Many procellariiforms use olfactory cues to locate food patches over the seemingly featureless ocean surface. In particular, some of them are able to detect and are attracted by dimethylsulphide (DMS), a volatile compound naturally occurring over worldwide oceans in correspondence with productive feeding areas. However, current knowledge is restricted to sub-Antarctic species and to only one study realized under natural conditions at sea. Here, for the first time, we investigated the response to DMS in parallel in two different environments in temperate waters, the Atlantic Ocean and the Mediterranean Sea, employing Cory's (Calonectris borealis) and Scopoli's (Calonectris diomedea) shearwaters as models. To test whether these birds can detect and respond to DMS, we presented them with this substance in a Y-maze. Then, to determine whether they use this molecule in natural conditions, we tested the response to DMS at sea. The number of birds that chose DMS in the Y-maze and that were recruited at DMS-scented slicks at sea suggests that these shearwaters are attracted to DMS in both non-foraging and natural contexts. Our findings show that the use of DMS as a foraging cue may be a strategy adopted by procellariiforms across oceans but that regional differences may exist, giving a worldwide perspective to previous hypotheses concerning the use of DMS as a chemical cue.

The first report of olfactory guidance in procellariiform foraging is more than a century old. In 1882, Collins reported that storm petrels and shearwaters were attracted by cod liver at sea under conditions of dense fog and ‘when not a bird of any kind had been seen for hours’. Subsequent controlled observations and more extensive studies under natural conditions at sea provided support for Collins' and other early field reports, confirming the attraction of procellariiforms to different odours directly linked to food, such as cod liver oil, krill and squid homogenates (Grubb, 1972; Hutchison and Wenzel, 1980; Hutchison et al., 1984; Jouventin and Robin, 1984; Lequette et al., 1989; Nevitt, 1999b; Nevitt et al., 2004; Verheyden and Jouventin, 1994). The use of olfaction to locate food has also been revealed in terrestrial environments by turkey vultures, kiwis, magpies and honeyguides (Buitron and Nuechterlein, 1985; Stager, 1964; Stager, 1967; Wenzel, 1971). More generally, odours are an essential component of navigation over land in homing pigeons, starlings, swifts and catbirds (Fiaschi et al., 1974; Holland et al., 2009; Papi, 1989; Wallraff et al., 1995). Very recently, the ‘olfactory spatial’ hypothesis has been proposed, which states that the primary function of olfaction in animals is navigation. According to this hypothesis, decoding and mapping patterns of odorants in the environment maximizes fitness by allowing animals to acquire resources and avoid competition and predation (Jacobs, 2012).

In 1995, Nevitt and collaborators (Nevitt et al., 1995) revealed that dimethylsulphide (DMS), a volatile compound that occurs naturally in worldwide oceans, is a strong attractant for some Antarctic procellariiforms at sea, making it the perfect candidate olfactory cue for locating foraging grounds and for navigation. DMS is a by-product of the metabolic decomposition of dimethylsulphoniopropionate (DMSP), produced during phytoplankton grazing; its production is often associated with zooplankton feeding and areas with high primary productivity, i.e. high phytoplankton concentration (Cantin et al., 1996; Dacey and Wakeham, 1986; Jean et al., 2009; Simó, 2001). DMSP may be a particularly strong source of chemical signal for zooplankton predators, and in situ observations indicate that schools of small fish aggregate along the periphery of plankton blooming areas (DeBose et al., 2008; DeBose and Nevitt, 2007). Eventually, local elevation in DMS on the sea surface may, opportunistically, alert higher order predators of rapidly accumulating aggregations of zooplankton and zooplankton predators, i.e. fish and squid that are among the main prey for many petrel species (del Hoyo et al., 1992; Hay and Kubanek, 2002; Warham, 1996). Indeed, satellite telemetry revealed an association between areas with high DMS emissions, prey aggregations and foraging grounds of different petrel species in the Antarctic and sub-Antarctic region (reviewed by Nevitt, 2000).

Following the identification of DMS as an attractant and possible chemical cue (Nevitt et al., 1995), the sensitivity to this odorant has been evidenced for a number of petrel and non-petrel species of the Southern oceans through physiological and behavioural tests carried out at the colony (Bonadonna et al., 2006; Cunningham et al., 2008; Nevitt and Bonadonna, 2005b). No other tests, however, were performed under natural conditions at sea, except for one study on African penguins (Wright et al., 2011), nor under different ecological conditions. With the aim of drawing attention to the current state of knowledge, Table 1 reviews all studies performed, to date, testing the response to different odours linked to foraging. It details the odorants tested, the species attracted and not attracted to these cues, the experimental conditions and the location of testing. The table emphasizes that not all species are equally sensitive to all odorants, and species responding to one odorant do not necessarily respond to another. For example, prions (Pachyptila spp.) did not respond to cod liver oil (Lequette et al., 1989) but were attracted by DMS (Nevitt et al., 1995); in contrast, cape petrels (Daption capense) were not attracted by DMS (Nevitt et al., 1995) but responded to krill odours (Nevitt, 1999b) and cod liver oil (Verheyden and Jouventin, 1994). Thus, lifestyle and foraging strategy probably modulate which kind of odorant constitutes a cue and elicits a behavioural response, and which does not (Nevitt and Bonadonna, 2005a). In the light of these variable results, there is no evidence that findings from a unique study on attraction to DMS at sea, though a valuable reference, apply to all petrels. In addition, even though the investigation of olfactory foraging began in the North Atlantic (Grubb, 1972) and North Pacific (Hutchison and Wenzel, 1980; Hutchison et al., 1984), all subsequent studies, and all studies investigating the response to DMS, were carried out in southern oceans (Table 1). The only exception is reported in a meeting abstract in which the response to different odorants was tested in the Bering Sea. Unfortunately, the methods and data of this experiment are not available, but it appears that northern petrel species are indifferent to DMS (Nevitt and Hunt, 1996). This result suggests that the response to DMS might be a local phenomenon. The marine environment is not homogeneous and oceanographic conditions in the two hemispheres are dramatically different as are the concentration and distribution of surface DMS. Antarctic waters, where the response to DMS by procellariiforms has only been tested so far, are the richest both in terms of primary production and DMS emissions. DMS emissions are greater near the poles and decrease by some order of magnitude towards sub-polar and temperate regions. Such reduction is more abrupt in the northern hemisphere than in southern oceans (Belviso et al., 2004; Kettle and Andreae, 2000). Therefore, DMS might be a strong signal only in the southern waters and responses recorded there cannot be directly transposed to other marine environments and feeding assemblages, i.e. temperate waters of the northern oceans where DMS emissions are dramatically lower and the diversity and abundance of procellariiform species is poorer. Moreover, in the northern hemisphere, a number of closed seas and basins are present. Seabirds living and foraging in these basins have to cope with extremely different habitat conditions compared with open oceans, including much lower DMS emissions (Belviso et al., 2003; Simó and Grimalt, 1998; Simó et al., 1997).

Table 1.

Review of all publications dealing with the response to different odours linked to foraging

Review of all publications dealing with the response to different odours linked to foraging
Review of all publications dealing with the response to different odours linked to foraging
Review of all publications dealing with the response to different odours linked to foraging
Review of all publications dealing with the response to different odours linked to foraging

In order to increase knowledge concerning the response to DMS and to provide experimental support for generalizations of DMS-driven foraging behaviour, we investigated the response of Cory's and Scopoli's shearwaters to this compound, in relation to different environmental and ecological settings in the northern hemisphere. These are two closely related medium-sized petrel species that breed in the northern hemisphere waters during summer and migrate south for wintering (Dias et al., 2011; Ristow et al., 2000). Until 2012, they were considered a single species (Sangster et al., 2012), so their employment as model species allows a direct comparison of the response to DMS in different habitats. Cory's shearwater (Calonectris borealis Cory 1881), breeds in north Atlantic islands and migrates to different areas of both hemispheres of the Atlantic Ocean (Dias et al., 2011), while the Scopoli's shearwater (Calonectris diomedea Scopoli, 1769) breeds in the Mediterranean Sea and migrates to the Atlantic Ocean during winter (Brooke, 2004; Ristow et al., 2000). As with all procellariiforms, during breeding they are central place foragers: they must return to the colony either to retrieve a mate or to provision the chick while the foraging grounds remain pelagic (Stephens and Krebs, 1986). This ecological strategy requires high efficiency in locating productive food sources to ensure effective foraging and breeding success. As for the other procellariiforms, olfactory guidance may be advantageous as odour emissions extend the prey patch detectability (Clark and Shah, 1992). Feeding habits are well known for Cory's shearwaters breeding in the Atlantic, where foraging behaviour exhibits great plasticity depending on the characteristics of the foraging grounds (Paiva et al., 2010). In contrast, very little is known about the habits and feeding grounds of Scopoli's shearwaters in the Mediterranean, which are mainly restricted to coastal areas, and foraging trips appear to be shorter, in terms of both duration and distance travelled (Cecere et al., 2013; Dell'Ariccia et al., 2010). These differences and the known foraging plasticity suggest that foraging strategies to locate productive areas could also be different; Mediterranean shearwaters, for example, may not rely on DMS to find food but could employ other cues, possibly taking advantage of coastlines as visual landmarks. The employment of these species as models allows us to address two main points. First, to explore the response to DMS in the northern hemisphere, where emissions are dramatically lower than in previously explored areas, in order to understand whether the attraction to DMS is widespread in oceans worldwide. Second, to directly compare the response by sister species in different marine environments so as to detail how the responses vary in relation to different ecological niches, an approach not previously used.

Two different experiments, at different spatial scales, were designed to determine whether these shearwaters would be attracted by DMS. To test whether the birds are able to detect and respond to DMS, we presented them with a binary choice between DMS and a control odour in a Y-maze at the colony, as other burrow-nesting petrel species significantly prefer the DMS arm in Y-mazes (Bonadonna et al., 2006; Nevitt and Bonadonna, 2005b). Then, to determine whether Cory's and Scopoli's shearwaters actually use this molecule in natural conditions, we also tested their response to DMS at sea.

Y-maze choice test

We tested 52 shearwaters in the Atlantic colony and 29 in the Mediterranean colony, of which 23 (44%) and 16 (55%), respectively, entered one arm successfully, thereby making a choice. In the Atlantic colony, 17 chose the DMS arm whereas six entered the control arm (two-tailed binomial test: 23, P=0.03; Fig. 1). In the Mediterranean colony, 13 chose the DMS arm whereas three preferred the control arm (two-tailed binomial test: 16, P=0.02; Fig. 1). The proportion of birds choosing DMS or control was not different in the two colonies (Fisher's exact test: P=0.7). Choice time (the time that the bird took to walk halfway down the arm) was similar for DMS and control. In the Atlantic colony, the median choice time for DMS was 4.1 min (range 7 s to 10 min) and the median choice time for control was 2.4 min (range 11 s to 6.7 min) (Wilcoxon–Mann–Whitney for independent samples: W=62.5, P=0.4). In the Mediterranean colony, the median choice time for DMS was 2.8 min (range 9 s to 12.8 min) and the median choice time for control was 7.7 min (range 10 s to 11.5 min) (Wilcoxon–Mann–Whitney for independent samples: W=16, P=0.7). Overall, birds showed no lateral preference (Atlantic: DMS, 8 right and 9 left arm; control, 4 right and 2 left arm; Mediterranean: DMS, 6 right and 7 left arm; control, 2 right and 1 left arm; Fisher's exact test: P=0.6 and P=1, respectively). In both colonies, the no-choice birds were mainly inactive after removal of the divider, remaining immobile inside the entry arm throughout the experiment. Body mass did not have an influence on the choice or on the absence of choice; all pairwise comparisons of body mass among birds choosing DMS and control and making no choice in the two colonies were not significant (Wilcoxon–Mann–Whitney for independent samples, P range=0.2–0.9).

Fig. 1.

Preference for dimethylsulphide in a Y-maze. The histogram shows the greater percentage of shearwaters that chose the dimethylsulphide (DMS, 1 μmol l−1) arm in preference to the control arm in the Y-maze in both the Atlantic (Calonectris borealis; binomial test, P=0.03) and Mediterranean (Calonectris diomedea; binomial test, P=0.02) colonies. The proportion of birds choosing DMS or control in the two colonies was not different (Fisher's exact test: P=0.7).

Fig. 1.

Preference for dimethylsulphide in a Y-maze. The histogram shows the greater percentage of shearwaters that chose the dimethylsulphide (DMS, 1 μmol l−1) arm in preference to the control arm in the Y-maze in both the Atlantic (Calonectris borealis; binomial test, P=0.03) and Mediterranean (Calonectris diomedea; binomial test, P=0.02) colonies. The proportion of birds choosing DMS or control in the two colonies was not different (Fisher's exact test: P=0.7).

Table 2.

Number of petrels recruited at DMS-scented and control slicks and wind conditions at the five Atlantic (A1–5) and Mediterranean (M1–5) sea sites

Number of petrels recruited at DMS-scented and control slicks and wind conditions at the five Atlantic (A1–5) and Mediterranean (M1–5) sea sites
Number of petrels recruited at DMS-scented and control slicks and wind conditions at the five Atlantic (A1–5) and Mediterranean (M1–5) sea sites

Attractiveness of DMS at sea

The scores of petrels observed at slicks at sea are summarized in Table 2. The majority of recruited birds were Cory's shearwaters (84.5%) in the Atlantic and Scopoli's shearwaters (84.6%) in the Mediterranean. But other petrel species were also observed: Bulwer's petrel (Bulweria bulwerii) and Pterodroma spp. in the Atlantic; Mediterranean storm petrel (Hydrobates pelagicus melitensis) in the Mediterranean. In both seas, the number of shearwaters flying upwind to DMS slicks was consistently greater than the number flying to control slicks (Fig. 2. G-test: G=31.0, P=2.6×10−8 in the Atlantic; G=11.09, P=0.0009 in the Mediterranean). Over the DMS scented slicks, some shearwater also exhibited an explorative behaviour, making one or two loops before flying away. In the Atlantic Ocean, we counted several Cory's shearwaters flying downwind over both kinds of slicks. In this case, there were no differences between DMS and control slicks (G-test: G=0.79, P=0.38). Moreover, when deploying DMS-scented slicks, Cory's shearwaters approached significantly more frequently moving upwind (Chi-square test: χ2=4.99, P=0.025). In contrast, downwind flights were the most frequent approach to control slicks (Chi-square test: χ2=5.96, P=0.015. Fig. 3). In the Mediterranean Sea, only three birds flew over the slicks downwind, eliminating the possibility of performing the same analysis. No bird landed or pattered on any of the slicks.

Fig. 2.

Number of petrels recruited at DMS-scented and control slicks at sea. (A) Atlantic Ocean (C. borealis, G-test, P=2.6×10−8). (B) Mediterranean Sea (C. diomedea, G-test, P=0.0009).

Fig. 2.

Number of petrels recruited at DMS-scented and control slicks at sea. (A) Atlantic Ocean (C. borealis, G-test, P=2.6×10−8). (B) Mediterranean Sea (C. diomedea, G-test, P=0.0009).

Fig. 3.

Number of Cory's shearwaters that approached the DMS-scented and control slicks downwind and upwind in the Atlantic Ocean. At DMS-scented slicks, Cory's shearwaters approached significantly more frequently moving upwind (Chi-square test: χ21=4.99, P=0.025). In contrast, downwind flights were the most frequent approach to control slicks (Chi-square test: χ21=5.96, P=0.015).

Fig. 3.

Number of Cory's shearwaters that approached the DMS-scented and control slicks downwind and upwind in the Atlantic Ocean. At DMS-scented slicks, Cory's shearwaters approached significantly more frequently moving upwind (Chi-square test: χ21=4.99, P=0.025). In contrast, downwind flights were the most frequent approach to control slicks (Chi-square test: χ21=5.96, P=0.015).

This comprehensive study is the first to simultaneously and specifically explore the response to DMS of closely related species in two different marine environments and in both natural and non-foraging controlled conditions. In addition, this is the first test of attraction to DMS in temperate waters and in the northern hemisphere. Previous studies investigating the attractiveness of DMS either were carried out at the colony, specifically testing the response of only one species (Bonadonna et al., 2006; Nevitt and Bonadonna, 2005b; Nevitt and Haberman, 2003), or tested the attraction of procellariiforms at sea, with no particular target species (Nevitt et al., 1995). All of them were carried out in the procellariiform assemblage in the Antarctic and sub-Antarctic waters, where DMS emissions are particularly high. There, several different species are present that may be in competition for food resources, and bigger and more aggressive species (e.g. albatrosses and giant petrels) may force smaller species (e.g. prions and storm petrels) out of prey patches (Nevitt, 2008; Nevitt and Bonadonna, 2005a). In these petrels, which feed in mixed-species aggregations, specific adaptations may have evolved, potentially preventing conclusions from studies on the olfactory senses of these birds from being transposed directly to other procellariiforms. However, our findings seem to justify such generalization to other marine habitats, including closed basins where coastlines may provide profitable and alternative sources of spatial information. We show here that species of shearwaters that occupy different ecological niches also can detect and are attracted to DMS at concentrations similar to those that they would naturally encounter at sea (Belviso et al., 2003; Simó et al., 1997). The number of birds that chose the DMS in the Y-maze and that were recruited at DMS-scented slicks at sea in our experiments suggests that both Cory's and Scopoli's shearwaters respond to DMS, in both non-foraging and natural contexts. The attraction to DMS by procellariiform seabirds is thus not limited to Antarctic waters.

In both the Atlantic and Mediterranean colonies, under controlled experimental conditions, tested birds significantly preferred the arm of the maze that contained the DMS solution, providing evidence that these shearwaters are able to smell this compound and have a tendency to head towards it. The preference for the DMS arm suggests that they recognize this odour as familiar and so they are motivated to move towards it, probably to find a possible exit out of the maze (Nevitt and Bonadonna, 2005b). Unfortunately, once in the maze, a large number of birds made no choice (56% in the Atlantic and 45% in the Mediterranean colonies). In all previous experiments performed on other petrel species with T- or Y-mazes, no-choice percentages ranged from 5% in Antarctic prions (Pachyptila desolata; mean no-choice in four published experiments was 19%) to 60% in common diving petrels (Pelecanoides urinatrix) probably because of the shy personality of some individuals (reviewed by Bonadonna and Sanz-Aguilar, 2012).

At sea, DMS-scented slicks systematically attracted more birds than control slicks, confirming that the tendency of both Cory's and Scopoli's shearwaters to head for this compound extends to a natural foraging context. Shearwaters flew upwind to the DMS-scented slick and often made one or two loops over it before flying away, suggesting either an interest in the slick or an attempt to scan the slick for prey. In contrast to upwind flights, the number of shearwaters overflying downwind over the slick was the same over DMS and control slicks. This equal number of downwind flights indicates that an equal number of birds was present in the area under the two experimental conditions and that other non-directional stimuli may have attracted birds to the slicks, i.e. visual stimuli provided by the glare of the slick over the water surface or by the boat. Moreover, it is not surprising that, over control slicks, a greater number of birds flew downwind because this is the preferred behaviour by flying petrels, linked to their particular flying strategy (Warham, 1996). The reversal of the distribution of upwind and downwind sightings over DMS and control slicks confirms that airborne stimuli were used for guidance and attraction by shearwaters.

Other petrel species were also sighted at slicks, but never non-procellariiforms. In the Mediterranean Sea, in addition to the Scopoli's shearwater, DMS-scented slicks also attracted the Mediterranean storm petrel. These birds are rarely observed at sea, and almost never during daylight, because of their very low abundance and nocturnal feeding habits (Brooke, 2004; del Hoyo et al., 1992; Warham, 1990). Their presence at DMS-scented slicks suggests that other Mediterranean petrel species may be attracted by this molecule and further studies would be necessary to confirm this. Other species attended the slicks in the Atlantic Ocean as well. In particular, Bulwer's petrels were often observed on both DMS and control slicks, flying very low over the water surface. Even if Bulwer's petrels did not show any preference for DMS or control slicks, their behaviour emphasizes the importance and the necessity of additional investigations.

Our results show that the attraction to DMS is not restricted to southern seabirds or those living exclusively in open oceans. At first glance, it may seem that the overall response in the Northern Hemisphere was much less important than in the southern one, where a previous study reported that several hundred petrels of different species were attracted by both DMS-scented and control slicks (Nevitt et al., 1995). However, these higher numbers were probably due to the greater abundance of petrels in the Southern Ocean compared with the Northern Hemisphere. To compare our results with those of Nevitt et al. (Nevitt et al., 1995), we calculated an index of response to DMS (ior, ranging from 0 to 1), corrected for the relative abundance of birds in the different experimental areas, from our current and previous results: we divided the number of birds attracted to DMS-scented slicks by the total number of birds attracted to both kinds of slick, and we corrected for the duration of slick presentation. This index of the intensity of the response to DMS had the highest value in the Mediterranean Sea (ior=1), where petrels flew over only DMS slicks and never showed up over control slicks, while it was lower in the Southern Ocean (ior=0.7; Atlantic Ocean, ior=0.9). This finding highlights the fact that the smaller number of birds attracted in the Mediterranean and in the North Atlantic compared with the Southern Ocean was not due to a lower attractiveness to DMS but rather to a lower abundance of petrels. However, it would be important to confirm whether the different number of birds attending the slicks at sea in our and previous experiments is driven only by bird density or whether other factors also have a role. For example, higher wind speeds, as often observed in the Southern Ocean, increase the distance over which the DMS can be dispersed, thus increasing the detectability of the slicks. In addition, high winds and large swells have been shown to enhance the wandering behaviour of petrels at sea and, thus, their presence at slicks (Hutchison and Wenzel, 1980). A similar phenomenon was also observed in our study and probably accounts for the different number of birds in the Mediterranean and Atlantic. In fact, we observed that when the Atlantic conditions at sea were similar to those in the Mediterranean, with no or low wind speed and no waves, the bird count was similarly low and restricted to DMS-scented slicks. Finally, recent research on carbon and nitrogen stable isotope signature in feathers during the summer suggests that Scopoli's shearwater may include krill in their diet during the breeding season (Peron and Gremillet, 2014), implying that DMS might be a direct foraging cue in the Mediterranean rather than an indirect cue of foraging aggregations. This hypothesis deserves further investigation.

Our finding that petrel species foraging in closed basins may use olfactory cues to locate productive areas at sea opens new interesting perspectives. It would be of great interest to investigate whether visual cues are also employed to locate foraging spots in littoral and shelf waters, how petrels integrate the information coming from different kinds of cues and how they modulate their response to olfactory and visual cues according to circumstances (i.e. during the breeding season in the Mediterranean and during migration in the open Atlantic Ocean).

In addition to its role as foraging cue, DMS has also been proposed in numerous reviews as a chemosignal for navigation in open waters (Nevitt, 1999a; Nevitt, 2000; Nevitt, 2008; Nevitt, 2011; Nevitt and Bonadonna, 2005a; Nevitt and Bonadonna, 2005b). However, this idea lacks empirical validation and the potential role of DMS in seabird navigation beyond foraging remains unknown. Recently, it has been experimentally shown that Cory's shearwaters need olfaction to navigate over long distances (Gagliardo et al., 2013), but the chemical cues used in this navigation process have not yet been elucidated. Experiments that directly test the use of DMS as a cue for long distance navigation are therefore crucial.

In conclusion, our data provide new essential elements to understand the role of DMS in environments other than sub-Antarctic waters. We show that Cory's and Scopoli's shearwaters are sensitive and attracted to DMS, indicating that the sensitivity and attraction to DMS are actually widespread among petrel species and different marine environments, including temperate waters. Our study opens a worldwide perspective to previous hypotheses concerning the use of DMS as a cue for foraging, providing an experimental basis to theoretical work.

The study was carried out on shearwaters breeding in two different colonies. The Mediterranean Scopoli's shearwater colony was on Linosa island (Sicilian Channel, Italy: 35°52′N, 12°52′E), where ~10,000 pairs breed (Massa and Lo Valvo, 1986). In the Atlantic Ocean, we selected the Cory's shearwater colony on Selvagem Grande (Macaronesia, Portugal: 30°09′N, 15°52′W), where the breeding population is estimated to be 30,000 pairs (Granadeiro et al., 2006). In both colonies, we repeated the same protocols during incubation in June–July 2011 and 2012 in the Mediterranean and Atlantic, respectively.

This study was authorized by the Regione Siciliana, Assessorato Agricoltura e Foreste, Prot. 17233 dated 01/12/10, and by the Serviço do Parque Natural da Madeira, licence number 5/2011.

Y-maze choice test

The Y-maze was similar to the maze used in previous experiments (Bonadonna et al., 2006; Bonadonna and Nevitt, 2004; Nevitt and Bonadonna, 2005b). In Linosa, it was made of opaque PVC wire housing (three symmetrical arms: 100×23×19 cm L×W×H, angled at 120 deg), while in Selvagem Grande we were constrained for logistic reasons to use a smaller one made of stainless steel (three symmetrical arms: 65×17×17 cm L×W×H, angled at 120 deg). In both cases one arm, used as the starting point, was fitted with two trapdoors that formed a temporary holding compartment for the bird. Because Mediterranean shearwaters are nocturnal at the colony, and nest in dark burrows, the PVC maze was covered with a thick blanket to darken the goal arms and thus increase the motivation of birds to leave the clear starting point and move towards one of the dark arms. The end of each goal arm was equipped with a CPU cooling fan (DC Pico Ace 25, Sanyo Denki Co. Ltd, Tokyo, Japan) mounted on a partition to provide a low-noise controlled airflow (13 CFM). In the compartment behind the fan, a Petri dish (5.5 cm diameter) containing either DMS or control solution provided the stimulus. DMS solution was prepared in propylene glycol (4 ml; 1 μmol l−1); the control solution contained propylene glycol only (4 ml) (Bonadonna et al., 2006). To eliminate any physical or positional bias, odour stimuli were alternated between arms at each trial and frequently exchanged (each 1–3 trials) with fresh solutions. In addition, the maze was washed with ethanol (70%) to remove any odour residue after each trial.

All experiments were performed during daylight, when there were no free-flying birds at the colony. For each experimental trial, one shearwater at a time was captured at the nest, transported in a cotton bag to the maze and then placed in the temporary holding compartment for a 3 min acclimation period. The inner trap door was then lifted for the bird to make a choice. Birds tended to stay in the intersection prior to making a choice, and could be heard sweeping their heads back and forth, presumably sampling each arm. The sounds of the bird walking in the maze allowed us to easily assess arm choice without disturbing the bird. A positive choice was scored if the bird travelled at least halfway down an arm and stopped for at least 30 s. Almost all birds stopped at the end of the arm and remained there. No-choice birds tended to sit quietly in the entryway, some facing away from the maze arms, and were removed from the maze after 15 min. Choice time was calculated as the time that a bird took to walk halfway down each maze arm. After the Y-maze test, birds were immediately returned to the nest burrow, where they promptly resumed warming the egg in a normal behaviour. Each bird was tested only once and was away from its nest for a maximum of 30 min. We noted no deleterious effects on breeding success.

Open sea test

To test the responsiveness of Cory's and Scopoli's shearwaters to DMS in natural foraging conditions, i.e. in the open sea, we compared the number of birds attracted by DMS-scented and non-scented vegetable oil slicks deployed on the water surface (Nevitt et al., 1995; Wright et al., 2011). Slicks were released upwind from a small boat at five different locations around Linosa island (mean ± s.e.m. distance from coast: 7.5±0.7 km; maximum 10.8 km; minimum 4.3 km) and five around Madeira island (8.7±0.5 km; maximum 9.9 km; minimum 7.5 km). At each location, DMS-scented slicks (0.2 mol l−1 DMS concentration in 2 l of vegetable oil) were coupled with non-scented slicks (consisting of 2 l of vegetable oil only) to control for any visual attraction that the slick could present to foraging birds. The DMS-scented and control slicks were presented consecutively, in a random order, separated by a 45–60 min interval (after complete dissipation; see below) and by 1 km distance to ensure roughly similar experimental conditions within slick pairs but with no cross-contamination. Slicks were deployed only when no birds were in sight in any direction. Slicks drifted away from the release point during trials as a result of marine currents and wind (0–6 knots around Linosa; 0–18 knots around Madeira) and dissipated within 20–30 min. One person with binoculars made observations and recorded data starting from 2 min before the deployment of the oil by a second person. Birds were counted if they (1) flew upwind (against the current) over the slick within ~1 m of the surface (continuous sampling) and (2) landed or (3) pattered on the slick (instantaneous sampling at 1 min intervals). We also separately counted the birds that flew downwind over the slick (continuous sampling).

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using R (R Development Core Team, 2011). Y-maze preferences were analysed using binomial tests (Zar, 1996). We then used the Wilcoxon–Mann–Whitney test for independent samples to check for differences in choice time and body mass of birds expressing different preferences in the maze, and the Fisher exact test to check for lateral choice and to compare Atlantic and Mediterranean choices. We compared the ratios of birds overflying DMS and control slicks at sea with the G-test for pooled data (McDonald, 2009), with an expected ratio of 1:1 in the case of no attraction by DMS-scented slicks, as in previous studies (Nevitt, 1994; Nevitt, 1999b; Nevitt et al., 2004; Nevitt et al., 1995). Finally, we compared the proportion of birds flying over the slicks upwind and downwind using the Chi-square test (Hutchison et al., 1984).

We are grateful to Nicolas Gaidet for the ornithological and scientific support during both sea experiments. This study was possible thanks to David Degueldre and Thierry Mathieu who built the two Y-mazes. We would like to thank the Terraferma diving centre (www.terrafermadiving.it) for renting out the boat in the Mediterranean and to Quintas das Eiras, veleiro ‘Il Quadrifoglio’ (www.wonderfulland.com/quadrifoglio) around Madeira and, in particular, to Claudia Rossetti, Giovanni Pesaresi and Luis Mendes Gomes for the fundamental help during sea experiments. We would like also to thank Salvatore Bonadonna in Linosa and Paulo Catry, José Pedro Granadeiro and the warden at the Nature Reserve in Selvagem Grande for the logistic support during fieldwork. Mary C. Olmstead kindly provided English proofreading prior to submission of the manuscript. Finally, we would like to thank Lorien Pichegru and other referees for their helpful comments and suggestions to improve previous versions of this manuscript.

Funding

G.D.A. was funded by the Fyssen Foundation (Fellowship 2010) and by a Marie Curie Intra-European Fellowship [PIEF-GA-2010-272282-SOMA]. The Mediterranean fieldwork of this research was possible thanks to a collaborative project between the CEFE-CNRS and the University of Palermo financed by a Journal of Experimental Biology Travelling Fellowship awarded to G.D.A. (Award 2010). M.G. was supported by the Agence Nationale de la Recherche Française [AMBO ANR-08-BLAN-0117-01].

Belviso
S.
,
Sciandra
A.
,
Copin-Montégut
C.
(
2003
).
Mesoscale features of surface water DMSP and DMS concentrations in the Atlantic Ocean off Morocco and in the Mediterranean Sea
.
Deep Sea Res. Part I Oceanogr. Res. Pap.
50
,
543
-
555
.
Belviso
S.
,
Moulin
C.
,
Bopp
L.
,
Stefels
J.
(
2004
).
Assessment of a global climatology of oceanic dimethylsulfide (DMS) concentrations based on SeaWiFS imagery (1998-2001)
.
Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci.
61
,
804
-
816
.
Bonadonna
F.
,
Nevitt
G. A.
(
2004
).
Partner-specific odor recognition in an Antarctic seabird
.
Science
306
,
835
.
Bonadonna
F.
,
Sanz-Aguilar
A.
(
2012
).
Kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance in wild birds: the first evidence for individual kin-related odour recognition
.
Anim. Behav.
84
,
509
-
513
.
Bonadonna
F.
,
Caro
S.
,
Jouventin
P.
,
Nevitt
G. A.
(
2006
).
Evidence that blue petrel, Halobaena caerulea, fledglings can detect and orient to dimethyl sulfide
.
J. Exp. Biol.
209
,
2165
-
2169
.
Brooke
M.
(
2004
).
Albatrosses and Petrels Across the World
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.
Buitron
D.
,
Nuechterlein
G. L.
(
1985
).
Experiments on olfactory detection of food caches by black-billed magpies
.
Condor
87
,
92
-
95
.
Cantin
G.
,
Levasseur
M.
,
Gosselin
M.
,
Michaud
S.
(
1996
).
Role of zooplankton in the mesoscale distribution of surface dimethylsulfide concentrations in the Gulf of St. Lawrence, Canada
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
141
,
103
-
117
.
Cecere
J. G.
,
Catoni
C.
,
Maggini
I.
,
Imperio
S.
,
Gaibani
G.
(
2013
).
Movement patterns and habitat use during incubation and chick-rearing of Cory Shearwater (Calonectris diomedea) from Central Mediterranean: influence of seascape and breeding stage
.
Italian Journal of Zoology
80
,
82
-
89
.
Clark
L.
,
Shah
P. S.
(
1992
).
Information content of prey odour plumes: what do foraging Leach's storm petrels know?
In
Chemical Signals in Vertebrates VI
(ed.
Doty
R. L.
,
Müller-Schwarze
D.
), pp.
421
-
427
.
New York, NY
:
Plenum Press
.
Cunningham
G. B.
,
Nevitt
G. A.
(
2005
).
The sense of smell in procellariiforms – an overview and new directions
. In
Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 10
(ed.
Mason
R. T.
,
LeMaster
M. P.
,
Müller-Schwarze
D.
), pp.
403
-
408
.
New York, NY
:
Springer
.
Cunningham
G. B.
,
Van Buskirk
R. W.
,
Bonadonna
F.
,
Weimerskirch
H.
,
Nevitt
G. A.
(
2003
).
A comparison of the olfactory abilities of three species of procellariiform chicks
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
1615
-
1620
.
Cunningham
G. B.
,
Van Buskirk
R. W.
,
Hodges
M. J.
,
Weimerskirch
H.
,
Nevitt
G. A.
(
2006
).
Behavioural responses of blue petrel chicks (Halobaena caerulea) to food-related and novel odours in a simple wind tunnel
.
Antarct. Sci.
18
,
345
-
352
.
Cunningham
G. B.
,
Strauss
V.
,
Ryan
P. G.
(
2008
).
African penguins (Spheniscus demersus) can detect dimethyl sulphide, a prey-related odour
.
J. Exp. Biol.
211
,
3123
-
3127
.
Dacey
J. W. H.
,
Wakeham
S. G.
(
1986
).
Oceanic dimethylsulfide: production during zooplankton grazing on phytoplankton
.
Science
233
,
1314
-
1316
.
DeBose
J. L.
,
Nevitt
G.
(
2007
).
Investigating the association between pelagic fish and dimethylsulfoniopropionate in a natural coral reef system
.
Mar. Freshw. Res.
58
,
720
-
724
.
DeBose
J. L.
,
Lema
S. C.
,
Nevitt
G. A.
(
2008
).
Dimethylsulfoniopropionate as a foraging cue for reef fishes
.
Science
319
,
1356
.
del Hoyo
J.
,
Elliott
A.
,
Sargatal
J.
(
1992
).
Handbook of the Birds of the World
.
Barcelona
:
Lynx Edicions
.
Dell'Ariccia
G.
,
Dell'Omo
G.
,
Massa
B.
,
Bonadonna
F.
(
2010
).
First GPS-tracking of Cory's shearwater in the Mediterranean Sea
.
Italian Journal of Zoology
77
,
339
-
346
.
Dias
M. P.
,
Granadeiro
J. P.
,
Phillips
R. A.
,
Alonso
H.
,
Catry
P.
(
2011
).
Breaking the routine: individual Cory's shearwaters shift winter destinations between hemispheres and across ocean basins
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
278
,
1786
-
1793
.
Fiaschi
V.
,
Farina
M.
,
Ioalé
P.
(
1974
).
Homing experiments on swifts Apus apus (L.) deprived of olfactory perception
.
Monitore Zoologico Italiano
8
,
235
-
244
.
Gagliardo
A.
,
Bried
J.
,
Lambardi
P.
,
Luschi
P.
,
Wikelski
M.
,
Bonadonna
F.
(
2013
).
Oceanic navigation in Cory's shearwaters: evidence for a crucial role of olfactory cues for homing after displacement
.
J. Exp. Biol.
216
,
2798
-
2805
.
Granadeiro
J. P.
,
Dias
M. P.
,
Rebelo
R.
,
Santos
C. D.
,
Catry
P.
(
2006
).
Numbers and population trends of Cory's shearwater Calonectris diomedea at Selvagem Grande, Northeast Atlantic
.
Waterbirds
29
,
56
-
60
.
Grubb
T. C.
(
1972
).
Smell and foraging in shearwaters and petrels
.
Nature
237
,
404
-
405
.
Hay
M. E.
,
Kubanek
J.
(
2002
).
Community and ecosystem level consequences of chemical cues in the plankton
.
J. Chem. Ecol.
28
,
2001
-
2016
.
Holland
R. A.
,
Thorup
K.
,
Gagliardo
A.
,
Bisson
I. A.
,
Knecht
E.
,
Mizrahi
D.
,
Wikelski
M.
(
2009
).
Testing the role of sensory systems in the migratory heading of a songbird
.
J. Exp. Biol.
212
,
4065
-
4071
.
Hutchison
L. V.
,
Wenzel
B. M.
(
1980
).
Olfactory guidance in foraging by procellariiforms
.
Condor
82
,
314
-
319
.
Hutchison
L. V.
,
Wenzel
B. M.
,
Stager
K. E.
,
Tedford
B. L.
(
1984
).
Further evidence for olfactory foraging by sooty shearwaters and northern fulmars
. In
Marine Birds: Their Feeding Ecology and Commercial Fisheries Relationships
(ed.
Nettleship
D. N.
,
Sanger
G. A.
,
Springer
P. F.
), pp.
72
-
77
.
Ottawa, ON
:
Canadian Wildlife Service
.
Jacobs
L. F.
(
2012
).
From chemotaxis to the cognitive map: the function of olfaction
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
109
Suppl. 1
,
10693
-
10700
.
Jean
N.
,
Bogé
G.
,
Jamet
J.-L.
,
Jamet
D.
,
Richard
S.
(
2009
).
Plankton origin of particulate dimethylsulfoniopropionate in a Mediterranean oligotrophic coastal and shallow ecosystem
.
Estuar. Coast. Shelf Sci.
81
,
470
-
480
.
Jouventin
P.
(
1977
).
Olfaction in Snow Petrels
.
Condor
79
,
498
-
499
.
Jouventin
P.
,
Robin
J. P.
(
1984
).
Olfactory experiments on some Antarctic birds
.
Emu
84
,
46
-
48
.
Kettle
A. J.
,
Andreae
M. O.
(
2000
).
Flux of dimethylsulfide from the oceans: a comparison of updated data sets and flux models
.
J. Geophys. Res.
105
,
26793
-
26808
.
Lequette
B.
,
Verheyden
C.
,
Jouventin
P.
(
1989
).
Olfaction in suantarctic seabirds:its phylogenetic and ecological significance
.
Condor
91
,
732
-
735
.
Massa
B.
,
Lo Valvo
M.
(
1986
).
Biometrical and biological considerations on the Cory's shearwater Calonectris diomedea
. In
Mediterranean Marine Avifauna
(ed.
Monbaillin
M. X.
), pp.
293
-
313
.
Berlin; Heidelberg
:
Springer Verlag
.
McDonald
J. H.
(
2009
).
Handbook of Biological Statistics
, 2nd edn.
Baltimore, MD
:
Sparky House Publishing
.
Nevitt
G. A.
(
1994
).
Antarctic procellariiform seabirds can smell krill
.
Antarct. J. US
29
,
168
-
169
.
Nevitt
G. A.
(
1999a
).
Foraging by seabirds on an olfactory landscape
.
Am. Sci.
87
,
46
-
53
.
Nevitt
G. A.
(
1999b
).
Olfactory foraging in Antarctic seabirds: a species-specific attraction to krill odors
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
177
,
235
-
241
.
Nevitt
G. A.
(
2000
).
Olfactory foraging by Antarctic procellariiform seabirds: life at high Reynolds numbers
.
Biol. Bull.
198
,
245
-
253
.
Nevitt
G. A.
(
2008
).
Sensory ecology on the high seas: the odor world of the procellariiform seabirds
.
J. Exp. Biol.
211
,
1706
-
1713
.
Nevitt
G. A.
(
2011
).
The neuroecology of dimethyl sulfide: a global-climate regulator turned marine infochemical
.
Integr. Comp. Biol.
51
,
819
-
825
.
Nevitt
G. A.
,
Bonadonna
F.
(
2005a
).
Seeing the world through the nose of a bird: new developments in the sensory ecology of procellariiform seabirds
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
287
,
292
-
295
.
Nevitt
G. A.
,
Bonadonna
F.
(
2005b
).
Sensitivity to dimethyl sulphide suggests a mechanism for olfactory navigation by seabirds
.
Biol. Lett.
1
,
303
-
305
.
Nevitt
G. A.
,
Haberman
K.
(
2003
).
Behavioral attraction of Leach's storm-petrels (Oceanodroma leucorhoa) to dimethyl sulfide
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
1497
-
1501
.
Nevitt
G. A.
,
Hunt
G. L.
(
1996
).
Olfactory sensitivities of foraging procellariid seabirds in the Aleutian Islands
.
Chem. Senses
21
,
649
-
650
.
Nevitt
G. A.
,
Veit
R. R.
(
1999
).
Mechanisms of prey-patch detection by foraging seabirds
. In
Proceedings of the 22nd International Ornithological Congress
(ed. by
Adams
N. J.
,
Slotow
R. H.
), pp.
2072
-
2082
.
Durban; Johannesburg
:
BirdLife South Africa
.
Nevitt
G. A.
,
Veit
R. R.
,
Kareiva
P.
(
1995
).
Dimethyl sulphide as a foraging cue for Antarctic Procellariiform seabirds
.
Nature
376
,
680
-
682
.
Nevitt
G.
,
Reid
K.
,
Trathan
P.
(
2004
).
Testing olfactory foraging strategies in an Antarctic seabird assemblage
.
J. Exp. Biol.
207
,
3537
-
3544
.
Nevitt
G. A.
,
Bergstrom
D. M.
,
Bonadonna
F.
(
2006
).
The potential role of ammonia as a signal molecule for procellariiform seabirds
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
315
,
271
-
277
.
Paiva
V. H.
,
Geraldes
P.
,
Ramírez
I.
,
Meirinho
A.
,
Garthe
S.
,
Ramos
J. A.
(
2010
).
Foraging plasticity in a pelagic seabird species along a marine productivity gradient
.
Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser.
398
,
259
-
274
.
Papi
F.
(
1989
).
Pigeons use olfactory cues to navigate
.
Ethology Ecology & Evolution
1
,
219
-
231
.
Peron
C.
,
Gremillet
D.
(
2014
).
Habitats Maritimes des Puffins de France Métropolitaine: Approche par Balises et Analyses Isotopiques: Final Report
.
Brest, France
:
Agence des Aires Marines Protégées
R Development Core Team
(
2011
).
R: A Language And Environment For Statistical Computing.
Vienna, Austria
:
R Foundation for Statistical Computing
.
Ristow
D.
,
Berthold
P.
,
Hashmi
D.
,
Querner
U.
(
2000
).
Satellite tracking of Cory's shearwater migration
.
Condor
102
,
696
-
699
.
Roper
T. J.
(
1999
).
Olfaction in birds
.
Adv. Study Behav.
28
,
247
-
332
.
Sangster
G.
,
Collinson
J. M.
,
Crochet
P.-A.
,
Knox
A. G.
,
Parkin
D. T.
,
Votier
S. C.
(
2012
).
Taxonomic recommendations for British birds: eighth report
.
Ibis
154
,
874
-
883
.
Simó
R.
(
2001
).
Production of atmospheric sulfur by oceanic plankton: biogeochemical, ecological and evolutionary links
.
Trends Ecol. Evol.
16
,
287
-
294
.
Simó
R.
,
Grimalt
J. O.
(
1998
).
Spring-summer emissions of dimethyl sulphide from the north-western Mediterranean Sea
.
Estuar. Coast. Shelf Sci.
47
,
671
-
677
.
Simó
R.
,
Grimalt
J. O.
,
Albaigés
J.
(
1997
).
Dissolved dimethylsulphide, dimethylsulphoniopropionate and dimethylsulphoxide in western Mediterranean waters
.
Deep Sea Res. Part II Top Stud. Oceanogr.
44
,
929
-
950
.
Stager
K. E.
(
1964
).
The Role of Olfaction in Food Location by Turkey Vulture (Cathartes aura)
.
Los Angeles, CA
:
Los Angeles County Museum Contributions in Science
.
Stager
K. E.
(
1967
).
Avian olfaction
.
Am. Zool.
7
,
415
-
420
.
Stephens
D. W.
,
Krebs
J. R.
(
1986
).
Monographs in Behavior and Ecology: Foraging Theory
.
Princeton, NJ
:
Princeton University Press
.
Verheyden
C.
,
Jouventin
P.
(
1994
).
Olfactory behavior of foraging procellariiforms
.
Auk
111
,
285
-
291
.
Wallraff
H. G.
,
Kiepenheuer
J.
,
Neumann
M. F.
,
Streng
A.
(
1995
).
Homing experiments with starlings deprived of the sense of smell
.
Condor
97
,
20
-
26
.
Warham
J.
(
1990
).
The Petrels Their Ecology and Breeding System
.
London
:
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich
.
Warham
J.
(
1996
).
The Behaviour, Population Biology and Physiology of The Petrels
.
London
:
Harcourt Brace & Company
.
Wenzel
B. M.
(
1971
).
Olfactory sensation in the kiwi and other birds
.
Ann. New York Acad. Sci.
188
,
183
-
193
.
Wenzel
B. M.
(
1986
).
The ecological and evolutionary challenges of procellariiform olfaction
. In
Chemical Signals in Vertebrates
(ed.
Duvall
D.
,
Müller-Schwarze
D.
,
Silverstein
R. M.
), pp.
357
-
368
.
New York, NY
:
Plenum Press
.
Wright
K. L. B.
,
Pichegru
L.
,
Ryan
P. G.
(
2011
).
Penguins are attracted to dimethyl sulphide at sea
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
2509
-
2511
.
Zar
J. H.
(
1996
).
Biostatistical Analysis
.
Upper Saddle River, NJ
:
Prentice Hall
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.