Communication signals may be energetically expensive or inexpensive to produce, depending on the function of the signal and the competitive nature of the communication system. Males of sexually selected species may produce high-energy advertisement signals, both to enhance detectability and to signal their size and body condition. Accordingly, the proportion of the energy budget allocated to signal production ranges from almost nothing for many signals to somewhere in excess of 50% for acoustic signals in short-lived sexually selected species. Recent data from gymnotiform electric fish reveal mechanisms that regulate energy allocated to sexual advertisement signals through dynamical remodeling of the excitable membranes in the electric organ. Further, males of the short-lived sexually selected species, Brachyhypopomus gauderio, trade off among different metabolic compartments, allocating energy to signal production while reducing energy used in other metabolic functions. Female B. gauderio, by contrast, do not trade off energy between signaling and other functions. To fuel energetically expensive signal production, we expect a continuum of strategies to be adopted by animals of different life history strategies. Future studies should explore the relation between life history and energy allocation trade-offs.

The structure and function of animal communication signals, like all traits, can be subject to physiological constraints and limits. Energetic costs of signals have interested those who work on the interface between evolution, behavior and physiology because signal costs can be measured more readily than many other socially relevant behaviors. Across the suite of signal forms, the amount of energy that animals allocate to communication signals depends on a suite of physical and life history factors (Fig. 1). For instance, passive visual signals, such as colorful plumage or iridescent scales (Endler and Thery, 1996; Rutowski et al., 2010), should require the least energy investment on the part of the signaler, because the sun provides the energy in the signal, although pigment accumulation or growth of diffracting structures may entail other nutritional or developmental costs (Andersson, 2000; Fitzpatrick, 1998; McGraw, 2006). Conversely, during bouts of active broadcast, mate-attraction signals can exceed the rest of the animal's energy budget. Where the interests of signaler and receiver are strategically aligned (Searcy and Nowicki, 2005), communication should be cooperative and signals should be as efficient as possible (signals to alert relatives of predators may be an exception, because the stakes are so high). By contrast, when interests of signaler and receiver differ, as occurs under sexual competition, the signaler may be under pressure to allocate considerable energy to signals. In such cases, high-energy signaling can be promoted through either of two mechanisms: efficacy or conspicuous consumption. Signals of efficacy are those in which a stronger signal is more effective for physical or physiological reasons, rather than for social or informational reasons (Maynard-Smith and Harper, 2003). Louder signals can be effective because they command a larger active space (detectable area), aiding in territorial defense or advertisement to distant conspecifics (Brenowitz, 1986). Alternatively, louder signals may be effective at close range because they render the signaler more impressive to prospective mates or rivals by providing more stimulation to the receiver's sensory system (Brenowitz, 1989). Researchers have shown that increasing signal amplitude can substitute for tuning the signal to the optimal part of the sensory spectrum in determining the attractiveness functions of a signal (Gerhardt, 2005; Ritschard et al., 2010). In such cases, signal intensity can be favored because it increases sensory stimulation, potentially over a range of distances. The concept of conspicuous consumption is that individuals consume resources to show their wealth (Veblen, 1899). Applied to signaling, strong signals may impress receivers because the energy consumed by the signal reflects general energy availability in the signaler, an honest indicator of the signaler's physical condition (Salazar and Stoddard, 2008), in keeping with handicap models (Grafen, 1990; Zahavi, 1975; Zahavi, 1977).

Fig. 1.

Close-range and long-range communication signals may be either high- or low-energy, depending on their function. At close range, high-energy signals may indicate energy availability through conspicuous consumption, indicating the quality of the signaler. Conversely, high-energy signals may render the signal more efficacious by increasing the transmission distance or by increasing the degree of sensory stimulation at any range. Low-energy signals include visual signals and cooperative signals, where the interests of the signaler and the receiver are strategically aligned.

Fig. 1.

Close-range and long-range communication signals may be either high- or low-energy, depending on their function. At close range, high-energy signals may indicate energy availability through conspicuous consumption, indicating the quality of the signaler. Conversely, high-energy signals may render the signal more efficacious by increasing the transmission distance or by increasing the degree of sensory stimulation at any range. Low-energy signals include visual signals and cooperative signals, where the interests of the signaler and the receiver are strategically aligned.

Fig. 2.

Energetics of signal production relative to energy at rest, expressed as the natural log of oxygen consumption. Horizontal lines represent equivalent energy expenditure on signals; diagonal lines represent equal proportion of energy allocated to signal and metabolism at rest (equal factorial scope). Comparative data can be viewed two ways, either by the total energy devoted to signaling (y-axis only) or as a ratio of energy in the signal versus energy used at rest (diagonal isolines). Considering only mass-specific energy of signaling, Orthoptera have the highest energy consumption, whereas a wolf spider, Orthoptera and frogs use the greatest amount of energy to signal relative to their baseline metabolism. See Table 1 for species numbers.

Fig. 2.

Energetics of signal production relative to energy at rest, expressed as the natural log of oxygen consumption. Horizontal lines represent equivalent energy expenditure on signals; diagonal lines represent equal proportion of energy allocated to signal and metabolism at rest (equal factorial scope). Comparative data can be viewed two ways, either by the total energy devoted to signaling (y-axis only) or as a ratio of energy in the signal versus energy used at rest (diagonal isolines). Considering only mass-specific energy of signaling, Orthoptera have the highest energy consumption, whereas a wolf spider, Orthoptera and frogs use the greatest amount of energy to signal relative to their baseline metabolism. See Table 1 for species numbers.

Biologists have carried out an unofficial contest to identify the creature that allocates the greatest amount of energy to its communication signals. Unfortunately, we have no data from the other end of the energy continuum; nobody has measured signal costs of passive and cooperative signalers because those costs have always been assumed to constitute an insignificant fraction of the energy budget. Even within the high end of the energy continuum, both absolute and relative energetic signal costs vary across three orders of magnitude (Table 1, Fig. 2). In terms of absolute costs, male orthopterans (e.g. the trilling katydid) expend the most energy per unit mass producing their acoustic mate attraction signals, followed by a notably loud and hyperactive woodland passerine, the Carolina wren. By comparing the ratio of energy for signal production to energy spent at rest, we can establish an index to compare the relative effort put into signaling (compare along diagonal isolines in Fig. 2). Here we see that male wolf spiders, orthopterans and hylid treefrogs put the most energy into signals relative to their resting metabolisms. Passerine birds have a relatively constant ratio. Roosters and male electric fish are tied, both in terms of absolute and relative metrics. Female electric fish use the least energy for signaling. As with bat navigational sonar, navigation signals in electric fish are a negligible fraction of the total energy budget (Julian et al., 2003; Speakman et al., 1989; Speakman and Racey, 1991).

Table 1.

Energetic costs of signal production and resting metabolic rate are shown for 24 species for which we found comparable data

Energetic costs of signal production and resting metabolic rate are shown for 24 species for which we found comparable data
Energetic costs of signal production and resting metabolic rate are shown for 24 species for which we found comparable data
Fig. 3.

Stronger signals are more effective in territorial electric fish. In nighttime field trials in a stream in the Venezuelan Llanos, male Brachyhypopomus diazi approached a playback electrode playing a conspecific signal over the range of natural intensities recorded for this species. Approach probability was in linear proportion to stimulus intensity. The results shown here are for a single male; data are means of eight 60-s trials per voltage. The interpretation is ambiguous: higher voltages could elicit a greater chance of approach because they signal a greater threat or because they command a larger active space and are more readily detected (P.K.S., unpublished).

Fig. 3.

Stronger signals are more effective in territorial electric fish. In nighttime field trials in a stream in the Venezuelan Llanos, male Brachyhypopomus diazi approached a playback electrode playing a conspecific signal over the range of natural intensities recorded for this species. Approach probability was in linear proportion to stimulus intensity. The results shown here are for a single male; data are means of eight 60-s trials per voltage. The interpretation is ambiguous: higher voltages could elicit a greater chance of approach because they signal a greater threat or because they command a larger active space and are more readily detected (P.K.S., unpublished).

Gymnotiform electric fish warrant a closer look because we have data on energetic costs of signal production for both sexes. Gymnotiform electric fish signal around the clock for electrolocation, but also signal for social function during their hours of activity, primarily at night. Three of the five gymnotiform families can modulate the power in their signals by remodeling the excitable membranes in their electrogenic cells in accordance with the prevailing social conditions through the fast actions of melanocortin peptides (Franchina et al., 2001; Markham et al., 2009; Markham and Stoddard, 2005). Stronger signals command more active space in territorial species and may be more potent in social interactions because they provide greater stimulation to receivers at close range; increasing active space and increasing stimulation are not mutually exclusive benefits of stronger signals. An example of the effect of signal amplitude on male territorial behavior is seen in field playback trials on territorial male gymnotiform electric fish (Fig. 3). Male Brachyhypopomus diazi cruise their territories at night and will approach and nudge electrodes playing digitized signals played at realistic voltages. The probability that a territorial male approaches a playback electrode is linearly proportional to the intensity of the electric playback signal. A stronger stimulus might indicate a greater threat and thus elicit a greater chance of response; conversely, the male might simply be more likely to detect a stronger signal. We also see that females prefer to mate with larger males, which produce larger electric signals (Curtis and Stoddard, 2003). For any of these reasons, territorial or sexually selected species may be under selective pressure to boost signal power.

Brachyhypopomus gauderio (Giora and Malabarba, 2009) is a sexually selected species of electric fish that inhabits basins south of the Amazon, recently split from B. pinnicaudatus (Giora and Malabarba, 2009). Males produce signals of greater amplitude and duration than females (Fig. 4A). We combined pharmacology and oxygen respirometry to analyze the cost of electrogenesis () in both sexes of this species (Salazar and Stoddard, 2008). First, we took baseline measurements of oxygen consumption and signal production in a resting fish. Second, we respired the fish with a tranquilizing solution containing the GABA binding enhancer (+) metomidate, which eliminated activity and muscle tone while allowing the electric organ discharge (EOD) to persist (Fig. 4B). Third, we injected the fish with a curare analog, which silenced the electric organ. By subtraction, we partitioned the energy budget into three components: standard metabolic rate (SMR), electricity production and ‘other’, which consists of activity and muscle tone (Fig. 4). The extremely high correlation between the cost of electrogenesis and the power in the signal vouch for the reliability of this method (Fig. 5). We found that females allocate a small fraction (∼3%) of their daily energy budget to electrogenesis, but males averaged 15%, with considerable inter-individual variation, as expected in a sexually selected species.

Fig. 4.

(A) The gymnotiform electric fish Brachyhypopomus gauderio shows extreme sexual dimorphism in the size of the tail and the amplitude and duration of the electric organ discharge (EOD). (B) The energetic cost of electric signals in stationary fish was determined through oxygen respirometry and pharmacology (Salazar and Stoddard, 2008). The fish was held in an electrically transparent ceramic tube, respired with the regular water or with the tranquilizer (+) metomidate. While the fish was tranquilized, its gills were irrigated with oxygenated water. Dissolved oxygen (DO) was measured while the EOD was digitized with an analog-to-digital converter (ADC). (C) Energetic compartments were separated with pharmacology and arithmetic. When the fish was on GABA binding enhancer (+) metomidate HCl, the EOD continued but the fish was still and lacked muscle tone. The curare analog flaxedil blocked the EOD, leaving just the standard metabolic rate minus muscle tone.

Fig. 4.

(A) The gymnotiform electric fish Brachyhypopomus gauderio shows extreme sexual dimorphism in the size of the tail and the amplitude and duration of the electric organ discharge (EOD). (B) The energetic cost of electric signals in stationary fish was determined through oxygen respirometry and pharmacology (Salazar and Stoddard, 2008). The fish was held in an electrically transparent ceramic tube, respired with the regular water or with the tranquilizer (+) metomidate. While the fish was tranquilized, its gills were irrigated with oxygenated water. Dissolved oxygen (DO) was measured while the EOD was digitized with an analog-to-digital converter (ADC). (C) Energetic compartments were separated with pharmacology and arithmetic. When the fish was on GABA binding enhancer (+) metomidate HCl, the EOD continued but the fish was still and lacked muscle tone. The curare analog flaxedil blocked the EOD, leaving just the standard metabolic rate minus muscle tone.

Fig. 5.

The proof of the method to assess energetic cost of electric signaling lies in the tight correlations between energy consumed in electrogenesis () and EOD power (estimated by the time integral of voltage-squared). The correlations are equally strong for (A) whole-animal metabolic rates and (B) mass-corrected metabolic rates (Salazar and Stoddard, 2008).

Fig. 5.

The proof of the method to assess energetic cost of electric signaling lies in the tight correlations between energy consumed in electrogenesis () and EOD power (estimated by the time integral of voltage-squared). The correlations are equally strong for (A) whole-animal metabolic rates and (B) mass-corrected metabolic rates (Salazar and Stoddard, 2008).

Subsequent analysis of this data set has revealed a trade-off between energy allocated to signaling and energy allocated to self-maintenance (Fig. 6). In sexually mature male B. gauderio, energy spent on general cellular metabolism () is inversely correlated with energy spent on electrogenesis (), whereas in females, energy spent on general metabolism and electric signaling are positively correlated. The relationship holds whether one considers whole-animal or mass-corrected metabolic rates. Thus, it appears that the male pattern of high allocation to signal energy comes at the cost of general cellular maintenance. Exactly which compartment pays the cost is not clear. Is it the overall rate of cellular metabolism or just some part, such as immune function? Do individuals adjust this trade-off across their lives, or do different individuals establish different balances of signal versus self-maintenance?

Speakman posited alternate energy allocation models that could account for positive and inverse relations between signal metabolism and resting metabolism as described above for female and male B. gauderio (Speakman, 1997). These have been termed the ‘performance model’ and the ‘allocation model’, respectively (Careau et al., 2008). Under the performance model, resting metabolic rate determines the energy available to tissues, which, in turn, determines activity level. Thus, resting metabolic rate and some other body function (signal energy in this case) are positively related. Under the allocation model, energy availability determines both resting metabolic rate and activity level: if the individual has an absolute ceiling on its energetic output, and its activity is high enough, it will experience a trade-off between resting metabolic rate and activity level. The most common pattern among vertebrates is for resting metabolic rate and activity to be positively correlated, although males tend to show the opposite trend (Careau et al., 2008). It seems unlikely that these two models would operate independently in both sexes of B. gauderio, but it might be the case that the allocation model applies to both sexes, whereas only males have a high enough total energy output to experience an energy ceiling and thus a trade-off between SMR and signal energy (Fig. 7).

Fig. 6.

In female Brachyhypopomus gauderio, energy allocated to electrogenesis () correlates positively with the standard metabolic rate (). In males, however, the correlation is negative, suggesting a trade-off in which males obtain the energy to boost signal power by reducing energy allocated to cellular metabolism. The correlations apply to (A) whole-animal and (B) and mass-corrected measurements.

Fig. 6.

In female Brachyhypopomus gauderio, energy allocated to electrogenesis () correlates positively with the standard metabolic rate (). In males, however, the correlation is negative, suggesting a trade-off in which males obtain the energy to boost signal power by reducing energy allocated to cellular metabolism. The correlations apply to (A) whole-animal and (B) and mass-corrected measurements.

Androgens mediate sexually dimorphic signal characteristics in electric fish over days or weeks (reviewed in Zakon, 2000; Zakon, 1996). We speculate that, as males come into breeding condition, rising androgen levels regulate the metabolic trade-off between signal energy and SMR. Testosterone treatment of male emberizine sparrows increases activity while lowering resting metabolic rate (Lynn et al., 2000; Wikelski et al., 1999). The opposite has been seen in male house sparrows, Passer domesticus, where androgens produce large male plumage signals (black bibs), while simultaneously raising basal metabolic rate (Buchanan et al., 2001). The black bib of the house sparrow consumes no energy once produced and initial melanin synthesis during feather production entails little energetic expense compared with activity. By contrast, the physical activity and acoustic or electric signal production entail significant energetic expense, presumably paid for through other metabolic savings. In addition, SMRs of every male B. gauderio we tested exceeded those of every female, despite the presumed trade-off (Fig. 6). Androgens increase signal power in B. gauderio (Allee et al., 2009), and also reduce immunocompetence in a variety of vertebrates (Cutolo et al., 2002; Deviche and Cortez, 2005). Thus, males with the strongest signals could be obtaining the energy needed to boost signal power by reallocating energy away from immune function or other ‘discretionary’ functions (Wedekind and Folstad, 1994).

Fig. 7.

Model of metabolic allocation and trade-offs, derived from Speakman (Speakman, 1997). As metabolic investment in reproduction rises, both the standard metabolic rate (SMR) and the signal metabolic rate (signal MR) (or other activities) will increase. Eventually, however, the animal reaches a metabolic ceiling and further investment in reproductive behavior necessitates a trade-off in other metabolic compartments, SMR in the case shown here. The investment patterns shown by female and male Brachyhypopomus gauderio in Fig. 6 are depicted here within the gray patches.

Fig. 7.

Model of metabolic allocation and trade-offs, derived from Speakman (Speakman, 1997). As metabolic investment in reproduction rises, both the standard metabolic rate (SMR) and the signal metabolic rate (signal MR) (or other activities) will increase. Eventually, however, the animal reaches a metabolic ceiling and further investment in reproductive behavior necessitates a trade-off in other metabolic compartments, SMR in the case shown here. The investment patterns shown by female and male Brachyhypopomus gauderio in Fig. 6 are depicted here within the gray patches.

We wish to highlight two outstanding areas for future inquiry. First, it would be useful to obtain comparable energetic costs for signalers in each of the four categories shown in Fig. 1: low-energy cooperative signalers, low-energy passive signalers, high-energy long-distance signalers and high-energy close-range signalers. Currently, we have the most data for the high-energy long-distance signalers. Phylogenetically controlled contrasts, where possible, would make such comparisons particularly valuable.

Second, outside the context of seasonal survival strategies such as migration and hibernation, little is known about the effects of life history choices on energy allocation (Garland et al., 1992). One would expect short-lived or semelparous species to allocate proportionally more energy to reproductive signals than long-lived or iteroperous species, even at the cost of self-maintenance and long-term survival. In fact, the electric fish B. gauderio fits the life-history profile of short-lived species, but we need taxonomically comparable data for longer-lived iteroparous taxa to determine whether whether longevity determines energy allocation to communication signals.

This work was supported by NIH grant MBRS GM08205 and NSF grant IOS-0956603 to P.K.S. Thanks to Jim Heffernan, John Speakman and Peter Biro for helpful discussion. Deposited in PMC for release after 12 months.

Allee
S. J.
,
Markham
M. R.
,
Stoddard
P. K.
(
2009
).
Androgens enhance plasticity of an electric communication signal in female knifefish, Brachyhypopomus pinnicaudatus
.
Horm. Behav.
56
,
264
-
273
.
Andersson
S.
(
2000
).
Efficacy and content in avian color signals
. In
Animal Signals: Signalling and Signal Design in Animal Communication
(ed.
Espmark
Y.
,
Amundsen
T.
,
Rosenquist
G.
), pp.
47
-
60
.
Trodheim, Norway
:
Tapir Academic Press
.
Brenowitz
E. A.
(
1986
).
Environmental influences on acoustic and electric animal communication
.
Brain Behav. Evol.
28
,
32
-
42
.
Brenowitz
E. A.
(
1989
).
Neighbor call amplitude influences aggressive behavior and intermale spacing in choruses of the Pacific tree frog (Hyla regilla)
.
Ethology
83
,
69
-
79
.
Buchanan
K. L.
,
Evans
M. R.
,
Goldsmith
A. R.
,
Bryant
D. M.
,
Rowe
L. V.
(
2001
).
Testosterone influences basal metabolic rate in male house sparrows: a new cost of dominance signalling?
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.
268
,
1337
-
1344
.
Bucher
T. L.
,
Ryan
M. J.
,
Bartholomew
G. A.
(
1982
).
Oxygen consumption during resting, calling, and nest building in the frog Physalaemus pustulosus
.
Physiol. Zool.
55
,
10
-
22
.
Careau
V.
,
Thomas
D.
,
Humphries
M. M.
,
Reale
D.
(
2008
).
Energy metabolism and animal personality
.
Oikos
117
,
641
-
653
.
Curtis
C. C.
,
Stoddard
P. K.
(
2003
).
Active mate choice in an electric fish Brachyhypopomus pinnicaudatus
.
Anim. Behav.
206
,
1353
-
1362
.
Cutolo
M.
,
Seriolo
B.
,
Villaggio
B.
,
Pizzorni
C.
,
Craviotto
C.
,
Sulli
A.
(
2002
).
Androgens and estrogens modulate the immune and inflammatory responses in rheumatoid arthritis
.
Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.
966
,
131
-
142
.
Deviche
P.
,
Cortez
L.
(
2005
).
Androgen control of immunocompetence in the male house finch, Carpodacus mexicanus Müller
.
J. Exp. Biol.
208
,
1287
-
1295
.
Eberhardt
L. S.
(
1994
).
Oxygen consumption during singing by male Carolina wrens (Thryothorus ludovicianus)
.
Auk
111
,
124
-
130
.
Endler
J. A.
,
Thery
M.
(
1996
).
Interacting effects of lek placement, display behavior, ambient light, and color patterns in three neotropical forest-dwelling birds
.
Am. Nat.
148
,
421
-
452
.
Fitzpatrick
S.
(
1998
).
Colour schemes for birds: structural coloration and signals of quality in feathers
.
Ann. Zool. Fenn.
35
,
67
-
77
.
Franchina
C. R.
,
Salazar
V. L.
,
Volmar
C. H.
,
Stoddard
P. K.
(
2001
).
Plasticity of the electric organ discharge waveform of male Brachyhypopomus pinnicaudatus. II. Social effects
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
187
,
45
-
52
.
Franz
M.
,
Goller
F.
(
2003
).
Respiratory patterns and oxygen consumption in singing zebra finches
.
J. Exp. Biol.
206
,
967
-
978
.
Garland
T.
Jr
,
Hayes
J. P.
,
Dohm
M. R.
(
1992
).
Individual variation in metabolism and reproduction of Mus: are energetics and life history linked?
Funct. Ecol.
6
,
5
-
14
.
Gerhardt
H. C.
(
2005
).
Acoustic spectral preferences in two cryptic species of grey treefrogs: implications for mate choice and sensory mechanisms
.
Anim. Behav.
70
,
39
-
48
.
Giora
J.
,
Malabarba
L. R.
(
2009
).
Brachyhypopomus gauderio, new species, a new example of underestimated species diversity of electric fishes in southern South America (Gymnotiformes: Hypopomidae)
.
Zootaxa
2093
,
60
-
68
.
Grafe
T. U.
,
Schmuck
R.
,
Linsenmair
K. E.
(
1992
).
Reproductive energetics of the African reed frogs, Hyperolius viridiflavus and Hyperolius marmoratus
.
Physiol. Zool.
65
,
153
-
171
.
Grafen
A.
(
1990
).
Biological signals as handicaps
.
J. Theor. Biol.
144
,
517
-
546
.
Hoback
W. W.
,
Wagner
W. E. J.
(
1997
).
The energetic cost of calling in the variable field cricket, Gryllus lineaticeps
.
Physiol. Entomol.
22
,
286
-
290
.
Horn
A. G.
,
Leonard
M. L.
,
Weary
D. M.
(
1995
).
Oxygen consumption during crowing by roosters: talk is cheap
.
Anim. Behav.
50
,
1171
-
1175
.
Julian
D.
,
Crampton
W. G. R.
,
Wohlgemuth
S. E.
,
Albert
J. S.
(
2003
).
Oxygen consumption in weakly electric neotropical fishes
.
Oecologia
137
,
502
-
511
.
Kavanagh
M. W.
(
1987
).
The efficiency of sound production in two cricket species, Gryllotalpa australis and Teleogryllus commodus (Orthoptera: Grylloidea)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
130
,
107
-
119
.
Kotiaho
J. S.
,
Alatalo
R. V.
,
Mappes
J.
,
Nielsen
M. G.
,
Parri
S.
,
Rivero
A.
(
1998
).
Energetic costs of size and sexual signalling in a wolf spider
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.
265
,
2203
-
2209
.
Lee
H. J.
,
Loher
W.
(
1993
).
The mating strategy of the male short-tailed cricket Anurogryllus muticus De Geer
.
Ethology
95
,
327
-
344
.
Lynn
S. E.
,
Houtman
A. M.
,
Weathers
W. W.
,
Ketterson
E. D.
,
Nolan
V.
(
2000
).
Testosterone increases activity but not daily energy expenditure in captive male dark-eyed juncos, Junco hyemalis
.
Anim. Behav.
60
,
581
-
587
.
Markham
M. R.
,
Stoddard
P. K.
(
2005
).
Adrenocorticotropic hormone enhances the masculinity of an electric communication signal by modulating the waveform and timing of action potentials within individual cells
.
J. Neurosci.
25
,
8746
-
8754
.
Markham
M. R.
,
McAnelly
M. L.
,
Stoddard
P. K.
,
Zakon
H. H.
(
2009
).
Circadian and social cues regulate ion channel trafficking
.
PLoS Biol.
7
,
e1000203
.
Maynard-Smith
J.
,
Harper
D.
(
2003
).
Animal Signals
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.
McGraw
K. J.
(
2006
).
Dietary mineral content influences the expression of melanin-based ornamental coloration
.
Behav. Ecol.
18
,
137
-
142
.
Prestwich
K. N.
,
Walker
T. J.
(
1981
).
Energetics of singing in crickets: effects of temperature in three trilling species (Orthoptera: Gryllidae)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
143
,
199
-
212
.
Prestwich
K. N.
,
Brugger
K. E.
,
Topping
M.
(
1989
).
Energy and communication in three species of hylid frogs: power input, power output and efficiency
.
J. Exp. Biol.
144
,
53
-
80
.
Reinhold
K.
,
Greenfield
M. D.
,
Jang
Y.
,
Broce
A.
(
1998
).
Energetic cost of sexual attractiveness: ultrasonic advertisement in wax moths
.
Anim. Behav.
55
,
905
-
913
.
Ritschard
M.
,
Riebel
K.
,
Brumm
H.
(
2010
).
Female zebra finches prefer high-amplitude song
.
Anim. Behav.
79
,
877
-
883
.
Rutowski
R. L.
,
Nahm
A. C.
,
Macedonia
J. M.
(
2010
).
Iridescent hindwing patches in the pipevine swallowtail: differences in dorsal and ventral surfaces relate to signal function and context
.
Funct. Ecol.
24
,
767
-
775
.
Salazar
V. L.
,
Stoddard
P. K.
(
2008
).
Sex differences in energetic costs explain sexual dimorphism in the circadian rhythm modulation of the electrocommunication signal of the gymnotiform fish Brachyhypopomus pinnicaudatus
.
J. Exp. Biol.
211
,
1012
-
1020
.
Searcy
W. A.
,
Nowicki
S.
(
2005
).
The Evolution of Animal Communication: Reliability and Deception in Signaling Systems
.
Princeton, NJ
:
Princeton University Press
.
Speakman
J. R.
(
1997
).
Doubly Labelled Water: Theory and Practice
.
London
:
Chapman and Hall
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Racey
P. A.
(
1991
).
No cost of echolocation for bats in flight
.
Nature
351
,
421
-
423
.
Speakman
J. R.
,
Anderson
M. E.
,
Racey
P. A.
(
1989
).
The energy-cost of echolocation in pipistrelle bats (Pipistrellus pipistrellus)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
165
,
679
-
685
.
Stevens
E. D.
,
Josephson
R. K.
(
1977
).
Metabolic rate and body temperature in singing katydids
.
Physiol. Zool.
50
,
31
-
42
.
Taigen
T. L.
,
Wells
K. D.
(
1985
).
Energetics of vocalization by an anuran amphibian (Hyla versicolor)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B
155
,
163
-
170
.
Veblen
T.
(
1899
).
The Theory of the Leisure Class, an Economic Study of Institutions
.
New York
:
MacMillan
.
Ward
S.
,
Lampe
H. M.
,
Slater
P. J. B.
(
2004
).
Singing is not energetically demanding for pied flycatchers, Ficedula hypoleuca
.
Behav. Ecol.
15
,
477
-
484
.
Ward
S.
,
Speakman
J. R.
,
Slater
P. J. B.
(
2003
).
The energy cost of song in the canary, Serinus canaria
.
Anim. Behav.
66
,
893
-
902
.
Wedekind
C.
,
Folstad
I.
(
1994
).
Adaptive or nonadaptive immunosuppression by sex-hormones
.
Am. Nat.
143
,
936
-
938
.
Wikelski
M.
,
Lynn
S.
,
Breuner
C.
,
Wingfield
J. C.
,
Kenagy
G. J.
(
1999
).
Energy metabolism, testosterone and corticosterone in white-crowned sparrows
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
185
,
463
-
470
.
Zahavi
A.
(
1975
).
Mate selection – selection for a handicap
.
J. Theor. Biol.
53
,
205
-
214
.
Zahavi
A.
(
1977
).
The cost of honesty (further remarks on the handicap principle)
.
J. Theor. Biol.
67
,
603
-
605
.
Zakon
H. H.
(
1996
).
Hormonal modulation of communication signals in electric fish
.
Dev. Neurosci.
18
,
115
-
123
.
Zakon
H.
(
2000
).
Sex steroids and weakly electric fish: a model system for activational mechanisms of hormone action
. In
Sexual Differentiation of the Brain
(ed.
Matsumoto
A.
), pp.
95
-
112
.
Boca Raton, FL
:
CRC Press
.