Fish must integrate information from multiple sensory systems to mediate adaptive behaviors. Visual, acoustic and chemosensory cues provide contextual information during social interactions, but the role of mechanosensory signals detected by the lateral line system during aggressive behaviors is unknown. The aim of this study was first to characterize the lateral line system of the African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni and second to determine the role of mechanoreception during agonistic interactions. The A. burtoni lateral line system is similar to that of many other cichlid fishes, containing lines of superficial neuromasts on the head, trunk and caudal fin, and narrow canals. Astatotilapia burtoni males defend their territories from other males using aggressive behaviors that we classified as non-contact or contact. By chemically and physically ablating the lateral line system prior to forced territorial interactions, we showed that the lateral line system is necessary for mutual assessment of opponents and the use of non-contact fight behaviors. Our data suggest that the lateral line system facilitates the use of non-contact assessment and fight behaviors as a protective mechanism against physical damage. In addition to a role in prey detection, the diversity of lateral line morphology in cichlids may have also enabled the expansion of their social behavioral repertoire. To our knowledge, this is the first study to implicate the lateral line system as a mode of social communication necessary for assessment during agonistic interactions.

Fish must integrate information from multiple sensory modalities to mediate adaptive social behaviors under varying environmental conditions and contexts. While many sensory systems are used during typical behavioral interactions in fishes, a disproportionate amount of research has focused on the role of visual (Chen and Fernald, 2011; Grosenick et al., 2007; Korzan and Fernald, 2007; Korzan et al., 2008; Rosenthal and Ryan, 2000), auditory (Amorim et al., 2003, 2004; Lobel, 1998; Maruska et al., 2012; Simões et al., 2008) and chemosensory signals (Almeida et al., 2005; Barata et al., 2007; Keller-Costa et al., 2015; Martinovic-Weigelt et al., 2012; Maruska and Fernald, 2012). In contrast, little is known about the role of the mechanosensory lateral line system in mediating adaptive social behaviors, especially during territorial or aggressive encounters.

The mechanosensory lateral line system senses changes in the environment through detection of near-flow water movements relative to the fish (Coombs, 1994; Coombs et al., 1996; Dijkgraaf, 1962; McHenry and Liao, 2014). The functional unit of the lateral line system is a neuromast composed of sensory hair cells and support cells covered by a gelatinous cupula (Dijkgraaf, 1962). Neuromasts are located either superficially on the skin surface (superficial neuromasts, SN) or enclosed within dermal canals (canal neuromasts, CN) (Webb, 1989). Neuromasts are stimulated when the cupula is deflected by viscous drag, activating the mechanotransduction channels in the hair cells, and allowing for sensory perception of water movements relative to the movement of the fish (van Netten and McHenry, 2014). Lateral line system morphology is highly variable across fishes (Webb, 1989, 2014), and its spatial organization within a species can provide insight into its sensory role(s).

The lateral line system has been well studied for its involvement in topographic interactions with the environment (e.g. rheotaxis; Kulpa et al., 2015; Montgomery et al., 1997), sensation of water movements generated by other fish (e.g. schooling behavior; Pitcher et al., 1976; and predator–prey interactions; Coombs and Patton, 2009; Schwalbe et al., 2012), but few studies have examined the role of the lateral line system for sensing intentional water movements during intra-specific social communication. For example, a few studies have demonstrated that lateral line-mediated vibrational communication is used during reproductive and spawning behaviors in different fishes (Marchesan et al., 2000; Medina et al., 2013; Satou et al., 1994). In contrast, while a hypothesized role for the lateral line system during aggressive interactions has been suggested in the literature for years (Enquist et al., 1990), there is currently no direct experimental support for the role of mechanoreception in mediating territorial behaviors in any fish species. As aggressive displays typically include body and fin movements that generate hydrodynamic cues (e.g. chases, opercular displays, tail movements, body quivers; Keenleyside and Yamamoto, 1962; McMillan and Smith, 1974; Stewart et al., 2013; Stewart et al., 2014; Yoshizawa et al., 2014), the reception of these signals by the lateral line system is poised to provide crucial information for opponent assessment and to mediate appropriate behavioral decisions.

The African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni is a highly social fish with well-characterized aggressive and courtship behaviors (Fernald, 1977; Fernald and Hirata, 1977; Maruska and Fernald, 2010b). Male A. burtoni exist as two distinct phenotypes (dominant and subordinate) and can reversibly switch between the two depending on their social environment, causing a suite of rapid behavioral and physiological changes (Maruska, 2014; Maruska and Fernald, 2014). Dominant, territorial males are brightly colored and aggressively defend a territory and court females. Subordinate, non-territorial males physically resemble females and are reproductively suppressed. Social status is tied to reproductive success, feeding opportunities and growth rate (Hofmann et al., 1999; Maruska and Fernald, 2014), and in A. burtoni is dependent on a male's ability to successfully defend his spawning territory. To do this, males use a variety of aggressive behaviors, such as lateral displays, border fights, biting and mouth fights (Fernald and Hirata, 1977; Table 1). During lateral displays and border fights, fish are in close proximity (<1 body length, BL), but not physically touching. While these behaviors produce visual cues, they also involve pushing water at each other, likely activating the lateral line system. Other behaviors, such as biting and mouth fighting, involve physical contact but also likely stimulate the lateral line system because of the close proximity of the opponent. In addition to territorial interactions, A. burtoni males produce hydrodynamic cues during courtship behaviors via body quivers, tail waggles and leads, presumably also providing relevant information (e.g. size, fitness) to potential mates. While visual, acoustic and chemosensory signaling is used during A. burtoni social interactions (Chen and Fernald, 2011; Maruska et al., 2012; Maruska and Fernald, 2012), it is also likely, but untested, that hydrodynamic information influences social decision making in this species.

List of abbreviations
     
  • BL

    body length

  •  
  • CN

    canal neuromast

  •  
  • DASPEI

    2-[4-(dimethylamino)styryl]-N-ethylpyridinium iodine

  •  
  • dpf

    days post-fertilization

  •  
  • IO

    infraorbital canal

  •  
  • Mb

    body mass

  •  
  • MD

    mandibular canal

  •  
  • N

    nares/naris

  •  
  • OT

    otic canal

  •  
  • pLLn

    posterior lateral line nerve

  •  
  • PO

    postotic canal

  •  
  • PR

    preopercular canal

  •  
  • SL

    standard length

  •  
  • SN

    superficial neuromast

  •  
  • SO

    supraorbital canal

  •  
  • ST

    supratemporal canal

  •  
  • T

    trunk canal

Table 1.

Definition and classification of male Astatotilapia burtoni aggressive behaviors

Definition and classification of male Astatotilapia burtoni aggressive behaviors
Definition and classification of male Astatotilapia burtoni aggressive behaviors

The goals of this study were to first characterize the morphology and spatial distribution of the mechanosensory lateral line system in A. burtoni, and then to ablate the lateral line system to test the hypothesis that mechanoreception plays an important role in male–male territorial interactions.

Experimental animals

Adult laboratory-bred Astatotilapia burtoni (Günther 1894) were derived from wild-caught stock from Lake Tanganyika, Africa, and maintained in an environment that mimicked their natural habitat. Fish were housed in 30 l aquaria at 28–30°C on a 12 h light:12 h dark cycle and fed cichlid flakes (AquaDine, Healdsburg, CA, USA) once daily, supplemented with brine shrimp. Animal care and experimental procedures followed approved Louisiana State University IACUC protocols.

Characterization of the mechanosensory lateral line system

To characterize and determine the distribution of the canals and pores of the lateral line system in A. burtoni, a 0.1% Methylene Blue solution was injected into cranial canal pores of freshly killed fish, resulting in visible blue stain throughout all cranial canals (N=6). Trunk canals were visible without staining using a stereomicroscope (SteREO Lumar V.12, Zeiss, Germany). Two adult male A. burtoni [standard length (SL) 71.50±4.50 mm, mean±s.e.m.] were also cleared and stained for further visualization of cranial and trunk canals (procedures outlined in Taylor and Van Dyke, 1985). For visualization of SNs on the skin and CNs inside the canals, fish [6 females, 6 males, 4 juveniles at 12 days post-fertilization (dpf); N=16 total] were immersed in 0.008% DASPEI (2-[4-(dimethylamino)styryl]-N-ethylpyridinium iodine; Molecular Probes, Carlsbad, CA, USA) solution for 20 min, killed in 0.01% benzocaine (Sigma, St Louis, MO, USA) dissolved in cichlid-system water, and imaged using an eGFP filter set (excitation filter 485/20; 525 LP filter) on a stereomicroscope. Neuromasts were counted, classified by location and averaged for each sex, and a composite distribution map was created using the most common placement and number of neuromasts across all animals. Canals and canal pores were then superimposed on the drawing to collectively represent the entire mechanosensory system.

Behavior experiments

To test the role of the lateral line system in aggressive interactions, fish were placed in a novel paradigm in which they had equal opportunity to acquire a territory. A single 10 gallon tank (50.8×31.12×25.4 cm) was divided into two equal compartments by a removable opaque acrylic barrier and a quarter of a terracotta pot was placed on either side of the barrier to serve as a territory for each subject fish (Fig. 1A). Experimental dominant male fish [SL 42.625±0.769 mm, body mass (Mb) 2.148±0.665 g, N=150 total] were chosen based on their displays of typical dominance behaviors in community tanks for ∼5 days before being moved to the experimental tank to acclimate to their new territory for 2 days. Experimental fish were randomly assigned to one of three groups (control, lateral line ablation or sham handling; see below), resulting in four different groups of paired interactions: control fish versus control fish (N=12), sham fish versus sham fish (N=15), sham fish versus ablated fish (N=14), and ablated fish versus ablated fish (N=20). Each animal was used for only one behavior trial. Animals were size matched for both SL and Mb so that no fish was more than 10% larger than his opponent, and to ensure that SL and Mb did not differ across trial pairings (ANOVA, SL: F2,53=0.969, P=0.386; Mb: F2,53=0.427, P=0.654).

Fig. 1.

Experimental tank set-up for territorial interactions between dominant male Astatotilapia burtoni and verification of lateral line ablation treatments. (A) Two dominant males acclimated on either side of an opaque barrier for 2 days with a quarter of a terracotta pot to serve as a territory. Fish were treated for 3 h (cobalt chloride or sham) and placed back in the experimental tank to recover for ∼18 h prior to behavioral testing. On the day of the trial, the center barrier was removed and the two quarter pots repositioned to form a single half-pot territory to induce a territorial fight. Opaque barriers were placed on both ends of the tank to block the view of neighboring fish during trials. (B–E) DASPEI staining of neuromasts  was used to verify treatment efficacy. Fish treated with 1 mmol l−1 EGTA (B) and 0.1 mmol l−1 CoCl2 (C) had normal cranial neuromast staining (green dots) but fish treated with 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2 (D) had reduced or absent staining of cranial neuromasts. e, eye; m, mouth. Yellow, white and red arrows indicate canal neuromasts, superficial neuromasts and the naris, respectively. Arrows are in consistent locations in B–D, demonstrating the absence of staining at arrows in D. (E) None of the treatments affected trunk neuromasts: Ei, normal fish water; Eii, 1 mmol l−1 EGTA; Eiii, 0.1 mmol l−1 CoCl2; Eiv, 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2. Scale bars: B–D, 1 cm; E, 200 μm.

Fig. 1.

Experimental tank set-up for territorial interactions between dominant male Astatotilapia burtoni and verification of lateral line ablation treatments. (A) Two dominant males acclimated on either side of an opaque barrier for 2 days with a quarter of a terracotta pot to serve as a territory. Fish were treated for 3 h (cobalt chloride or sham) and placed back in the experimental tank to recover for ∼18 h prior to behavioral testing. On the day of the trial, the center barrier was removed and the two quarter pots repositioned to form a single half-pot territory to induce a territorial fight. Opaque barriers were placed on both ends of the tank to block the view of neighboring fish during trials. (B–E) DASPEI staining of neuromasts  was used to verify treatment efficacy. Fish treated with 1 mmol l−1 EGTA (B) and 0.1 mmol l−1 CoCl2 (C) had normal cranial neuromast staining (green dots) but fish treated with 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2 (D) had reduced or absent staining of cranial neuromasts. e, eye; m, mouth. Yellow, white and red arrows indicate canal neuromasts, superficial neuromasts and the naris, respectively. Arrows are in consistent locations in B–D, demonstrating the absence of staining at arrows in D. (E) None of the treatments affected trunk neuromasts: Ei, normal fish water; Eii, 1 mmol l−1 EGTA; Eiii, 0.1 mmol l−1 CoCl2; Eiv, 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2. Scale bars: B–D, 1 cm; E, 200 μm.

On the morning (08:00 h–08:30 h) of the trial, fish were allowed to acclimate to the video camera for ∼10 min before a 5 min pre-trial behavior baseline was recorded. The opaque barrier was then removed and the two quarter pots were repositioned to form a single half-pot territory. In this paradigm, the two dominant males with individual territories are forced to fight over the new single shelter in the same tank and have equal opportunity to acquire the territory. During the trial, opaque barriers were also placed on either end of the tank to block the view of fish in adjacent tanks.

All trials were recorded and later quantified for pre-fight and fight behaviors, including contact (fish physically touching each other) and non-contact (fish within 1 BL but not physically touching) behaviors (Table 1). Fight onset was defined as the first reciprocal exchange of aggressive behaviors, and fights had to last a minimum of 30 s. The fight was allowed to continue until a clear winner and loser was established based on criteria similar to those used previously in a study on social defeat (Maruska et al., 2013). Specifically, the winner fish had to fulfill two of the following characteristics: (1) enter the shelter >3 times within a 1 min period, (2) enter and stay in the shelter for >10 consecutive seconds, (3) perform at least three dominance behaviors within 1 min, and (4) chase or bite the other male. The loser fish had to fulfill both of the following criteria: (1) loss of eye bar and bright coloration, and (2) perform typical submissive behaviors (e.g. fleeing, hiding). Latency to fight was defined as the time between removing the barrier and the fight onset, and fight duration as the time between fight onset and establishment of a winner. If no fight occurred within 30 min, the trial was ended and only non-fight behaviors (i.e. time with in 1 BL) were quantified, and a latency time of 30 min was assigned. If during the 30 min trial one fish became dominant and the other subordinate without an aggressive interaction occurring, the trial was marked as a ‘no-contest win’ and behavior data were not used.

Ablation of the mechanosensory lateral line system

To compare behavior of lateral line-intact and lateral line-ablated fish, experimental fish were randomly assigned to one of three groups prior to use in the behavioral experiments described above: control, sham handled or lateral line ablated. Control fish were handled exactly as described above with no additional treatment. There are a variety of methods for pharmacological and chemical ablation of the lateral line system, such as treatment with aminoglycoside antibiotics or cobalt chloride (CoCl2) (Brown et al., 2011; Janssen, 2000; Karlsen and Sand, 1987; Song et al., 1995; Van Trump et al., 2010). As pilot experiments showed that treatment with gentamicin in A. burtoni was only effective after a 4 day exposure at much higher than the recommended dose, we opted to use CoCl2. We first tested 0.1 mmol l−1 CoCl2 for 24 h (as recommended by Janssen, 2000; Karlsen and Sand, 1987), but this treatment resulted in only slightly reduced DASPEI staining of cranial neuromasts and had no effect on trunk neuromasts. Through pilot experiments, we determined that a 3 h treatment with 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2 in low-calcium cichlid-system water effectively ablated >90% of the cranial lateral line system, but still had minimal effect on the trunk lateral line. This CoCl2 treatment was therefore combined with severing the posterior lateral line nerve (pLLn), which resulted in near ablation of the entire lateral line system. Transecting the pLLn was shown to have similar behavioral effects to CoCl2 and antibiotic treatment in other species (Mirjany et al., 2011). Lateral line ablation was done by immersing fish for 3 h in cichlid-system water containing 2 mmol l−1 cobalt chloride hexahydrate (Sigma) with 1 mmol l−1 EGTA (calcium chelating agent; Sigma) that was pH corrected to ∼8.0 with NaOH (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) after CoCl2 and EGTA were dissolved in cichlid-system water. All solutions used in these experiments were adjusted in a similar manner so that pH was between 7.8 and 8.1. Immediately following CoCl2 treatment, fish were placed in ice-cold cichlid-system water for 3 min before the pLLn was bilaterally transected. To cut the nerve, 2–3 scales were gently removed at the posterior dorsal edge of the operculum and a scalpel was used to make a small incision ∼4 mm in length through the skin and superficial musculature. The pLLn was then visible and a 2 mm portion of the nerve was removed. CoCl2 treatment caused ∼90% ablation of cranial CNs and SNs (verified by reduced/absence of DASPEI staining; N=6), and transecting the pLLn removed both SN and CN input from the trunk and caudal fin.

Sham-handled fish were immersed in normal cichlid-system water for 3 h. Following treatment, fish were placed in ice-cold cichlid-system water and a small incision was made into their dorsal musculature near the dorsal fin to mimic the surgery of pLLn-transected fish but ensuring that it did not affect the lateral line system. Following treatment or sham handling, fish were returned to the experimental tank overnight, and behavior experiments took place 18 h after the end of treatment, during which time neuromast function did not recover from CoCl2 (verified by reduced/absence of DASPEI staining; N=4).

Toxicity and anosmic controls

As a calcium channel antagonist, it is possible for cobalt chloride treatment to have unwanted effects, most notably toxicity effects leading to death (Janssen, 2000; Ridgway and Kamofsky, 2006). To ensure that any observed behavior differences were due to lack of mechanoreception, we included several toxicity and anosmic (elimination of olfactory inputs) controls. Previous research suggested 0.1 mmol l−1 as the highest dose usable in fish (Janssen, 2000); however, a 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2 treatment was the lowest dose that consistently ablated the cranial portion of the A. burtoni lateral line system but still had little effect on trunk neuromasts. As this dose is much higher than recommended by Janssen (2000), we included several controls to ensure any behavioral changes were not due to toxicity of CoCl2, EGTA or NaOH (used to raise the pH of the treatment solution to ∼8.0). Fish were treated for 3 h in either 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2 with no EGTA (pH adjusted with NaOH) or 1 mmol l−1 EGTA with no CoCl2 (pH adjusted with NaOH) and allowed to fight as described above (N=3 trials for each pairing). All 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2-treated fish ate almost immediately after treatment, showed normal swimming behavior, had no excess mucus production and had normal coloration. It is also important to note that no fish died as a result of the 3 h, 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2 treatment (N=40), and that a 4 mmol l−1 CoCl2 treatment for 3 h is the lethal dose for A. burtoni.

During verification of our lateral line system ablation treatments, we noted reduced DASPEI staining in the olfactory epithelium compared with control and sham fish, suggesting that 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2 treatment may also impair chemosensory systems. To ensure that any behavior differences were not due to impaired olfaction, we used a micro-cauterizer (Cautery High Temp Adjust-A-Temp Fine Tip, Bovie Medical Corporation, Clearwater, FL, USA) to ablate the olfactory epithelium (anosmic fish) prior to placing fish in the same experimental paradigm used for our mechanosensory trials (N=3). Fish were allowed to recover for 3 days prior to the forced territorial interaction, and behavior from CoCl2-treated, EGTA-treated and anosmic control fish were then compared with behavior of fish from both control and sham trials.

Statistical analyses

Data were analyzed in SigmaPlot 12.3 (Systat Inc., San Jose, CA, USA). Comparisons between fight winners and losers were made with a Student's t-test or Mann–Whitney U-test if normality assumptions were not met. Square root and log transformations were used to normalize data if needed. Comparisons among trial groups were made with one-way ANOVA followed by post hoc Student–Newman–Keuls (SNK) tests for pairwise comparisons (when normality passed), or Kruskal–Wallis (KW) ANOVA on ranks followed by post hoc Dunn's test for multiple comparisons (when normality failed and data could not be normalized by transformation).

Student's t-tests and Pearson correlations were used to compare winner and loser behaviors. Because there was no difference between winners and losers (P>0.05 for all comparisons) and winner and loser behaviors were strongly correlated (P<0.05 for all behaviors), a composite score (winner and loser added together or averaged) for each category was used to avoid pseudo-inflating sample size or influencing statistical tests.

Characterization of the A. burtoni lateral line system

A generalized distribution map of the canals and neuromasts of the A. burtoni lateral line system was created using cleared and stained fish, with Methylene Blue injections to visualize canals and pores, and DASPEI staining to identify neuromasts (Fig. 2A). The A. burtoni lateral line system is similar to the typical teleost lateral line system with seven cranial canals: infraorbital (IO), supraorbital (SO), preopercular (PR), mandibular (MD), postotic (PO), otic (OT) and supratemporal (ST) canal. The SO canal runs down the dorsal part of the head and ends in a pore just prior to the upper lip, and has a tubule that branches laterally to surround the eye and terminates in a pore just above each naris. Typically, a CN was located between adjacent pores and each canal had nearby clusters or lines of SNs (Fig. 2D). The MD canal on the ventral side of the lower jaw is continuous across the midline to connect the left and right sides, while tubules of the SO canal on the dorsal portion of the head also connect the two sides of the fish. The OT and PO canals serve as links between IO and SO, PR and trunk (T) canals. Like most cichlids, A. burtoni possesses a disjunct trunk lateral line canal (Webb, 1989), with a dorsally located rostral portion and a medially located caudal portion. Each trunk scale has a single CN with several (1–6, but most commonly 2–3) SNs. Although some tubules branched off the main trunk canal (tubules found on 2–3 scales per fish; see Fig. 2A inset, Fig. 2E), a single straight canal was most common. The caudal fin had 2–3 small SNs in a cluster at the base of the caudal fin and two straight lines of SNs extending down both sides of the caudal fin. A ring of SNs also surrounded each naris.

Fig. 2.

Distribution and morphology of the mechanosensory lateral line system in A. burtoni. (A) The A. burtoni lateral line system is similar to that of other cichlids with seven main cranial canals and a disjunct trunk canal. Canal neuromasts (black ovals) are located inside canals (gray shading) under the skin between pores (open ovals). Superficial neuromasts (black dots) are located around each naris, in rows or clusters in the vicinity of canals, and in two rows along the length of each side of the caudal fin. (B) Canal structures can be seen clearly in cleared and stained fish, including the wide tubules of the infraorbital canal. (C) Higher magnification image of the boxed portion of B showing tubules that extend toward the mouth. The plus sign indicates canal pores just above the mouth and beneath the eye. (D) Ventral view of DASPEI-stained fish to illustrate labeling of both superficial (yellow arrow) and canal neuromasts (white arrow) in the mandibular canal region. (E) Although the trunk canal is typically unbranched, occasional side tubules (arrow) from the trunk canal were found on some scales in some fish. IO, infraorbital; MD, mandibular; N, naris; OT, otic; PR, preopercular; PO, postotic; SO, supraorbital; ST, supratemporal; T, trunk. Scale bars: B, 1 cm; C, 0.5 cm; D, 1 cm; and E, 200 μm.

Fig. 2.

Distribution and morphology of the mechanosensory lateral line system in A. burtoni. (A) The A. burtoni lateral line system is similar to that of other cichlids with seven main cranial canals and a disjunct trunk canal. Canal neuromasts (black ovals) are located inside canals (gray shading) under the skin between pores (open ovals). Superficial neuromasts (black dots) are located around each naris, in rows or clusters in the vicinity of canals, and in two rows along the length of each side of the caudal fin. (B) Canal structures can be seen clearly in cleared and stained fish, including the wide tubules of the infraorbital canal. (C) Higher magnification image of the boxed portion of B showing tubules that extend toward the mouth. The plus sign indicates canal pores just above the mouth and beneath the eye. (D) Ventral view of DASPEI-stained fish to illustrate labeling of both superficial (yellow arrow) and canal neuromasts (white arrow) in the mandibular canal region. (E) Although the trunk canal is typically unbranched, occasional side tubules (arrow) from the trunk canal were found on some scales in some fish. IO, infraorbital; MD, mandibular; N, naris; OT, otic; PR, preopercular; PO, postotic; SO, supraorbital; ST, supratemporal; T, trunk. Scale bars: B, 1 cm; C, 0.5 cm; D, 1 cm; and E, 200 μm.

The distribution and number of neuromasts was similar between males and females (Table 2; Student's t-test, P>0.05 for all comparisons). The only exception was that males had more SNs located on their caudal fins than did females (fish with part of the caudal fin missing excluded; t-test, t=−3.790, d.f.=7, P=0.016). Fry (12 dpf; total length 12.58±0.27 mm) had neuromasts located in typical adult canal locations, but the neuromasts were on the skin and not yet enclosed in canals. The only adult-typical SNs present in the 12 dpf fry were those surrounding the nares.

Table 2.

Quantification of neuromasts in female, male and 12 dpf A. burtoni

Quantification of neuromasts in female, male and 12 dpf A. burtoni
Quantification of neuromasts in female, male and 12 dpf A. burtoni

Role of the mechanosensory lateral line system in territorial interactions

Pre-fight and non-fight behaviors

To test the role of the lateral line system in territorial interactions, we used a behavioral paradigm in which two dominant males had equal opportunity to acquire a new territory (Fig. 1). For a fight to occur, one fish must initiate a fight and the opponent has the option to fight back if he determines that the benefits of winning the territory (e.g. spawning substrate) outweigh the possible costs of losing a fight (e.g. physical damage, reproductive suppression). These conditions resulted in three possible outcomes: no fight occurred within the 30 min trial, one fish became aggressive while the other became subordinate (no-contest win), or a fight occurred. Eleven of the 15 (73.33%) trials between two sham fish and 9 of 14 (64.29%) trials between one sham and one ablated fish, but only 9 of 20 (45%) trials between two ablated fish resulted in a fight within the trial time (Fig. 3A). A no-contest win occurred in 2 of 15 (13.33%) trials between two sham fish, 4 of 13 (30.76%) trials between a sham and an ablated fish but only 1 of 20 (5%) trials between two lateral line-ablated fish. Interestingly, the sham fish won all of the no-contest wins in trials with one sham and one ablated fish. The last category (no fight during trial time) occurred predominantly during trials between two lateral line-ablated fish: 2 of 15 (13.33%) trials between two sham fish, only 1 trial between a sham and an ablated fish (7.69%), but 10 of the 20 (50%) trials between two ablated fish ended with no fight occurring. By assigning a latency time of 30 min for these no-fight trials, trials between two ablated fish had a longer latency to fight onset when compared with trials involving a sham fish (KW, H=7.693, d.f.=2, P=0.021; Dunn's, P<0.05; Fig. 3B).

Fig. 3.

Lack of mechanosensory input impairs fight motivation and opponent assessment in male A. burtoni. (A) When lateral line-ablated fish interacted with other ablated fish, fewer trials resulted in a fight than when sham-handled fish interacted with other sham fish or with ablated fish (45%, 64.29% and 73.33%, respectively). (B) By assigning a latency time of 30 min to trials in which no fight occurred, the latency to initiate a territorial interaction was longer in trials that had two lateral line-ablated fish (ablated–ablated) (ANOVA: P=0.021; no-contest wins excluded from analysis). (C) During trials where sham fish were paired with lateral line-ablated fish, the sham fish initiated the fight 7 out of the 8 times (87.5%), which differs from random chance. (D) In trials that had at least one ablated individual (sham–ablated and ablated–ablated pairings), fish spent significantly more of the non-fight time within 1 body length (BL) of their opponent compared with trials where both fish had intact lateral lines (sham–sham) (Kruskal–Wallis, P<0.001). Data in B and D are plotted as means±s.e.m. and sample sizes are given in parentheses. Different letters indicate significant differences among groups at P≤0.05.

Fig. 3.

Lack of mechanosensory input impairs fight motivation and opponent assessment in male A. burtoni. (A) When lateral line-ablated fish interacted with other ablated fish, fewer trials resulted in a fight than when sham-handled fish interacted with other sham fish or with ablated fish (45%, 64.29% and 73.33%, respectively). (B) By assigning a latency time of 30 min to trials in which no fight occurred, the latency to initiate a territorial interaction was longer in trials that had two lateral line-ablated fish (ablated–ablated) (ANOVA: P=0.021; no-contest wins excluded from analysis). (C) During trials where sham fish were paired with lateral line-ablated fish, the sham fish initiated the fight 7 out of the 8 times (87.5%), which differs from random chance. (D) In trials that had at least one ablated individual (sham–ablated and ablated–ablated pairings), fish spent significantly more of the non-fight time within 1 body length (BL) of their opponent compared with trials where both fish had intact lateral lines (sham–sham) (Kruskal–Wallis, P<0.001). Data in B and D are plotted as means±s.e.m. and sample sizes are given in parentheses. Different letters indicate significant differences among groups at P≤0.05.

Seven of the 8 (87.5%) fights between a sham and ablated fish were initiated by the sham fish (Fig. 3C). The probability of this occurring is 3.125%, suggesting that this was not random chance. Using time spent within 1 BL (the effective range of the lateral line system; Braun and Coombs, 2000) of the opponent as a measure of time assessing the opponent, fish in trials with a lateral line-ablated fish spent significantly more time assessing their opponent than did sham fish in trials between two sham fish (KW, H=13.411, d.f.=2, P<0.001; Dunn's, P<0.05; Fig. 3D). While this measure does not distinguish between the sham and ablated fish in the sham–ablated pairing, it should be noted that the ablated fish was often observed following the sham fish around the tank while the sham fish occasionally performed aggressive behaviors directed at the ablated fish.

Fight behaviors

A ratio of contact to non-contact behaviors was generated by dividing the total number of contact behaviors by the total number of non-contact behaviors. A number >1 signifies a tendency for contact behaviors while <1 signifies use of predominantly non-contact behaviors. Lateral line-ablated fish in trials between two ablated fish had a significantly higher contact to non-contact ratio than fish from trials between two sham fish or a sham and ablated fish (ANOVA, F2,51=32.547, P<0.001; SNK multiple comparison: P<0.001; Fig. 4A). Fish in trials between two sham fish and between one sham and one ablated fish used predominantly non-contact behaviors (0.331±0.049; mean±s.e.m.). In contrast, lateral line-ablated fish in trials between two ablated fish used primarily contact behaviors (1.893±0.403). Fights between two sham fish began with mostly non-contact behaviors and escalated to the use of some contact behaviors over time (Fig. 4B). In trials between two ablated fish, however, lateral line-ablated fish began with contact behaviors and shifted to non-contact throughout the trials (Fig. 4C). Further, only 1 of all 30 sham-handled fish performed an abnormal lateral display (physically touching the opponent as opposed to being within 1 BL but not physically touching), whereas 17 of all 26 lateral line-ablated fish performed abnormal lateral displays (Fig. 4D).

Fig. 4.

Lateral line-ablated fish have altered fight behaviors. (A) Fish in ablated–ablated trials had a higher contact to non-contact behavior ratio (total number of contact behaviors/total number of non-contact behaviors) than did fish in sham–sham and sham–ablated trials (ANOVA, P<0.001). Data are plotted as means±s.e.m., sample size is given in parentheses, and different letters indicate significant differences among groups at P≤0.05. (B,C) Raster plots of contact and non-contact behaviors during the trial fight time show a difference in the sequence of behavioral patterns. Fish in sham–sham trials (B) use primarily non-contact behaviors and escalate to some contact behaviors over time. In contrast, fish in ablated–ablated trials (C) use mostly contact behaviors from the start of the trial. (D) Only 3.34% of sham-handled fish performed abnormal lateral displays (lateral display that occurs with physical contact to the opponent), while 65.38% of ablated fish performed abnormal lateral displays.

Fig. 4.

Lateral line-ablated fish have altered fight behaviors. (A) Fish in ablated–ablated trials had a higher contact to non-contact behavior ratio (total number of contact behaviors/total number of non-contact behaviors) than did fish in sham–sham and sham–ablated trials (ANOVA, P<0.001). Data are plotted as means±s.e.m., sample size is given in parentheses, and different letters indicate significant differences among groups at P≤0.05. (B,C) Raster plots of contact and non-contact behaviors during the trial fight time show a difference in the sequence of behavioral patterns. Fish in sham–sham trials (B) use primarily non-contact behaviors and escalate to some contact behaviors over time. In contrast, fish in ablated–ablated trials (C) use mostly contact behaviors from the start of the trial. (D) Only 3.34% of sham-handled fish performed abnormal lateral displays (lateral display that occurs with physical contact to the opponent), while 65.38% of ablated fish performed abnormal lateral displays.

To test whether aggression levels varied depending on lateral line function, we calculated an aggressive score for each fish (total number of aggressive behaviors divided by the fight duration), and winner and loser scores were combined to have an aggressive score per trial. Aggression levels did not differ based on the presence or absence of lateral line function (KW, H=4.059, d.f.=2, P=0.131; Fig. 5A). Fight duration did not differ between trial pairings (ANOVA, F2,25=1.171, P=0.372; Fig. 5B), and lateral line ablation did not affect the ability of a fish to win a fight (Fig. 5C). Of the 8 fights between one sham and one ablated fish, sham fish won 3 and ablated fish won 5 trials, suggesting that lack of hydrodynamic cues does not affect a fish's ability to win a territorial fight.

Fig. 5.

Ablation of the lateral line system does not alter overall aggressiveness, fight duration or the ability to win a fight. (A) Lateral line ablation did not alter the total number of aggressive behaviors performed per minute (Kruskal–Wallis, P=0.131). (B) Fight duration was not affected by loss of lateral line function (ANOVA, P=0.372). (C) Lack of lateral line function did not impair a fish's ability to win a fight. Sham fish won 3 of 8 trials while ablated fish won the other 5. Data in A and B are plotted as means±s.e.m. and sample sizes are given in parentheses. Same letters indicate no significant differences among groups at P>0.05.

Fig. 5.

Ablation of the lateral line system does not alter overall aggressiveness, fight duration or the ability to win a fight. (A) Lateral line ablation did not alter the total number of aggressive behaviors performed per minute (Kruskal–Wallis, P=0.131). (B) Fight duration was not affected by loss of lateral line function (ANOVA, P=0.372). (C) Lack of lateral line function did not impair a fish's ability to win a fight. Sham fish won 3 of 8 trials while ablated fish won the other 5. Data in A and B are plotted as means±s.e.m. and sample sizes are given in parentheses. Same letters indicate no significant differences among groups at P>0.05.

Sham-handling and control treatments do not affect behavior

To ensure that any observed behavioral differences were due to the lack of hydrodynamic cues associated with ablating the lateral line system and not to handling stress or treatment toxicity, we included a variety of control trials (e.g. cobalt control, EGTA control, anosmic control). No difference in latency to fight or fight duration was observed between any of the control pairings (ANOVA; latency: F4,27=0.308, P=0.870; fight duration: F4,27=0.876, P=0.491). There was also no difference between the number of aggressive behaviors per minute among control treatments (KW: H=5.471, d.f.=4, P=0.242) indicating that treatment (i.e. cobalt, EGTA) and elimination of olfactory cues do not impact overall aggression. In addition, control treatments had no impact on the ratio of contact to non-contact behaviors (ANOVA, F4,59=0.758, P=0.557) or the time spent within 1 BL of the opponent (KW, H=0, d.f.=4, P=1.00), indicating that treatment handling and toxicity did not influence the behavioral preference of fish or their ability to assess opponents. Based on these data, we only compared behavior data from lateral line-ablated fish with those from sham-handled fish in the previous sections.

We sought to determine whether use of the mechanosensory lateral line system was important for territorial interactions in the African cichlid fish, A. burtoni, and whether the morphology of the lateral line system differed between sexes. We mapped the canal and neuromast distribution of the A. burtoni lateral line system and found that the number of neuromasts was consistent across sexes, social status and reproductive state. By chemically and physically ablating the lateral line system in dominant males prior to forced territorial interactions, we also show that hydrodynamic cues are used to assess opponents and to facilitate the use of less dangerous non-contact aggressive behaviors. Our data suggest that the use of the lateral line system for reception of hydrodynamic cues associated with territorial behaviors is a protective mechanism to allow fish the opportunity to assess the opponent without engaging in direct, and potentially damaging, physical contact. This is the first direct experimental evidence in any species that mechanosensory information detected by the lateral line system plays an important role in aggressive social interactions.

Morphology of the mechanosensory lateral line system

Lateral line canal morphology in A. burtoni is consistent with that described for several other cichlid species (Greenwood, 1981). Canal morphology is similar to that of the Lake Malawi cichlids Tramitichromis sp., Labeotropheus fuelleborni and Metriaclima zebra, all of which are classified as having narrow canals (Bird and Webb, 2014; Webb et al., 2014). While A. burtoni are found in shallow shore pools of Lake Tanganyika, genetic techniques have classified them as a riverine species (Brawand et al., 2014), and the narrow canal morphology is consistent with river-dwelling fish. The more turbulent environment of rivers creates hydrodynamic noise, and the narrow canal morphology and small pore diameter help reduce noise to better detect relevant signals (Klein and Bleckmann, 2015).

The distribution of cranial neuromasts in A. burtoni is consistent with all previously examined African cichlids, independent of whether they possess widened or narrow canals (Webb et al., 2014; Bird and Webb, 2014). We did not observe any major neuromast differences between males and females, or between social and reproductive states. Although the overall number of neuromasts did not differ between reproductive states, it is possible that neuromasts could have increased sensitivity during reproductively active states, particularly in females, as hydrodynamic cues are produced during body quivers and tail waggle behaviors performed by courting males. For example, aromatase and estrogen receptors are necessary for neuromast development and continue to be expressed into adulthood (Froehlicher et al., 2009; Hamilton et al., 2014; Tingaud-Sequeira et al., 2004). It is possible, therefore, that sex steroid receptors in neuromasts modulate mechanosensory function similar to that described for the inner ear of fish (Maruska and Fernald, 2010a; Maruska and Sisneros, 2015; Sisneros et al., 2004), but this requires further study.

In our population of A. burtoni, fry are typically released from the female buccal cavity at ∼12 dpf when the lateral line system canals are still developing. Although we found most neuromasts were present, canals were not completely formed at this point. This is consistent with ontogenetic studies of other cichlids in which canal ossification does not occur until 42–56 dpf (Webb et al., 2014), but future studies are needed to fully examine the morphological development of the A. burtoni lateral line system.

The lateral line system is used to assess opponents

Lateral line-ablated fish spent more time within 1 BL of their opponents, suggesting that the lateral line system facilitates assessment of opponents during male–male territorial interactions in A. burtoni. Lateral line-ablated fish were less inclined to start territorial fights and more likely to forfeit a fight. This decreased fight motivation was likely due to an impaired ability to adequately assess the opponent. Models of animal assessment range from pure self-assessment to pure mutual assessment but it is likely that animals use a combination of self and mutual assessment throughout an agonistic interaction (Arnott and Elwood, 2009; Enquist et al., 1990; Elias et al., 2008; Hofmann and Schildberger, 2001; Hsu et al., 2008; Neat et al., 1998). For instance, Hsu et al. (2008) suggested that animals rely on mutual assessment to determine when to escalate a fight, but then switch to self-assessment to determine how long to sustain the fight. The mutual assessment phase is when animals use non-dangerous behaviors and close-range sensory modalities to gauge the potential threat of an opponent. These behaviors can range from circling and sniffing in rodents (Gosling and McKay, 1990) to antenna fencing in insects (Hofmann and Schildberger, 2001). We propose that in A. burtoni, mutual assessment is a multimodal behavioral task dependent on visual and hydrodynamic cues (and potentially chemical and acoustic cues, although not examined here). In typical cichlid fights, the fight is initiated by a non-contact behavior, normally a lateral display. During lateral displays, one fish flares its fins and gently quivers its body while the opponent is oriented perpendicular to him. This orientation and behavior maximizes the production and reception of both visual and hydrodynamic cues. Fish will alternate roles between sender and receiver, until the fight escalates to include contact behaviors (e.g. bites, mouth fights). Fights between two lateral line-ablated fish did not have the initial lateral display circling behaviors observed in typical cichlid fights (based on our experiments and those of Enquist et al., 1990). This, combined with the decreased fight motivation in lateral line-ablated fish, suggests that hydrodynamic cues are important for fish assessment. When input to the lateral line system is eliminated, fish cannot properly assess their opponent and instead are more likely to forfeit a fight. Crickets with removed antennal input have highly reduced male–male aggression, suggesting that both chemosensory and mechanosensory input are critical to their mutual assessment (Murakami and Itoh, 2003). Similarly, hydrodynamic cues appear to be essential for mutual assessment in A. burtoni, and potentially in other territorial fishes.

Decreased fight motivation in A. burtoni is only seen when engaging in a fight to acquire a new territory as opposed to defending an already acquired territory. When used in a resident–intruder paradigm, all lateral line-ablated resident fish immediately defended their territory when exposed to lateral line-intact or ablated intruders (J.M.B., unpublished observations), suggesting that the decreased fight motivation observed in our experiments is unique to acquiring a new territory and likely due to impaired assessment abilities and not decreased aggression. This suggests that although similar behaviors are used in each interaction, fish view these as different behavioral scenarios. In crayfish, these two behavioral scenarios are often linked, with the most dominant crayfish being the quickest to acquire a territory and perform the most evictions (territory defense behavior) (Fero and Moore, 2008; Martin and Moore, 2008). However, it is possible that these two social situations may be perceived and processed differently in the brain, similar to how affective and predatory aggression have different neural pathways in mammals (Gregg and Siegel, 2001). Although the causes of differences in threat perception between these two scenarios are unknown, the territorial defense behaviors towards an intruder used by lateral line-ablated fish indicate that lack of hydrodynamic cues only impacts assessment ability and not overall aggression.

Many fish species use visual cues during social encounters to gain insight into their perceived social standing (Chen and Fernald, 2011; Grosenick et al., 2007; Korzan and Fernald, 2007), but few studies have isolated visual cues without other sensory systems to test the importance of visual cues alone. However, Chen and Fernald (2011) found that visual cues alone were sufficient for male fish to assess others and make the appropriate behavioral decision, but it is important to note that these effects were seen only when one fish was ∼4 times larger than the other. In our experiments, fish were size matched (<10% size difference) and able to use visual cues, yet we still saw noticeable differences in assessment capabilities (e.g. time within 1 BL) when the lateral line system was ablated. This suggests that visual cues alone are not enough to properly assess an opponent when closely size matched, and that fish assessment, like many other cichlid fish behaviors, is dependent on multimodal sensory inputs and not just vision.

Lateral line-ablated fish have altered fight behaviors

Astatotilapia burtoni males use a variety of behaviors, such as lateral displays, frontal threats, mouth fighting and biting, when engaged in a territorial fight. These behaviors always occur within 1–2 BL of the opponent and create water movements that should activate the opponent's lateral line. Although previous studies suggested possible involvement of mechanoreception during agonistic interactions in fishes (Enquist et al., 1990; McMillan and Smith, 1974; Montgomery et al., 2014), our study is the first to directly show that the lateral line system is used in fight behaviors. We classified behaviors as either contact (physical contact occurred) or non-contact (within 1 BL but without physical contact) and showed that lateral line-ablated fish used predominately contact instead of non-contact behaviors. It is possible, therefore, that lateral line system inputs help mediate decisions to perform appropriate agonistic behaviors, such that without the reception of hydrodynamic cues, fish choose to use contact instead of non-contact behaviors. The use of non-contact instead of contact behaviors is important for a couple of reasons. First, these non-contact behaviors act as aggressive displays with less risk of physical damage, and second, they allow fish to be more aware of their surroundings. For example, the South American cichlid Nannacara anomala was slower to detect approaching predators when engaged in contact behaviors compared with non-contact behaviors (Jakobsson et al., 1995), suggesting that non-contact behaviors may act as a self-preservation technique. Without lateral line system input, fish cannot use non-contact behaviors for mutual assessment, causing them to proceed directly to more costly contact behaviors that can be perceived by the somatosensory system.

In A. burtoni, we also saw more abnormal lateral displays (performed with physical contact to the opponent) in lateral line-ablated fish. This could be caused by the ablated fish's inability to perceive water movements and identify an opponent's location, thereby accidently causing contact. It is also possible that the opponent initiates the contact, because without detection of water movements from an approaching fish, the ablated fish may not be able to quickly react and evade the bite. While we cannot distinguish between these different possibilities, future research should examine areas of the brain implicated in social decision making to determine the relative importance of hydrodynamic inputs from the mechanosensory lateral line system in mediating adaptive behavioral decisions during territorial contests.

Although lateral line system ablation in A. burtoni affected the type of behaviors performed (e.g. contact versus non-contact) during territorial interactions, overall aggression, fight duration and fight outcome were not impacted. These data suggest that ablated fish simply replaced the use of non-contact behaviors with contact behaviors, without significant consequences on fight outcome. This supports the idea that once a fight escalates to contact behaviors, fish rely more on self-assessment (Hsu et al., 2008), while fight duration and outcome are more dependent on a fish's perceived standing in the fight and damage incurred throughout the fight. Thus, hydrodynamic cues detected by the lateral line system likely facilitate mutual assessment and the use of non-contact fight behaviors, but have no impact on self-assessment or the ability to win a fight.

Cobalt chloride treatment

To effectively ablate the entire mechanosensory lateral line system in A. burtoni, we used a combination of 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2 treatment and transection of the posterior lateral line nerve (see Materials and methods). Although others have used this cobalt concentration (Medina et al., 2013; Montgomery et al., 1997), it has been scrutinized (Janssen, 2000) for its high dosage and potentially toxic effects. To address this, we included several controls to verify that the treatment was not toxic to A. burtoni. These controls included treatment with 1 mmol l−1 EGTA (used to lower calcium levels in the water) and 2 mmol l−1 CoCl2 in calcium-rich water. Neither of these treatments impaired the lateral line system or altered behavior when compared with control and sham-handled fish. Because cobalt is a non-specific calcium channel antagonist, we also checked for potential effects on other exposed sensory systems. DASPEI staining of the olfactory epithelium was noticeably reduced after treatment (observed via decreased fluorescence, see Fig. 1), suggesting that CoCl2 likely also impairs olfaction, and future studies will evaluate the extent of cobalt effects on olfactory function. However, because of this observation, and the fact that A. burtoni uses chemosensory signaling via urine release in male–male interactions (Maruska and Fernald, 2012), we also examined behavior of anosmic (olfactory epithelium ablated) fish to verify that any behavioral differences we observed in lateral line-ablated fish were not due to impaired olfaction. These trials showed that anosmic fish did not differ behaviorally from control and sham-handled fish, indicating that lack of mechanosensory rather than olfactory input in lateral line-ablated fish was responsible for the observed behavioral differences. We did not ablate the taste or solitary chemosensory cells because it is not feasible, but neither has been implicated in social communication. Thus, we are confident that the observed behavioral differences were due to lack of hydrodynamic inputs and not chemosensory inputs. Fish responded to visual stimuli, indicating that the cobalt treatment did not affect vision, and we have no reason to suspect that cobalt treatment affected hair cells of the inner ear.

Despite the unwanted, but controlled for, effects on the olfactory system, we are also confident that the treatment had no toxicity effects. Fish could be housed in the cobalt solution for several days without any ill-effects, no fish died as a result of the treatment, and all animals used in behavioral trials were alive and behaving normally for months after the treatment. In addition, all treated fish had normal swimming and feeding behaviors. There are many possible reasons why our fish required a higher dose of cobalt than many other species. Aminoglycosides are already known to be highly species specific (Brown et al., 2011), with some species not responding to treatment at all. Cobalt treatment is also likely species specific, and the use of salts in our water to mimic the low salinity environment of Lake Tanganyika potentially added ionic interference. Based on our experience with A. burtoni and the species-specific variations of lateral line system ablation treatment methods in other species (Brown et al., 2011), we propose that researchers using a chemical or pharmacological ablation approach should verify that their treatment is effective for their particular species using a vital dye such as DASPEI, rather than relying on published treatment regimes for other species. Those using cobalt should also test, and then control for, any comorbid effects on chemosensory systems, especially when examining the role of the lateral line system in a multimodal behavior, such as prey detection, predator avoidance or social communication. These controls are crucial for testing the relative role of the lateral line system in any behavioral context.

Summary

Use of the mechanosensory lateral line system during territorial behaviors in fishes has long been suggested (Enquist et al., 1990), but never directly tested. To our knowledge, ours is the first evidence in any fish species that hydrodynamic cues detected by the lateral line system are used to assess opponents during agonistic interactions. In addition, animals lacking lateral line input relied predominantly on contact over non-contact behaviors, suggesting that mechanoreception facilitates the use of non-contact behaviors as a protective mechanism during territorial interactions. Contact behaviors are inherently more dangerous, and the use of non-contact behaviors allows fish to send and receive information, and potentially resolve conflicts, without physical damage. While the lateral line system has largely been studied in terms of its role in feeding behavior, our study demonstrates that mechanosensory information is also used in territorial interactions and should be considered when studying social behaviors involving multimodal sensory input.

We thank Caleb McMahan for clearing and staining the A. burtoni fish used for characterization, Polly Gwan for assistance with treating fish and collecting behavior data, and Hollie Hale-Donze from the Shared Instrumentation Facility at LSU for technical assistance on the stereomicroscope. We also thank the Maruska lab members for discussion and insight and the reviewers for helpful comments on the manuscript.

Author contributions

Both authors had full access to the data and take full responsibility for the integrity of the data analysis and approved the final manuscript. Both authors designed experiments and wrote the manuscript; J.M.B. performed experiments and analyzed data; K.P.M. provided funding, equipment and supplies.

Funding

Funding was provided by startup funds from the College of Science and Department of Biological Sciences at Louisiana State University (K.P.M.), Louisiana Board of Regents RCS Grant (K.P.M.), Powe Faculty Enhancement Award from ORAU (K.P.M.), Sigma Xi (J.M.B.), and a Raney Award from ASIH (J.M.B.). J.M.B. was supported by a Louisiana Board of Regents Graduate Fellowship and National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship.

Almeida
,
O. G.
,
Miranda
,
A.
,
Frade
,
P. C.
,
Hubbard
,
P. C.
,
Barata
,
E. N.
and
Canário
,
A. V. M.
(
2005
).
Urine as a social signal in the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus)
.
Chem. Senses
30
,
i309
-
i310
.
Amorim
,
M. C. P.
,
Fonseca
,
P. J.
and
Almada
,
V. C.
(
2003
).
Sound production during courtship and spawning of Oreochromis mossambicus: male-female and male-male interactions
.
J. Fish Biol.
62
,
658
-
672
.
Amorim
,
M. C. P.
,
Knight
,
M. E.
,
Stratoudakis
,
Y.
and
Turner
,
G. F.
(
2004
).
Differences in sounds made by courting males of three closely related Lake Malawi cichlid species
.
J. Fish Biol.
65
,
1358
-
1371
.
Arnott
,
G.
and
Elwood
,
R. W.
(
2009
).
Assessment of fighting ability in animal contests
.
Anim. Behav.
77
,
991
-
1004
.
Barata
,
E. N.
,
Hubbard
,
P. C.
,
Almeida
,
O. G.
,
Miranda
,
A.
and
Canário
,
A. V. M.
(
2007
).
Male urine signals social rank in the Mozambique tilapia (Oreochromis mossambicus)
.
BMC Biol.
5
,
54
.
Bird
,
N. C.
and
Webb
,
J. F.
(
2014
).
Heterochrony, modularity, and the functional evolution of the mechanosensory lateral line canal system of fishes
.
EvoDevo
5
,
21
.
Braun
,
C. B.
and
Coombs
,
S.
(
2000
).
The overlapping roles of the inner ear and lateral line: The active space of dipole source detection
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B
355
,
1115
-
1119
.
Brawand
,
D.
,
Wagner
,
C. E.
,
Li
,
Y. I.
,
Malinsky
,
M.
,
Keller
,
I.
,
Fan
,
S.
,
Simakov
,
O.
,
Ng
,
A. Y.
,
Lim
,
Z. W.
,
Bezault
,
E.
, et al. 
(
2014
).
The genomic substrate for adaptive radiation in African cichlid fish
.
Nature
513
,
375
-
381
.
Brown
,
A. D.
,
Mussen
,
T.
,
Sisneros
,
J.
and
Coffin
,
A. B.
(
2011
).
Reevaluating the use of aminoglycoside antibiotics in behavioral studies of the lateral line
.
Hear. Res.
272
,
1
-
4
.
Chen
,
C.-C.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2011
).
Visual information alone changes behavior and physiology during social interactions in a cichlid fish (Astatotilapia burtoni)
.
PLoS ONE
6
,
e20313
.
Coombs
,
S.
(
1994
).
Nearfield detection of dipole sources by the goldfish (Carassius auratus) and the mottled sculpin (Cottus baird)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
190
,
109
-
129
.
Coombs
,
S.
and
Patton
,
P.
(
2009
).
Lateral line stimulation patterns and prey orienting behavior in the Lake Michigan mottled sculpin (Cottus bairdi)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
195
,
279
-
297
.
Coombs
,
S.
,
Hastings
,
M.
and
Finneran
,
J.
(
1996
).
Modeling and measuring lateral line excitation patterns to changing dipole source locations
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
178
,
359
-
371
.
Dijkgraaf
,
S.
(
1962
).
The functioning and significance of the lateral-line organs
.
Biol. Rev.
38
,
51
-
105
.
Elias
,
D. O.
,
Kasumovic
,
M. M.
,
Punzalan
,
D.
,
Andrade
,
M. C. B.
and
Mason
,
A. C.
(
2008
).
Assessment during aggressive contests between male jumping spiders
.
Anim. Behav.
76
,
901
-
910
.
Enquist
,
M.
,
Leimar
,
O.
,
Ljungberg
,
T.
,
Mallner
,
Y.
and
Segerdahl
,
N.
(
1990
).
A test of the sequential assessment game: fighting in the cichlid fish Nannacara anomala
.
Anim. Behav.
40
,
1
-
14
.
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
1977
).
Quantitative behavioural observations of Haplochromis burtoni under semi-natural conditions
.
Anim. Behav.
25
,
643
-
653
.
Fernald
,
R. D.
and
Hirata
,
N. R.
(
1977
).
Field study of Haplochromis burtoni: quantitative behavioural observations
.
Anim. Behav.
25
,
964
-
975
.
Fero
,
K.
and
Moore
,
M. A.
(
2008
).
Social spacing of crayfish in natural habitats: what role does dominance play?
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
62
,
1119
-
1125
.
Froehlicher
,
M.
,
Liedtke
,
A.
,
Groh
,
K.
,
López-Schier
,
H.
,
Neuhauss
,
S. C. F.
,
Segner
,
H.
and
Eggen
,
R. I. L.
(
2009
).
Estrogen receptor subtype β2 is involved in neuromast development in zebrafish (Danio rerio) larvae
.
Dev. Biol.
330
,
32
-
43
.
Gosling
,
L. M.
and
McKay
,
H. V.
(
1990
).
Competitor assessment by scent matching: an experimental test
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
26
,
415
-
420
.
Greenwood
,
P. H.
(
1981
).
The Haplochromine Fishes of the East African Lakes
.
Ithaca, NY
:
Cornell Univeristy Press
.
Gregg
,
T. R.
and
Siegel
,
A.
(
2001
).
Brain structures and neurotransmitters regulating aggression in cats: implications for human aggression
.
Prog. Neurophyscopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry
25
,
91
-
140
.
Grosenick
,
L.
,
Clement
,
T. S.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2007
).
Fish can infer social rank by observation alone
.
Nature
445
,
429
-
432
.
Hamilton
,
C. K.
,
Navarro-Martin
,
L.
,
Neufeld
,
M.
,
Basak
,
A.
,
Trudeau
,
V. L.
(
2014
).
Early expression of aromatase and the membrane estrogen receptor GPER in neuromasts reveals a role for estrogens in the development of the frog lateral line system
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
205
,
242
-
250
.
Hofmann
,
H.
and
Schildberger
,
K.
(
2001
).
Assessment of strength and willingness to fight during aggressive encounters in crickets
.
Anim. Behav.
62
,
337
-
348
.
Hofmann
,
H. A.
,
Benson
,
M. E.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
1999
).
Social status regulates growth rate: consequences for life-history strategies
.
Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA
96
,
14171
-
14176
.
Hsu
,
Y.
,
Lee
,
S.-P.
,
Chen
,
M.-H.
,
Yang
,
S.-Y.
and
Cheng
,
K.-C.
(
2008
).
Switching assessment strategy during a contest: fighting in killifish Kryptolebias marmoratus
.
Anim. Behav.
75
,
1641
-
1649
.
Jakobsson
,
S.
,
Brick
,
O.
and
Kullberg
,
C.
(
1995
).
Escalated fighting behaviour incurs increased predation risk
.
Anim. Behav.
49
,
235
-
239
.
Janssen
,
J.
(
2000
).
Toxicity of Co2+: implications for lateral line studies
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A Sens. Neural. Behav. Physiol.
186
,
957
-
960
.
Karlsen
,
H. E.
and
Sand
,
O.
(
1987
).
Selective and reversible blocking of the lateral line in freshwater fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
133
,
249
-
262
.
Keenleyside
,
M. H. A.
and
Yamamoto
,
F. T.
(
1962
).
Territorial behaviour of juvenile Atlantic Salmon (Salmo salar L.)
.
Behaviour
19
,
139
-
168
.
Keller-Costa
,
T. T.
,
Canário
,
A. V. M.
and
Hubbard
,
P. C.
(
2015
).
Chemical communication in cichlids: a mini-review
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
Klein
,
A.
and
Bleckmann
,
H.
(
2015
).
Function of lateral line canal morphology
.
Int. Zool.
10
,
111
-
121
.
Korzan
,
W. J.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2007
).
Territorial male color predicts agonistic behavior of conspecifics in a color polymorphic species
.
Behav. Ecol.
18
,
318
-
323
.
Korzan
,
W. J.
,
Robison
,
R. R.
,
Zhao
,
S.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2008
).
Color change as a potential behavioral strategy
.
Horm. Behav.
54
,
463
-
470
.
Kulpa
,
M.
,
Bak-Coleman
,
J.
and
Coombs
,
S.
(
2015
).
The lateral line is necessary for blind cavefish rheotaxis in non-uniform flow
.
J. Exp. Biol.
218
,
1603
-
1612
.
Lobel
,
P. S.
(
1998
).
Possible species specific courtship sounds by two sympatric cichlid fishes in Lake Malawi, Africa
.
Environ. Biol. Fishes
52
,
443
-
452
.
Marchesan
,
M.
,
Ota
,
D.
and
Ferrero
,
E. A.
(
2000
).
The role of mechanical stimulation during breeding in the grass goby Zosterisessor ophiocephalus (Teleostei, Gobiidae)
.
Ital. J. Zool.
67
,
25
-
30
.
Martin
,
A. L.
and
Moore
,
P. A.
(
2008
).
The influences of dominance on shelter preference and evicition rates in the crayfish, Orconectes rusticus
.
Ethology
114
,
351
-
360
.
Martinovic-Weigelt
,
D.
,
Ekman
,
D.
,
Villeneuve
,
D. L.
,
James
,
C. M.
,
Teng
,
Q.
,
Collette
,
T. W.
and
Ankley
,
G. T.
(
2012
).
Fishy aroma of social status: urinary chemo-signalling of territoriality in male fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas)
.
PLoS ONE
7
,
e46579
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
(
2014
).
Social regulation of reproduction in male cichlid fishes
.
Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.
207
,
2
-
12
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2010a
).
Steroid receptor expression in the fish inner ear varies with sex, social status, and reproductive state
.
BMC Neurosci.
11
,
58
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2010b
).
Behavioral and physiological plasticity: rapid changes during social ascent in an African cichlid fish
.
Horm. Behav.
58
,
230
-
240
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2012
).
Contextual chemosensory urine signaling in an African cichlid fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
68
-
74
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2014
).
Social regulation of gene expression in the African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni
. In
Oxford Handbooks Online
(ed.
T.
Canli
), pp.
52
-
78
.
London
:
Oxford University Press
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
and
Sisneros
,
J. A.
(
2015
).
Sex steroid-dependent modulation of acoustic communication systems in fishes
. In
Sound Communication in Fishes
(ed.
F.
Ladich
), pp.
207
-
233
.
Berlin
:
Springer-Verlag
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
,
Ung
,
U.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2012
).
The African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni uses acoustic communication for reproduction: sound production, hearing, and behavioral significance
.
PLoS ONE
7
,
e37612
.
Maruska
,
K. P.
,
Becker
,
L.
,
Neboori
,
A.
and
Fernald
,
R. D.
(
2013
).
Social descent with territory loss causes rapid behavioral, endocrine and transcriptional changes in the brain
.
J. Exp. Biol.
216
,
3656
-
3666
.
McHenry
,
M. J.
and
Liao
,
J. C.
(
2014
).
The hydrodynamics of flow stimuli
. In
The Lateral Line System
(ed.
S.
Coombs
,
H.
Bleckmann
,
R.
Fay
and
A.
Popper
), pp.
73
-
98
.
New York
:
Springer-Verlag
.
McMillan
,
V. E.
and
Smith
,
R. J. F.
(
1974
).
Agonistic and reproductive behaviour of the fathead munnow (Pimephales promelas Rafinesque)
.
Z. Tierpsychol.
34
,
25
-
38
.
Medina
,
L.
,
Garcia
,
C.
,
Urbina
,
A.
,
Manjarrez
,
J.
and
Moyaho
,
A.
(
2013
).
Female vibration discourages male courtship behaviour in the Amarillo fish (Girardinichthys multiradiatus)
.
Behav. Process.
100
,
163
-
168
.
Mirjany
,
M.
,
Preuss
,
T.
and
Faber
,
D. S.
(
2011
).
Role of the lateral line mechanosensory system in directionality of goldfish auditory evoked escape response
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
3358
-
3367
.
Montgomery
,
J. C.
,
Baker
,
C. J.
and
Carton
,
A. G.
(
1997
).
The lateral line can mediate rheotaxis in fish
.
Nature
389
,
960
-
963
.
Montgomery
,
J.
,
Bleckmann
,
H.
and
Coombs
,
S.
(
2014
).
Sensory ecology and neuroethology of the lateral line
. In
The Lateral Line System
(ed.
S.
Coombs
,
H.
Bleckmann
,
R.
Fay
and
A.
Popper
), pp.
121
-
150
.
New York
:
Springer-Verlag
.
Murakami
,
S.
and
Itoh
,
M. T.
(
2003
).
Removal of both antennae influences the courtship and aggressive behaviors in male crickets
.
J. Neurobiol.
57
,
110
-
118
.
Neat
,
F. C.
,
Huntingford
,
F. A.
and
Beveridge
,
M. M. C.
(
1998
).
Fighting and assessment in male cichlid fish: the effects of asymmetries in gonadal state and body size
.
Anim. Behav.
55
,
883
-
891
.
Pitcher
,
T.
,
Partridge
,
B.
and
Wardle
,
C.
(
1976
).
A blind fish can school
.
Science
194
,
963
-
965
.
Ridgway
,
L. P.
and
Karnofsky
,
D. A.
(
2006
).
The effects of metals on the chick embryo: toxicity and production of abnormalities in development
.
Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.
55
,
203
-
215
.
Rosenthal
,
G. G.
and
Ryan
,
M. J.
(
2000
).
Visual and acoustic communication in non-human animals: a comparison
.
J. Biosci.
25
,
285
-
290
.
Satou
,
M.
,
Takeuchi
,
H.
,
Nishii
,
J.
,
Tanabe
,
M.
,
Kitamura
,
S.
,
Okumoto
,
N.
and
Iwata
,
M.
(
1994
).
Behavioral and electrophysiological evidences that the lateral line is involved in the inter-sexual vibrational communication of the himé salmon (landlocked red salmon, Oncorhynchus nerka)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
174
,
539
-
549
.
Schwalbe
,
M. A. B.
,
Bassett
,
D. K.
and
Webb
,
J. F.
(
2012
).
Feeding in the dark: lateral-line-mediated prey detection in the peacock cichlid Aulonocara stuartgranti
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
2060
-
2071
.
Simões
,
J. M.
,
Duarte
,
I. G.
,
Fonseca
,
P. J.
,
Turner
George F.
and
Clara Amorim
M.
(
2008
).
Courtship and agonistic sounds by the cichlid fish Pseudotropheus zebra
.
J. Acoust. Soc. Am.
124
,
1332
.
Sisneros
,
J. A.
,
Forlano
,
P. M.
,
Deitcher
,
D. L.
and
Bass
,
A. H.
(
2004
).
Steroid-dependent auditory plasticity leads to adaptive coupling of sender and receiver
.
Science
305
,
404
-
407
.
Song
,
J.
,
Yan
,
H. Y.
and
Popper
,
A. N.
(
1995
).
Damage and recovery of hair cells in fish canal (but not superficial) neuromasts after gentamicin exposure
.
Hear. Res.
91
,
63
-
71
.
Stewart
,
W. J.
,
Cardenas
,
G. S.
and
McHenry
,
M. J.
(
2013
).
Zebrafish larvae evade predators by sensing water flow
.
J. Exp. Biol.
216
,
388
-
398
.
Stewart
,
W. J.
,
Nair
,
A.
,
Jiang
,
H.
and
McHenry
,
M. J.
(
2014
).
Prey fish escape by sensing the bow wave of a predator
.
J. Exp. Biol.
217
,
4328
-
4336
.
Taylor
,
W. R.
and
VanDyke
,
G. C.
(
1985
).
Revised procedures for staining and clearing small fishes and other vertebrates for bone and cartilage study
.
Cybium
9
,
107
-
119
.
Tingaud-Sequeira
,
A.
,
André
,
M.
,
Forgue
,
J.
,
Barthe
,
C.
and
Babin
,
P. J.
(
2004
).
Expression patterns of three estrogen receptor genes during zebrafish (Danio rerio) development: evidence for high expression in neuromasts
.
Gene Express. Patterns
4
,
561
-
568
.
Van Netten
,
S. M.
and
McHenry
,
M. J.
(
2014
).
The biophysics of the fish lateral line
. In
The Lateral Line System
(ed.
S.
Coombs
,
H.
Bleckmann
,
R.
Fay
and
A.
Popper
), pp.
99
-
120
.
New York
:
Springer-Verlag
.
Van Trump
,
W. J.
,
Coombs
,
S.
,
Duncan
,
K.
and
McHenry
,
M. J.
(
2010
).
Gentamicin is ototoxic to all hair cells in the fish lateral line system
.
Hear. Res.
261
,
42
-
50
.
Webb
,
J. F.
(
1989
).
Neuromast morphology and lateral line trunk canal ontogeny in two species of cichlids: an SEM study
.
J. Morphol.
202
,
53
-
68
.
Webb
,
J. F.
(
2014
).
Morphological diversity, evolution and development of the mechanosensory lateral line system
. In
The Lateral Line System
(ed.
S.
Coombs
,
H.
Bleckmann
,
R.
Fay
and
A.
Popper
), pp.
17
-
72
.
New York
:
Springer-Verlag
.
Webb
,
J. F.
,
Bird
,
N.
,
Carter
,
L.
and
Dickson
,
J.
(
2014
).
Comparative development and evolution of two lateral line phenotypes in lake malawi cichlids
.
J. Morphol.
275
,
678
-
692
.
Yoshizawa
,
M.
,
Jeffery
,
W. R.
,
van Netten
,
S. M.
and
McHenry
,
M. J.
(
2014
).
The sensitivity of lateral line receptors and their role in the behavior of Mexican blind cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
217
,
886
-
895
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.