SUMMARY
Life-history theory predicts that animals face a trade-off in energy allocation between performing strenuous exercise, such as migratory flight, and mounting an immune response. We experimentally tested this prediction by studying immune function in European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris, flown in a wind tunnel. Specifically, we predicted that constitutive immune function decreases in response to training and, additionally, in response to immediate exercise. We compared constitutive immune function among three groups: (1) ‘untrained’ birds that were kept in cages and were not flown; (2) ‘trained’ birds that received flight training over a 15 day period and performed a 1-4 h continuous flight, after which they rested for 48 h before being sampled; and (3) ‘post-flight’ birds that differed from the ‘trained’ group only in being sampled immediately after the final flight. A bird in our trained group represents an individual during migration that has been resting between migratory flights for at least 2 days. A bird in our post-flight group represents an individual that has just completed a migratory flight and has not yet had time to recover. Three of our four indicators (haptoglobin, agglutination and lysis) showed the predicted decrease in immune function in the post-flight group, and two indicators (haptoglobin, agglutination) showed the predicted decreasing trend from the untrained to trained to post-flight group. Haptoglobin levels were negatively correlated with flight duration. No effect of training or flight was detected on leukocyte profiles. Our results suggest that in European starlings, constitutive immune function is decreased more as a result of immediate exercise than of exercise training. Because of the recent emergence of avian-borne diseases, understanding the trade-offs and challenges faced by long-distance migrants has gained a new level of relevance and urgency.
INTRODUCTION
The immune system is crucial for an animal's survival, but it is costly to maintain and use (Klasing, 2004; Lochmiller and Deerenberg, 2000). Immune function is increasingly understood as a life-history parameter, and as such it is subject to energy and nutrient trade-offs (Ardia and Schat, 2008; Buehler et al., 2008b; Lee, 2006; Norris and Evans, 2000; Sheldon and Verhulst, 1996; Tella et al., 2002). In situations of limited energy availability, animals must maximise fitness by differential allocation of resources to satisfy competing energetic demands (King, 1974; Willmer et al., 2000). Trade-offs between immune function and other life-history parameters have been documented across taxa, including insects (Kraaijeveld et al., 2001; Rolff and Siva-Jothy, 2003), reptiles (López and Martín, 2005), birds (Norris and Evans, 2000) and mammals (Graham et al., 2010).
The trade-off between the immune response and strenuous exercise is particularly well documented in human athletes. Intensive training suppresses the immune system (Gleeson, 2007; Gleeson et al., 1995; Hoffmann et al., 2010; Pedersen and Hoffmann-Goetz, 2000; Wolach et al., 1998), while strenuous exercise, or in fact any physical activity, is harder to perform when the immune system is being challenged (Apanius, 1998; Nieman, 1997). Flight is energetically much more demanding than running, as the minimum energy cost of flying is thought to be about twice the maximal oxygen consumption () compared with a running mammal of similar body size (Butler and Woakes, 1990). It remains comparatively little understood, however, whether migratory flight suppresses immune function or whether immune-challenged birds have a lowered flight performance (Ardia and Schat, 2008; Buehler and Piersma, 2008; Buehler et al., 2010a). It is, however, particularly relevant in the context of the transmission of avian-borne diseases. Migratory birds are often blamed for the spread of avian influenza, but little is known about whether birds infected with highly pathogenic avian influenza can actually perform a long-distance migration (Altizer et al., 2011). Natural infection with low-pathogenic avian influenza hampered flight performance in migratory Bewick's swans, Cygnus columbianus (Van Gils et al., 2007), but an experimental study failed to find an effect of the equivalent of a 1500 km flight in a wind tunnel on immune function in red knots, Calidris canutus (Hasselquist et al., 2007).
Here, we studied this trade-off by conducting a wind tunnel experiment with three groups of European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris L.: ‘trained’,‘untrained’ and ‘post-flight’. The acquired arm of the immune system seems generally less affected by training and intensive exercise than the innate arm (Baj et al., 1994; Nieman et al., 1995; Tvede et al., 1991). We therefore focused on the constitutive aspects of the innate arm of the immune system and predicted that constitutive immune function decreases in response to training and, additionally, in response to immediate exercise. Birds in our trained group mimic individuals during migration that have been resting between migratory flights for at least 2 days. Birds in our post-flight group represent individuals that have just completed a migratory flight and have not yet had time to recover. Birds in the untrained group represent birds that did not perform a migratory flight and serve as a control. We hoped to thereby elucidate whether reduced immune measurements at a stopover site are a short-term response to flight rather than a reduced immunity associated with the whole migratory period.
We measured four components of the constitutive (non-inducible) branch of the immune system, which represents the birds' first line of defence. It may thus be important during short stopovers between migratory flights when there is not enough time to mount an acquired response (Schmid-Hempel and Ebert, 2003). Constitutive immune function must be maintained even when not in use, generating costs that may be important in the physiological trade-off with migratory flight (Buehler et al., 2008b; Martin et al., 2008). Specifically, we measured the activity of haptoglobin (Matson, 2006), an acute phase protein that offers protection against harmful end products of the immune response (Delers et al., 1988). We employed the haemolysis-haemagglutination assay (Matson et al., 2005) to quantify complement-like and natural antibody activity, respectively, which provide the first defence against spreading infections via cell lysis (Ochsenbein and Zinkernagel, 2000). And we examined leukocyte concentrations, which are indicative of circulating cellular immunity (Campbell, 1995).
We tested three predictions generated by the hypothesised life-history trade-off between mounting an immune response and satisfying the costs of exercise: constitutive immunity is lower (1) in post-flight birds compared with untrained birds, (2) in post-flight birds compared with trained birds that have had 2 days to recover from flight, and (3) in trained birds that had not flown for 2 days compared with untrained birds.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Bird capture and husbandry
All European starlings used in this study were captured during July 2009 in the vicinity of London, Ontario, Canada (43.17°N, 81.32°E). Starlings in the Great Lakes region only migrate short distances (Dolbeer, 1982); it may therefore be possible that flights of a few hours duration constitute intense exercise for these individuals. Birds were maintained in large aviaries (two sizes: 3.7×2.4×3.1 m or 2.3×2.4×3.5 m) until the beginning of the experiment in September 2009 when they were held in individual cages in a single room. The birds used in this study were part of a larger project that involved studying the effects of different diets on energetics during flight. All diets were isocaloric, had the same macronutrient composition (41% carbohydrate:13% protein:30% fat), and differed only in the relative amounts of plant oils that comprised the dietary fat (olive oil, sunflower oil) and the amount of supplementary vitamin E (5 or 30 IU kg-1 diet). We found no differences in any of the constitutive immunity measures between diets (general linear model; P>0.2), so we pooled these data for the remainder of the analyses reported here.
Experimental groups and flights
Birds were randomly assigned to one of the three treatment groups. Untrained birds (N=55) were housed in the large aviaries and never flew in the wind tunnel; all other birds were flown together in randomly assigned groups of three in the wind tunnel at 12 m s-1 and 15°C for 15 consecutive days according to the following schedule: days 1 and 2, 10 min; day 3=20 min; days 4 and 5, 30 min; day 6, 45 min; day 7, 60 min; day 8, 90 min; day 9, 30 min; day 10, 120 min; day 11, 180 min; day 12, no flight training; day 13, 60 min; day 14, 30 min; day 15, 240 min. On day 16, birds were flown together in randomly assigned groups of three for as long as they voluntarily flew (up to 4 h). We either sampled blood immediately after this final flight (post-flight, N=16) or 48 h afterwards (trained, N=29). In this way, trained and post-flight birds had very similar total flight times over the 16 days of exercise training with the primary difference between these two groups being whether we sampled the bird's blood immediately after flight (post-flight) or 2 days after their final flight (trained). All animal care protocols followed the Canadian Council on Animal Care guidelines and were approved by the University of Western Ontario Council on Animal Care and the Animal Use Subcommittee (protocol no. 2006-011-04) and by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Rhode Island (AN08-02-014).
Wind tunnel set-up
Birds were flown in a recirculating wind tunnel at the Advanced Facility for Avian Research at the University of Western Ontario. The test section is enclosed in a plenum (approximately 4×5×2.5 m high), which allows for precise control of temperature between -15 and 30°C and humidity between 10% and 90% RH. The closed portion of the test section is a 2 m long octagon with 1 m vertical and 1.5 m horizontal dimensions. A 0.5 m long open section at the rear of the test section allows birds to enter and exit the air stream with minimal disturbance to the flow. True air speed can be controlled between approximately 2 and 21 m s-1. Without an upstream net and outside of the wall boundary layer velocity uniformity (standard deviation of velocity) is 0.5% and turbulence intensity ranges between 0.12% and 0.27% (Gerson and Guglielmo, 2011). In this study, birds were flown with a net (0.15 mm filament thickness, 2.5×2.5 cm mesh) at the front of the test section.
Blood collection
Blood samples were taken from untrained and trained birds usually within 10 min (25 min if a second bird was sampled on any given day) of capture from their cages. Blood samples were taken from post-flight birds within 5 min of completion of their final flight. Birds were anaesthetised by inhaling isoflurane and decapitated for the study of the effect of diet on tissue composition (see ‘Bird capture and husbandry’). From each bird, we collected ca. 1 ml arterial blood into a heparinised 1.5 ml microcentrifuge tube. Immediately after sampling we made two blood smears on glass slides (Campbell, 1995); the remainder of the blood was processed within 1 h of sampling. Plasma was obtained by centrifuging blood samples for 10 min at 12,000 g and stored at -80°C until processing. At time of blood sampling, we also measured body mass with an electronic balance to the nearest 0.1 g, and tarsus length with digital calipers to the nearest 0.01 mm.
Measuring immune function
Haptoglobin is an acute phase protein that offers protection against harmful end products of the immune response (Delers et al., 1988). To quantify plasma haptoglobin levels, we followed the ‘manual method’ in a commercial kit (no. TP801; Tri-Delta Diagnostics, Morris Plains, NJ, USA), which exploits the peroxidase activity of haptoglobin bound to haemoglobin.
The haemolysis-haemagglutination assay quantifies complement-like and natural antibody activity, which provides the first line of defence against spreading infections via cell lysis (Ochsenbein and Zinkernagel, 2000). The agglutination reaction measures blood clumping caused by the interaction between natural antibodies and antigens in rabbit erythrocytes. The lytic reaction measures the amount of haemoglobin released from the lysis of rabbit erythrocytes. In both cases, quantification is achieved by serial dilution of plasma samples and assessment of the dilution step at which either the agglutination or the lysis reaction stopped (Matson et al., 2005). Specifically, we placed 25 μl of plasma in the first two rows of a 96-well round-bottomed plate. From rows 2-11, we performed ten 1:2 dilutions using Dulbecco's PBS. We then added 25 μl of 1% rabbit red blood cell suspension to each well and placed the plates in a water bath at 37°C for 90 min. Afterwards, we tilted the plates 45 deg and then scanned them (HP Scanjet G3110) after 20 min for agglutination and after 90 min for lysis. The scans were scored blindly by a single observer (S.N.) using the criteria outlined elsewhere (Matson et al., 2005).
The concentrations of the different types of leukocytes are indicative of different aspects of the immune function (Campbell, 1995). Heterophils are phagocytes that respond immediately to novel pathogens during the initial immune response. They are the counterparts of the mammalian neutrophils and form the first line of cellular defence against invading microbial pathogens. Inflammation or infectious conditions cause a dramatic influx of heterophils (Juul-Madsen et al., 2008). Eosinophils are cytotoxic cells with the ability to kill other cells as well as large extracellular parasites. Lymphocytes orchestrate the antibody and cell-mediated functions of the acquired immune system. Monocytes link constitutive, non-specific (innate) immunity to induced, specific (acquired) immune functions. They are precursors of macrophages, which are involved in initiating and directing the innate and specific immune responses, the systemic acute phase response, tissue repair and tissue remodelling (Klasing, 1998). Thrombocytes play a role in blood clotting and are also phagocytic (Campbell, 1995).
Blood smears were fixed in 100% methanol prior to staining in Hemacolor stain set (Harleco, Gibbstown, NJ, USA) and examined at ×1000 magnification with oil immersion. The first 100 leukocytes were counted and classified as heterophils, eosinophils, lymphocytes or monocytes. While counting the first 100 leukocytes, we also recorded the number of thrombocytes seen as an estimate of the relative number of thrombocytes per leukocyte (Buehler et al., 2008b). In combination with the blood smears, we obtained total leukocyte concentrations using the indirect eosinophil Unopette method (Campbell, 1995), following the manufacturer's instructions (No. 5877, Becton Dickinson, Franklin Lakes, NJ, USA). Note that the sample size differs among assays based on plasma availability.
Statistical analyses
To eliminate between-plate variation in haptoglobin data, we normalised the distribution of each plate by subtracting the mean from each value and dividing it by the standard deviation, yielding a z-score with a mean of zero (Zar, 1999). Agglutination data were squared, lysis data square-root transformed, and leukocyte data log10 (x+0.001) transformed to meet the assumptions of normality and equal variance.
To compare immune function among treatment groups, we grouped the soluble immune components (haptoglobin, agglutination and lysis) and the cellular components (leukocyte counts) and analysed both sets of data using a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). To control for body mass differences, we regressed body mass against tarsus length and included the residuals as a covariate. ‘Treatment group’ was the main factor and ‘training group’ was included as a covariate. Flights of less than 60 min duration (N=8) were excluded from this analysis. To verify whether the MANOVA revealed significant differences among groups, we used the Pillai trace test statistic, as it is considered to be less sensitive to deviations from assumptions than other multivariate statistics (Johnson and Field, 1993; Olson, 1976). Among-group comparisons were evaluated using Hotelling's T-square statistic. When the Pillai trace test was significant, we performed one-tailed t-tests to describe how each individual immune indicator varied with experimental treatment. We present two- and one-tailed tests as we predict the direction of the expected effect (Sokal and Rohlf, 1995); namely, decreased immune function in the post-flight compared with the untrained as well as the trained group and in the trained compared with the untrained group.
We also tested for an effect of experimental flight duration on immune indicators while controlling for experimental group using the general linear model (GLM) package (type III sum of squares). Here, flights of all durations were included. We used visual examination of the distribution of the residuals of the GLM to evaluate normality of the data distribution (Zuur et al., 2009). The mean final flight duration (including flights of less than 60 min) was 142 min (N=53; range 7-431 min, s.d. 79.261). The birds in the post-flight group (mean 180 min) flew on average longer (t=2.492; d.f.=52, P=0016) than those in the trained group (mean 124 min). All analyses were performed with the statistical software SYSTAT v.13 (SYSTAT® 13 Statistics I II III IV, SYSTAT Software Inc., www.systat.com).
RESULTS
MANOVA
Size-corrected body mass did not affect haptoglobin, agglutination or lysis scores (Pillai trace=0.044, d.f.=3,47, P=0.541), nor did flight group (Pillai trace=0.462, d.f.=18,147, P=0.103). Experimental treatment, however, significantly affected these three measures of immune function (Pillai trace=0.291, d.f.=6,96, P=0.018). Pair-wise comparisons showed that the post-flight group differed significantly from the trained group (Hotelling's T-square=28.554, P<0.001) and from the untrained group (Hotelling's T-square=16.175, P=0.003) while the trained and untrained groups did not differ (Hotelling's T-square=6.430, P=0.126).
Haptoglobin
Plasma haptoglobin concentration decreased from the untrained group to the trained group, and was lowest in the post-flight group. The mean of the untrained group was higher than that of the post-flight group (pooled variance: t=1.976, d.f.=57, one-tailed t-test P=0.027). There was no significant difference between untrained and trained (pooled variance: t=0.953, d.f.=68, one-tailed P=0.172), or between trained and post-flight groups (pooled variance: t=-1.088, d.f.=37, one-tailed P=0.142; Fig. 1A).
We then tested for an effect of group and flight time on haptoglobin concentration. There was no interaction between flight time and group (F1,40=0.208, P=0.651), nor was there an effect of group (F1,40=0.665, P=0.420). We therefore pooled birds from the two groups and showed that plasma haptoglobin concentration decreased with increasing flight duration (F1,42=7.241, P=0.001, r2=0.147; Fig. 2). Removal of the longest flight (431 min) did not alter the result (F1,41=4.817, P=0.034).
Three measures of constitutive immune function in untrained, trained and post-flight European starlings. Untrained birds were kept in cages and not flown, trained birds were sampled 48 h after a continuous 1-4 h flight, and post-flight birds were sampled immediately after a continuous 1-4 h flight. Sample sizes are given in parentheses, and letters indicate among-group differences. (A) Plasma haptoglobin concentration (z-score ±1 s.e.m.). (B) Agglutination scores (squared ±1 s.e.m.). (C) Lysis scores (square root ±1 s.e.m.).
Three measures of constitutive immune function in untrained, trained and post-flight European starlings. Untrained birds were kept in cages and not flown, trained birds were sampled 48 h after a continuous 1-4 h flight, and post-flight birds were sampled immediately after a continuous 1-4 h flight. Sample sizes are given in parentheses, and letters indicate among-group differences. (A) Plasma haptoglobin concentration (z-score ±1 s.e.m.). (B) Agglutination scores (squared ±1 s.e.m.). (C) Lysis scores (square root ±1 s.e.m.).
Agglutination
We also saw a decrease in agglutination scores from the untrained group to the trained group to the post-flight group. The mean of the untrained group was higher than that of the post-flight group (pooled variance: t=2.557, d.f.=70, one-tailed P=0.006) and trained birds also tended to have higher agglutination than post-flight birds (pooled variance: t=-1.430, d.f.=43, one-tailed P=0.080; Fig. 1B). Trained and untrained birds did not differ (pooled variance: t=1.163, d.f.=83, one-tailed P=0.124). We did not find an effect of flight time on agglutination scores (group: F1,48=1.865, P=0.178; flight time: F1,48=0.905, P=0.346).
Lysis
The pattern of lysis scores differed somewhat from those shown for haptoglobin and agglutination as the trained group did not differ from the untrained group. However, as with the other immune data, the post-flight group scored lowest. Both untrained birds and trained birds had higher scores than post-flight birds (separate variance: t=2.328, d.f.=43, one-tailed P=0.012; pooled variance: t=-2.817, d.f.=43, one-tailed P=0.004), but untrained and trained birds were not different (pooled variance: t=-0.606, d.f.=83, one-tailed P=0.727; Fig. 1C). We did not find an effect of flight time on lysis scores (group: F1,48=7.638, P=0.008; flight time: F1,48=0.056, P=0.813).
Leukocyte concentration
No effect of experimental group on either total leukocyte concentration or the concentration of any type of leukocyte was detected (Pillai trace=1.460, d.f.=12,8, P=0.205; Table 1), nor was there an effect of size-corrected body mass (Pillai trace=0.749, d.f.=6,3, P=0.399) or flight group (Pillai trace=2.674, d.f.=36,48, P=0.407). We also did not detect any significant changes in total leukocyte concentration or the concentration of any type of leukocyte with flight time (P>0.6, data not shown).
DISCUSSION
In European starlings, three out of four indicators of the constitutive immune system decreased immediately after an endurance flight in a wind tunnel. These results are consistent with the predicted trade-off between constitutive immune function and intense exercise. Constitutive immune function is maintained even when not actively fighting an infection, and the costs of maintaining it are thus considered to be important in the physiological trade-off with migratory flight (Buehler et al., 2008b; Martin et al., 2008). These results add to the growing body of evidence supporting an energetic trade-off between immune function and other life-history traits. For example, studies in birds have shown that immune function is negatively related to reproductive effort (Ardia, 2005; Deerenberg et al., 1997; Lochmiller and Deerenberg, 2000; Moreno et al., 1999; Norris and Evans, 2000), growth rate (Mauck et al., 2005) and rapid feather growth during moult (Martin, 2005). Likewise, experimentally inducing immune responses leads to a decrease in body condition (Sanz et al., 2004) and nestling growth (Alonso-Alvarez and Tella, 2001).
This is the first study to examine constitutive immune function in the context of training (trained) versus the immediate effect of exercise (post-flight). The physiological demands placed upon individuals in the two groups resemble those experienced by birds during the migratory period; namely, birds that have been resting between migratory flights for at least 2 days, and birds that have just completed a migratory flight. Lysis was lower in post-flight than in trained birds, and haptoglobin and agglutination showed the same trend. These results can be interpreted in two ways. (1) Constitutive immune function decreases in response to being in the migratory state (trained), and decreases further as an immediate effect of having performed a migratory flight (post-flight). (2) Constitutive immune function decreases in response to exercise, but recovers again within a short time period.
A recent study on red knots provides support for the second explanation. In red knots C. c. rufa caught during a migratory stop-over at Delaware Bay, USA, haptoglobin and lysis scores were higher in birds storing fuel for subsequent flight (presumed equivalent to our ‘trained’ birds) than in those recovering immediately after a preceding flight (our post-flight group) (Buehler et al., 2010b). Assuming that the recovering birds arrived more recently than those depositing fuel stores, two possible explanations for their finding are discussed. First, fuelling birds may experience an increased rate of infection or may be bolstering their immune defence in response to high antigen exposure. Second, birds recovering from flight may be immuno-compromised because of the physical strain of migratory flight or as a result of adaptive trade-offs between immune function and migration. Our results are consistent with their second hypothesis. Contrary to our results, Buehler and colleagues did not find an effect of migratory flight on agglutination (Buehler et al., 2010b). It is possible that the effect of flight on immune measures differs between species.
We did not find a difference among experimental groups in the concentration of any type of leukocyte. This may be for one of five reasons. (1) The leukocyte profiles in our study were highly variable (Table 1), which is consistent with other studies (Buehler et al., 2010b), and so the effect size was too small to be detected with the available sample size. (2) The flight time was not long enough to result in a difference in leukocyte profiles. The average flight duration was somewhat longer than 2 h, but studies in other taxa have shown that the initial leukocyte response to strenuous exercise only becomes apparent after a time span of hours or even days (Cardinet et al., 1964; Davis et al., 2008). (3) The constitutive arm of the immune function has both cellular (i.e. leukocytes) and soluble components (e.g. acute phase proteins and antibodies). It is possible that the trade-off between strenuous exercise and immune function happens at the soluble but not at the cellular level of the constitutive arm. (4) The effect of migratory flight is masked by the effect of stress. Handling and blood sampling is thought to be an acute stressor, which triggers changes in leukocyte profiles (Davis, 2005; Davis et al., 2008; Dhabhar, 2002; Dhabhar et al., 1995; Gross and Siegel, 1983). However, because our handling times never exceeded 30 min, this effect should be minimal (Buehler et al., 2008a). (5) Finally, the blood that was used to make the smears had come into contact with heparin. We chose heparin because other anticoagulants (such as EDTA) can cause haemolysis in bird species (Campbell and Ellis, 2007), which negatively affects haemolysis-haemagglutination and haptoglobin assays. However, heparin may cause clumping of leukocytes and thrombocytes, which may have increased the variability in our counts.
We showed not only that the indicators of the soluble constitutive immune system are lower in the post-flight than in the untrained group but also that haptoglobin decreased with increasing flight duration. Haptoglobin binds iron (haeme), which prevents it from participating in free radical chain reactions (Dobryszychka, 1997; Gutteridge, 1987) and from serving as a nutrient for invading bacteria (Exton, 1997). Iron is also an important promoter of oxidative stress during exercise (Cooper et al., 2002). Haptoglobin appears to play an important physiological role as an antioxidant, particularly during haemolysis, i.e. the breakdown of red blood cells (Lim et al., 2000). Exercise-induced haemolysis is well documented in humans (Sentürk et al., 2005), and has been suggested to occur in birds (Landys-Ciannelli et al., 2002; Piersma et al., 1996). If haptoglobin levels are depleted by binding to oxidative iron during migration, this may explain the decrease in haptoglobin we found with increasing flight duration. One implication of this finding is that haptoglobin may not be available to offer protection against harmful end products of the immune response (Delers et al., 1988) during stopover immediately after a long flight.
Our results are in contrast to those of Hasselquist and colleagues, who found no effect of prolonged flight in a wind tunnel on immune function in red knots (Hasselquist et al., 2007). They measured cell-mediated and humoral acquired induced immunity, while our study measured indicators of the innate constitutive response. The difference in results may reflect the different costs of the various aspects of the immune system during maintenance and use. Acquired, induced immunity is expensive to develop, but cheap to maintain and use, while innate constitutive immunity is cheap to develop and use and of medium cost to maintain (Klasing, 2004; Lee, 2006). It may thus be that a trade-off between exercise and immune function only becomes apparent at the level of maintaining (as opposed to developing or using) the different arms of the immunity. Another possible explanation is that, like in humans, acquired induced immunity in birds may not respond to exercise to the same degree as innate immunity (Nieman and Pedersen, 1999). Or there could be a trade-off between the different arms of the immune system itself (Buehler et al., 2009; Klasing, 2004; Schmid-Hempel and Ebert, 2003).
Our study has implications for the transmission of avian-borne diseases. Migrating birds can become long-range vectors for any bacterium, virus or parasite that they carry (Reed et al., 2003). Studies on avian malaria in migratory shorebirds have shown a low prevalence for the parasite (Earle and Underhill, 1993; Mendes et al., 2005; Peirce, 1981). Parasite infection could simply be a rare occurrence but it is also possible that infected individuals do not migrate. In this context it is interesting to note that migration distance was inversely related to parasite prevalence in monarch butterflies Danaus plexippus (Altizer et al., 2000), and to the intensity of infection with warble fly larvae, Hypoderma tarandi, in reindeer, Rangifer tarandus (Folstad et al., 1991). While migration may be a behavioural adaptation to reduce parasitic infection (Altizer et al., 2011; Folstad et al., 1991), it may also be possible that infected individuals are not capable of migrating as far as healthy ones, suggesting the same trade-off between immune function and migration in other taxa.
The next step will now be to test whether the reported trade-off between prolonged flight and constitutive immune function makes migrating birds more susceptible to infections and whether infections hamper their ability to perform long-distance migrations. Because of the recent emergence of avian-borne diseases such as West Nile virus (Marra et al., 2004), severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS) and avian influenza (Liu et al., 2005), understanding the trade-offs and challenges faced by long-distance migrants has gained a new level of relevance and urgency.
FOOTNOTES
FUNDING
Funding was provided to S.R.M. by the US National Science Foundation [grant no. IOS-0748349], the United States Department of Agriculture [grant no. RIAES-538748] and the University of Rhode Island, and to C.G.G. by the Natural Science and Engineering Council of Canada [NSERC Discovery Grant; 311901-2005 RGPIN], the Canada Foundation for Innovation [grant no. 11826], the Ontario Research Fund [grant no. 11743], and the University of Western Ontario Academic Development Fund [grant no. SG10-18]. D.M.B. was supported by the Natural Science and Engineering Council of Canada [NSERC grant no. PDF-373488-2009] and the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research [NWO; Rubicon 825.09.0190].
Acknowledgements
We thank Alexander Macmillan for performing the differential leukocyte counts, W. Bezner-Kerr for assistance with wind tunnel operations and M. Rebuli for help with animal care. Liam McGuire provided statistical advice.