants. During trophallaxis, the ant receiving food contacts the donor's head and mouth with its forelegs. At low levels of deprivation, this rate of contact and the probability that receivers accept trophallaxis increases with food concentration. This effect of food concentration is not observed at high levels of starvation (Mc Cabe et al., 2006). Therefore, we hypothesized that after a long starvation duration, the total number and the structure of the networks of trophallaxis, the dynamics of food accumulation and the heterogeneity (distribution) of the trophallactic activity among individuals (Cassill, 2003; Sendova-Franks et al., 2010) should be similar regardless of food concentration. To test this hypothesis and to provide a baseline model, we compared the food dissemination activity for two food sources differing in their sucrose concentration (0.1 versus 1 mol l−1) in highly starved (5 days) groups of Camponotus cruentatus workers.

Experimental setup and procedure

From five large mother colonies of the ant Camponotus cruentatus (Latreille 1802) (collected in Rochefort du Gard, France, September 2016), we created 10 subcolonies of 25 randomly picked individuals, kept in a plastic box (175×125×50 mm) containing a circular nest (95×6 mm), with access to water and sucrose solution (0.3 mol l−1). All subcolonies were maintained in a controlled environment, with a temperature of 21±1°C, a relative humidity of 60±5% and a constant 12 h/12 h photoperiod. Ants were individually labelled with an Aruco tag (https://sites.google.com/site/usetrackerac/) allowing automatic identification of ants. Each tag was attached to the abdomen, had a side length of 1.25 mm and weighed less than 1% of the average mass of the ant. After 5 days of starvation, ants had access to a 0.1 mol l−1 (low concentration) or 1 mol l−1 (high concentration) sucrose solution feeder placed 40 mm from the nest entrance (Fig. S1A) and were filmed for a period of 120 min. Each colony was randomly tested three times at low and high concentration (N=60), with a 5 day break between the two trials.

Statistical analysis

Experiments were filmed using Panasonic GH4 cameras at a resolution of 4K. Based on snapshots taken every 4 min, in each experiment, we manually counted the number of trophallactic events. Using this footage, trophallactic events and the respective role of both partners (donor or receiver), were identified on the basis of body posture and the mandible position (Cassill and Tschinkel, 1999a; Greenwald et al., 2015; see also Fig. S1B). A Mann–Whitney U-test (MW) and a Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (KS) were respectively used to compare the total (Fig. 1A) and the temporal cumulative number of trophallactic events (Fig. 1B) between all experiments at high versus low food source concentration. Additionally, in a sample of four colonies, each tested once at high and low concentration, we recorded the identity of the donor and the receiver of each trophallactic event in the nest based on body posture and the position of the mandibles (Greenwald et al., 2015). The complete trophallaxis network was then built (see Fig. S2). Each node corresponds to one ant having performed at least one trophallactic event. Pairs of distinct nodes were connected with a directed and weighted edge, from the donor to the receiver. We calculated the ‘betweenness’, the closeness, the eigenvector and the clustering coefficient of each node of the network. Betweenness is an estimate of how important an individual ant is for promoting connectivity across the entire colony and is measured by the number of times an individual acts as a bridge along the shortest path between two other ants (Dell et al., 2014). Closeness is based on the shortest paths from an individual to every other individual: the more central an ant is, the lower its total distance from all other ants (Wey et al., 2008). The eigenvector is a value accounting for the centrality of a node's neighbours (Butts, 2008). The clustering coefficient determines the existence of ‘communities’ in a network, such as nodes with many more edges between these nodes than with the others (Saramäki et al., 2007). To test for an effect of food concentration on the structure of the trophallaxis network and on the distribution of the trophallaxis activity, for each metric, we applied a Kruskall–Wallis test (KW) of homogeneity on the N=8 (4×2) experiments (Table 1). If the KW indicated an inhomogeneity (P<0.05), we compared the two distributions (respectively for 0.1 and 1 mol l−1), for each colony, with a MW.

Fig. 1.

Mean and cumulative number of trophallactic events. (A) Comparison of the total number of trophallactic events at high (1 mol l−1) and low (0.1 mol l−1) food source concentration. ‘Current concentration' indicates the number of trophallactic events counted in the experiment (Ni) irrespective of the food source concentration of the previous experiment (Ni–1). ‘Previous concentration' indicates the number of trophallactic events counted in the experiment (Ni) given a food source concentration of the past experiment (Ni–1), irrespective of the food concentration of the current experiment (Ni). Numbers above bars indicate the number of experiments; lines inside bars are s.d. (Mann–Whitney U-test: P>0.33 in each case). (B) Cumulative number of trophallactic events (mean±s.d.) at both high and low food concentration. A Kolmogorov–Smirnov test indicates an absence of difference in the dynamics of trophallaxis (P=0.94).

Fig. 1.

Mean and cumulative number of trophallactic events. (A) Comparison of the total number of trophallactic events at high (1 mol l−1) and low (0.1 mol l−1) food source concentration. ‘Current concentration' indicates the number of trophallactic events counted in the experiment (Ni) irrespective of the food source concentration of the previous experiment (Ni–1). ‘Previous concentration' indicates the number of trophallactic events counted in the experiment (Ni) given a food source concentration of the past experiment (Ni–1), irrespective of the food concentration of the current experiment (Ni). Numbers above bars indicate the number of experiments; lines inside bars are s.d. (Mann–Whitney U-test: P>0.33 in each case). (B) Cumulative number of trophallactic events (mean±s.d.) at both high and low food concentration. A Kolmogorov–Smirnov test indicates an absence of difference in the dynamics of trophallaxis (P=0.94).

Table 1.

Comparison of the homogeneity of network parameters between the eight individually analysed experiments

Comparison of the homogeneity of network parameters between the eight individually analysed experiments
Comparison of the homogeneity of network parameters between the eight individually analysed experiments

We statistically evaluated the heterogeneity of the distribution of the trophallaxis activity among all the workers of each of N=8 experiments, using the Lorenz curve (Fig. 2; Fig. S3A) and the Gini coefficient (Fig. S3B). The Lorenz curve displays the share of trophallaxis activity (Y-axis) accounted for by X% of workers (sorted by the number of trophallactic events per individual) in the colony. A perfectly equitable distribution of foraging activity would correspond to the line Y=X. The Gini coefficient is known as the ratio between the area below the experimental Lorenz curve and the triangular area below the perfect equality case Y=X, and provides a measure of the degree of inequality in the distribution of trophallaxis activity, ranging from 0 (perfect equality) to 1 (perfect inequality).

Fig. 2.

Distribution of trophallaxis activity. Lorenz curves showing the cumulative percentage of all trophallactic events (Y-axis) performed by the X% of workers (sorted by the number of trophallactic events per individual) for each experiment (N=8) and food source quality.

Fig. 2.

Distribution of trophallaxis activity. Lorenz curves showing the cumulative percentage of all trophallactic events (Y-axis) performed by the X% of workers (sorted by the number of trophallactic events per individual) for each experiment (N=8) and food source quality.

To estimate whether the observed Gini coefficients and the social network metrics were different from random expectation, we compared each empirical network against two ensembles of N=1000 randomized networks we created by (1) randomly rewiring all edges between all nodes, destroying all features of the original network (full random network, FR); (2) rewiring the edges of the original network while maintaining the distribution of the number of trophallactic events (given/received) by each individual (degree random network, DR) (Holme and Saramäki, 2012). We then performed a Z-test (ZT) to evaluate the significance of the differences between the observed and random metrics. All analysis was conducted in Python 3.6 with NetworkX 2.1, PyGraphViz 1.4, Numpy 1.14, Scipy 1.0.0 and MatPlotLib 2.2.2 packages.

The total number of trophallactic events observed (Fig. 1A, MW: P>0.33 in each case) and the dynamics of food accumulation (Fig. 1B, KS, D=0.07, P=1.0) were independent of the current and the previous (whatever the current concentration) food source concentration.

The proportion of ants involved in trophallaxis activity was independent of the food concentration: ∼90% of ants performed at least one trophallactic event and ∼70% of ants gave and received food in each experiment (Fisher exact test, P>0.40 in each case). All eight experiments were homogeneous in terms of the distribution of the number of trophallactic events individually performed and of social network metrics of each individual (Table 1, KW, P>0.05), showing a robustness of the food dissemination and trophallaxis network against food concentration. The closeness was, however, significantly different between the high and low food concentration in two colonies, but without a clear trend, as closeness was higher at high concentration in one of the colonies and at low concentration in the other (MW: P<0.05). All trials, regardless of food concentration, had a significant skew in the distribution of the trophallaxis activity: 25% of the workers accounted for more than 50% of the total number of trophallactic events over the course of each experiment (Fig. 2). The observed Gini coefficients were systematically higher than the corresponding theoretical ones obtained from the FR networks (Fig. S3A,B; ZT: P<0.0001). The comparison of each experimentally measured social network metric revealed a significant difference from the corresponding FR network (Figs S4, S5; ZT: P<0.05), except for the clustering coefficient of two experiments at 1 mol l−1. Finally, no difference occurred between experimentally measured metrics and those from DR networks (Figs S4, S5; ZT: P>0.05), except for the closeness coefficient of an experiment and the eigenvector coefficient of two experiments. The highlighted heterogeneity of the trophallaxis activity seems therefore to be the main factor shaping the trophallaxis network as the experimental networks were not different from the theoretical DR ones. Crop content modulates the individual trophallaxis rate (Greenwald et al., 2018) but we observed a skew in the total number of trophallactic events performed by each ant. This suggests that a high proportion of ants that receive food in a quantity that exceeds their individual nutritional need redistribute it. Therefore, trophallaxis allows them not only to meet their individual needs but also to contribute to food dissemination throughout the colony (Quevillon et al., 2014). Division of labour is assumed to be based on a distribution of the threshold responses among the workers (Pinter-Wollman et al., 2012). In contrast, starvation lowers thresholds (Mailleux et al., 2011) and potentially allows for identical responses to very different food concentrations. Despite this phenomenon, our results suggest that interindividual differences in activity are still maintained.

Our study is, to our knowledge, the first one concerned with the effects of food source concentration on food dissemination activity in ants. Foraging and recruitment processes are affected by food concentration (Cassill, 2003; Josens et al., 1998; Mailleux et al., 2006; Reid et al., 2012). However, our results suggest that the food dissemination network and dynamics depend mainly on the nutritional needs rather than on the concentration of food for long starvation durations (at least for the two different concentrations tested here). This is in agreement with the literature showing that insects consume a large range of food types/quality when starved (Jackson et al., 1998; Mayor et al., 1987; Scharf, 2016). Note that this study concerns subgroups of workers and that further experiments are needed to generalize our results by testing more natural ant colonies (in the presence of queen and brood).

Further investigations are also required to link the mechanisms underlying the observed interindividual variability in trophallaxis activity, colony needs and food dissemination. For example, testing the effect of shorter starvation durations would be important as less hungry individuals with non-empty crop content (Greenwald et al., 2018) exchange only highly energy-rich food and ignore food at low concentration (Cassill, 2003). In contrast, the starvation duration we tested could have led to a high level of excitation that may prevent any effect of food concentration on the dynamics and the structure of the intranidal trophallaxis network.

We thank two anonymous referees for their helpful comments.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: O.B.; Methodology: O.B., S.C.N.; Validation: O.B., J.-L.D., S.C.N.; Formal analysis: O.B., S.C.N.; Resources: O.B.; Data curation: O.B.; Writing - original draft: O.B., J.-L.D., S.C.N.; Writing - review & editing: O.B., J.-L.D., S.C.N.; Visualization: O.B.; Supervision: S.C.N.; Project administration: S.C.N.; Funding acquisition: O.B.

Funding

O.B. was financed by a PhD grant from Fonds pour la Recherche dans l'Industrie et dans l'Agriculture and a grant from the Van Buuren Fund.

Data availability

Data are available from the Dryad Digital Repository (Bles et al., 2018): dryad.r29th82

Beckers
,
R.
,
Deneubourg
,
J.-L.
and
Goss
,
S.
(
1992
).
Trails and U-turns in the selection of a path by the ant Lasius niger
.
J. Theor. Biol.
159
,
397
-
415
.
Beckers
,
R.
,
Deneubourg
,
J.-L.
and
Goss
,
S.
(
1993
).
Modulation of trail laying in the ant Lasius niger (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) and its role in the collective selection of a food source
.
J. Insect Behav.
6
,
751
-
759
.
Bles
,
O.
,
Deneubourg
,
J.
and
Nicolis
,
S. C.
(
2018
).
Data from: Food dissemination in ants: robustness of the trophallactic network against resource quality
.
Dryad Digital Repository
.
Buffin
,
A.
,
Goldman
,
S.
and
Deneubourg
,
J. L.
(
2012
).
Collective regulatory stock management and spatiotemporal dynamics of the food flow in ants
.
FASEB J.
26
,
2725
-
2733
.
Butts
,
C. T.
(
2008
).
Social network analysis: a methodological introduction
.
Asian J. Soc. Psychol.
11
,
13
-
41
.
Cassill
,
D.
(
2003
).
Rules of supply and demand regulate recruitment to food in an ant society
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
54
,
441
-
450
.
Cassill
,
D. L.
and
Tschinkel
,
W. R.
(
1999a
).
Regulation of diet in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta
.
J. Insect Behav.
12
,
307
-
328
.
Cassill
,
D. L.
and
Tschinkel
,
W. R.
(
1999b
).
Task selection by workers of the fire ant Solenopsis invicta
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
45
,
301
-
310
.
Chong
,
A.
,
Chong
,
N. L.
,
Yap
,
H. H.
and
Lee
,
C. Y.
(
2002
).
Effects of starvation on nutrient distribution in the pharaoh ant, Monomorium pharaonis (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) workers and various larval stages
.
Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Urban Pests
.
Charleston, SC
,
USA
.
7-10 July 2002
,
121
-
128
.
Dell
,
A. I.
,
Bender
,
J. A.
,
Branson
,
K.
,
Couzin
,
I. D.
,
de Polavieja
,
G. G.
,
Noldus
,
L. P. J. J.
,
Pérez-Escudero
,
A.
,
Perona
,
P.
,
Straw
,
A. D.
,
Wikelski
,
M.
, et al. 
(
2014
).
Automated image-based tracking and its application in ecology
.
Trends Ecol. Evol.
29
,
417
-
428
.
Greenwald
,
E.
,
Segre
,
E.
and
Feinerman
,
O.
(
2015
).
Ant trophallactic networks: simultaneous measurement of interaction patterns and food dissemination
.
Sci. Rep.
5
,
12496
.
Greenwald
,
E. E.
,
Baltiansky
,
L.
and
Feinerman
,
O.
(
2018
).
Individual crop loads provide local control for collective food intake in ant colonies
.
eLife
7
,
e31730
.
Holme
,
P.
and
Saramäki
,
J.
(
2012
).
Temporal networks
.
Phys. Rep.
519
,
97
-
125
.
Jackson
,
R. R.
,
Li
,
D.
,
Barrion
,
A. T.
and
Edwards
,
G. B.
(
1998
).
Prey-capture techniques and prey preferences of nine species of ant-eating jumping spiders (Araneae: Salticidae) from the Philippines
.
New Zeal. J. Zool.
25
,
249
-
272
.
Josens
,
R. B.
and
Roces
,
F.
(
2000
).
Foraging in the ant Camponotus mus: nectar-intake rate and crop filling depend on colony starvation
.
J. Insect Physiol.
46
,
1103
-
1110
.
Josens
,
R. B.
,
Farina
,
W. M.
and
Roces
,
F.
(
1998
).
Nectar feeding by the ant Camponotus mus: intake rate and crop filling as a function of sucrose concentration
.
J. Insect Physiol.
44
,
579
-
585
.
Mailleux
,
A.-C.
,
Detrain
,
C.
and
Deneubourg
,
J.-L.
(
2006
).
Starvation drives a threshold triggering communication
.
J. Exp. Biol.
209
,
4224
-
4229
.
Mailleux
,
A.-C.
,
Sempo
,
G.
,
Depickère
,
S.
,
Detrain
,
C.
and
Deneubourg
,
J. L.
(
2010
).
How does starvation affect spatial organization within nests in Lasius niger?
Insectes Soc.
58
,
219
-
225
.
Mailleux
,
A.-C.
,
Buffin
,
A.
,
Detrain
,
C.
and
Deneubourg
,
J.-L.
(
2011
).
Recruitment in starved nests: the role of direct and indirect interactions between scouts and nestmates in the ant Lasius niger
.
Insectes Soc.
58
,
559
-
567
.
Markin
,
G. P.
(
1970
).
Food distribution within laboratory colonies of the argentine ant, Tridomyrmex Humilis (Mayr)
.
Insectes Soc.
17
,
127
-
158
.
Mayor
,
K. L.
,
Aracena
,
J. M.
and
Bell
,
W. J.
(
1987
).
Search duration of Drosophila melanogaster on Homogeneous sucrose patches: relative effects of starvation period, sucrose concentration and patch size
.
J. Ethol.
5
,
67
-
74
.
Mc Cabe
,
S.
,
Farina
,
W. M.
and
Josens
,
R. B.
(
2006
).
Antennation of nectar-receivers encodes colony needs and food-source profitability in the ant Camponotus mus
.
Insectes Soc.
53
,
356
-
361
.
Pinter-Wollman
,
N.
,
Hubler
,
J.
,
Holley
,
J.-A.
,
Franks
,
N. R.
and
Dornhaus
,
A.
(
2012
).
How is activity distributed among and within tasks in Temnothorax ants?
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
66
,
1407
-
1420
.
Portha
,
S.
,
Deneubourg
,
J.-L.
and
Detrain
,
C.
(
2002
).
Self-organized asymmetries in ant foraging: a functional response to food type and colony needs
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
13
,
776
-
781
.
Price
,
R. I. A.
,
Grüter
,
C.
,
Hughes
,
W. O. H.
and
Evison
,
S. E. F.
(
2016
).
Symmetry breaking in mass-recruiting ants: extent of foraging biases depends on resource quality
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
70
,
1813
-
1820
.
Quevillon
,
L. E.
,
Hanks
,
E. M.
,
Bansal
,
S.
and
Hughes
,
D. P.
(
2014
).
Social, spatial, and temporal organization in a complex insect society
.
Sci. Rep.
5
,
1
-
11
.
Reid
,
C. R.
,
Latty
,
T.
and
Beekman
,
M.
(
2012
).
Making a trail: informed Argentine ants lead colony to the best food by U-turning coupled with enhanced pheromone laying
.
Anim. Behav.
84
,
1579
-
1587
.
Saramäki
,
J.
,
Kivelä
,
M.
,
Onnela
,
J.-P.
,
Kaski
,
K.
and
Kertész
,
J.
(
2007
).
Generalizations of the clustering coefficient to weighted complex networks
.
Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlinear, Soft Matter Phys.
75
,
2
-
5
.
Schafer
,
R. J.
,
Holmes
,
S.
and
Gordon
,
D. M.
(
2006
).
Forager activation and food availability in harvester ants
.
Anim. Behav.
71
,
815
-
822
.
Scharf
,
I.
(
2016
).
The multifaceted effects of starvation on arthropod behaviour
.
Anim. Behav.
119
,
37
-
48
.
Sendova-Franks
,
A. B.
,
Hayward
,
R. K.
,
Wulf
,
B.
,
Klimek
,
T.
,
James
,
R.
,
Planqué
,
R.
,
Britton
,
N. F.
and
Franks
,
N. R.
(
2010
).
Emergency networking: famine relief in ant colonies
.
Anim. Behav.
79
,
473
-
485
.
Sorensen
,
A. A.
and
Vinson
,
S. B.
(
1981
).
Quantitative food distribution studies within Laboratory colonies of the imported fire ant, Solenopsis invicta Buren
.
Insectes Soc.
28
,
129
-
160
.
Sorenşen
,
A. A.
,
Mirenda
,
J. T.
and
Vinson
,
S. B.
(
1981
).
Food exchange and distribution by three functional worker groups of the imported fire ant Solenopsis invicta Buren
.
Insectes Soc.
28
,
383
-
394
.
Sorensen
,
A. A.
,
Busch
,
T. M.
and
Vinson
,
S. B.
(
1985
).
Control of food influx by temporal subcastes in the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
17
,
191
-
198
.
Wey
,
T.
,
Blumstein
,
D. T.
,
Shen
,
W.
and
Jordán
,
F.
(
2008
).
Social network analysis of animal behaviour: a promising tool for the study of sociality
.
Anim. Behav.
75
,
333
-
344
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information