The lateral line system of fish and amphibians detects water flow with receptors on the surface of the body. Although differences in the shape of these receptors, called neuromasts, are known to influence their mechanics, it is unclear how neuromast morphology affects the sensitivity of the lateral line system. We examined the functional consequences of morphological variation by measuring the dimensions of superficial neuromasts in zebrafish larvae (Danio rerio) and mathematically modeling their mechanics. These measurements used a novel morphometric technique that recorded landmarks in three dimensions at a microscopic scale. The mathematical model predicted mechanical sensitivity as the ratio of neuromast deflection to flow velocity for a range of stimulus frequencies. These predictions suggest that variation in morphology within this species generates a greater than 30-fold range in the amplitude of sensitivity and more than a 200-fold range of variation in cut-off frequency. Most of this variation was generated by differences in neuromast height that do not correlate with body position. Our results suggest that natural variation in cupular height within a species is capable of generating large differences in their mechanical filtering and dynamic range.

The detection of water flow by the fish lateral line system influences behaviors as varied as spawning (Satou et al., 1994), obstacle detection(Hassan, 1986) and rheotaxis(Montgomery et al., 1997) (for reviews, see Bleckmann, 1994; Mogdans and Bleckmann, 2001; Coombs and van Netten, 2006). The lateral line detects flow with morphologically diverse receptors, called neuromasts, that are distributed over the surface of the body. Although it is clear that the shape of a neuromast greatly determines its mechanics(van Netten and Kroese, 1987)(McHenry et al., in press), it is unknown how natural variation in morphology affects how the lateral line detects flow. Therefore, we examined how neuromast morphology affects its mechanical sensitivity in zebrafish larvae (Danio rerio) by measurement and mathematical modeling.

Two types of neuromast can be distinguished by morphological differences. Superficial neuromasts are directly exposed to flow over the body and canal neuromasts are recessed within channels beneath the scales. Both types include a cluster of hair cells in the epithelium with kinocilia that extend into the water. The kinocilia and a surrounding gelatinous matrix form the cupula of the neuromast. The cupulae of canal neuromasts are generally hemispherical with a diameter of hundreds of micrometers. Superficial neuromasts have elongated cupulae that are an order of magnitude smaller in diameter(Münz, 1989)(Fig. 1). In both, water flow causes the cupula to deflect, which is transduced into graded receptor potentials in the hair cells by bundles of stereocilia that are linked to the kinocilia (Gillespie and Walker,2001). Therefore, the neurophysiological response of a neuromast depends on the degree to which cupular mechanics permit the deflection of the kinocilia in response to water flow. The relationship between cupular deflection and receptor potentials has been demonstrated in the ruffe(Acerina cernua L.), where physiological recordings of hair cell potentials closely matched recorded mechanical deflections up to 300Hz(Kroese and van Netten,1989).

Differences in the morphology of canal neuromasts create differences in mechanical sensitivity. Direct recordings of neuromast deflection using laser interferometry found that the frequency response of the canal neuromasts of the ruffe (Acerina cernua) exhibits a peak sensitivity around 116Hz,and the smaller neuromasts of the African knife fish (Xenomystus nigri) are most sensitive at 460 Hz(Wiersinga-Post and van Netten,2000). According to a mathematical model of their mechanics, this difference in peak sensitivity is due entirely to the discrepancy in cupula size (van Netten and Kroese,1987; Wiersinga-Post and van Netten, 2000).

A recently developed mathematical model examines the effects of morphology on the sensitivity of superficial neuromasts(McHenry et al., in press). This model treats the structure of the cupula as two beams joined end-to-end that are excited by a pressure-driven oscillatory boundary layer. It predicts the response of a neuromast by calculating the cupular deflections over a range of stimulus frequencies. This model suggests that the dimensions of the cupula dictate the generation of hydrodynamic forces and thereby affect the cupular deflections that determine neuromast sensitivity(McHenry et al., in press). The present study employs this model as a basis for interpreting how cupular morphology affects neuromast sensitivity in the superficial neuromasts of zebrafish larvae.

Zebrafish larvae are an excellent system for the study of superficial neuromasts. As in other species (Blaxter and Fuiman, 1989), the lateral line at the larval stage includes only a small number of neuromasts, which are similar to the superficial neuromasts of adult fish (Münz,1989; Webb and Shirey,2003). Additionally, almost all of the 31 neuromasts on each side of the body are easily visualized with transmitted illumination because of the transparent bodies of the larvae (McHenry and van Netten, 2007). Finally, the lateral line of zebrafish larvae has become a focus of investigation on vertebrate hair cell mechanotransduction (e.g. Sidi et al.,2003; Corey et al.,2004) and regeneration (e.g. Harris et al., 2003; Ma et al., 2008). Therefore,understanding the morphological basis of sensitivity in this model system has the potential to offer insight on the physiology of vertebrate hair cells.

Experimental preparation

Zebrafish larvae were raised with standard culturing techniques. A breeding colony of wild-type (AB line) zebrafish (Danio rerio, Hamilton 1922)was housed in a flow-through tank system (Aquatic Habitats, Apopka, FL, USA)that was maintained at 28.5°C on a 14 h: 10 h light–dark cycle. The fertilized eggs from randomized mating were cultured according to standard protocols (Westerfield, 1993)and larvae were raised in an incubator in E3 embryo media(Brand et al., 2002). Morphological measurements were made from the cupulae of neuromasts in larvae that were between 3 and 20 days post fertilization (d.p.f.). Larvae were anesthetized for these measurements in a solution of 0.0017 g l–1 MS-222 (tricaine methanesulfonate; Finquel Inc., Argent Chemical Laboratories Inc., Redmond, WA, USA) and embryo media buffered with Tris to a pH of ∼7. All measurements were conducted on live larvae, the health of which we assessed by monitoring blood flow throughout the study.

The results of a pilot study suggested that cupular morphology changes during hatching and immediately afterward. We therefore examined cupular morphology in two groups. In the first group, variation in cupular morphology was assessed in larvae between 5 and 20 d.p.f. In the second, we examined the consequences of hatching by comparing measurements between hatched larvae at 3 or 4 d.p.f. with unhatched larvae at 3 d.p.f. that were extracted by tearing open the chorion with forceps.

Morphometrics

Cupulae were visualized by coating their surface with polystyrene microspheres. A concentrated solution of these particles (0.1 μm in diameter; Polysciences Inc., Warrington, PA, USA) was injected by syringe around the body of an anesthetized larva. Once coated, the periphery of the cupular matrix was visible under differential interference contrast optics. This approach was recently developed(McHenry and van Netten, 2007)as a means of avoiding the shrinkage (Cahn and Shaw, 1962; Blaxter,1984a; Rouse and Pickles,1991; Higgs and Fuiman,1998) and destruction (Webb,1989; Webb and Shirey,2003; Carton and Montgomery,2004; Gibbs and Northcutt,2004; Faucher et al.,2006) of cupulae that had accompanied previous visualization techniques. After coating the cupulae, larvae were mounted in a 0.3% agarose solution in embryo media and 0.0017 g l–1 MS-222 within a deep-welled glass slide. Individual neuromasts were observed with a ×40 water-immersion objective with an additional stage of ×10 magnification. The location of each neuromast was determined using the conventions established by Harris and colleagues(Harris et al., 2003), which combined prior labeling practices for cranial(Raible and Kruse, 2000) and trunk (Metcalfe et al., 1985)neuromasts (Fig. 1).

The coordinates of morphological landmarks were measured with a custom-designed technique of 3D micromorphometrics. Neuromasts were visualized with a fixed-stage compound microscope (Zeiss Axioskop 2 FS plus, Carl Zeiss Microimaging Inc., Thornwood, NY, USA) mounted onto a translating base(MT-1078, Sutter Instrument Co., Novato, CA, USA). This setup allowed an investigator to locate neuromasts on the body of a stationary larva while the microscope was translated with three degrees of freedom (d.f.). Upon locating a neuromast, the locations of morphological landmarks were selected from photographs (640 pixels × 480 pixels, 8-bit monochromic; Fire-i Digital Board Camera, Unibrain Inc., San Ramon, CA, USA) of the microscope field of view (Fig. 2Ai). These landmarks were found in 3D space through the use of a custom-designed program in Matlab (v. 7.4 with video acquisition toolbox; Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA). This program prompts the user to select 2D coordinates of landmarks within the photographs. These coordinates are defined with respect to a local coordinate system (xlocal and ylocal) having an origin at the top of the circular field of view within the photograph(Fig. 2Aii,Aiii). For each photo, the program prompts the user for the focus setting on the microscope(zglobal) and the position of the origin of the local coordinate system from a reading of the micrometers that actuate the translation base of the microscope (xglobal and yglobal). These coordinates were used to find the position of photographs within a global frame of reference that was fixed with respect to the body of the larva. The program then calculated the 3D position of landmarks in the global frame of reference as the vector sum of the origin of the local system and the coordinates within the local system(Fig. 2Aiv). We defined the central axis of the body as a vector between the anterior tip of the rostrum and posterior margin of the tail fin (Fig. 2B). All coordinates were transformed with respect to this axis to calculate the body position of landmarks (see McHenry and Lauder, 2006).

Fig. 1.

Morphology of the lateral line system of zebrafish larvae. (A) Lateral and(B) dorsal views illustrate the distribution of neuromasts along the body,grouped by region (Harris et al.,2003). The supraorbital region (blue) includes the preoptic (PO)and supraorbital (SO) neuromasts. The infraorbital region (gold) includes the mandibular (M), infraorbital (IO) and opercular (OP) neuromasts. The caudal–cranial region (purple) includes the otic (O), occipital (OC),dorsal (D) and middle (MI) neuromasts. Finally, the posterior (P) neuromasts are located in the trunk region (green). (C) The morphology of an individual neuromast illustrates its major anatomical features. Four hair cells have been highlighted to clarify the major features of each cell. Scale bar, 10μm.

Fig. 1.

Morphology of the lateral line system of zebrafish larvae. (A) Lateral and(B) dorsal views illustrate the distribution of neuromasts along the body,grouped by region (Harris et al.,2003). The supraorbital region (blue) includes the preoptic (PO)and supraorbital (SO) neuromasts. The infraorbital region (gold) includes the mandibular (M), infraorbital (IO) and opercular (OP) neuromasts. The caudal–cranial region (purple) includes the otic (O), occipital (OC),dorsal (D) and middle (MI) neuromasts. Finally, the posterior (P) neuromasts are located in the trunk region (green). (C) The morphology of an individual neuromast illustrates its major anatomical features. Four hair cells have been highlighted to clarify the major features of each cell. Scale bar, 10μm.

We recorded the coordinates of as many as seven landmarks from each neuromast. Three landmarks defined the centerline of a cupula by recording the middle of the cupula at the base, the distal tip of the longest kinocilium and the distal tip of the cupular matrix. The margins of the cupula were recorded at its base (where the cupula joins the surface of the sensory hillock) and in the middle (radially outward from the distal tip of the longest kinocilium;see dots on Fig. 2Aiv). The height of the cupula (hc) and kinocilia(hk) was calculated from the distance between center points. The diameter of the cupula at its base (db) and at the kinocilia tips (dk) was calculated from the periphery of the cupula at these two heights (Fig. 2C). The accuracy of measurements (at the 95% level) was verified to 1 μm precision by performing the coordinate acquisition on micrometer scales of known length and variable orientation.

Mathematical modeling

We used a mathematical model to calculate the frequency responses of neuromasts from their cupular dimensions(Fig. 3). This model treats the stimulus as an oscillatory pressure field that generates a boundary layer of flow over the surface of a fish's body(McHenry et al., in press). If the body is modeled as a flat plate, the velocity U within the boundary layer varies with distance z normal to the surface, as described by the following equation(Batchelor, 1967):
\[\ U(z)=U_{{\infty}}\left[1-\mathrm{exp}\left(\frac{-z(1+i)}{{\delta}}\right)\right],\]
(1)
where δ is the boundary layer thickness[δ=(2μ/ρω)0.5], U is the freestream velocity, ω is the angular speed, and ρ and μ are the density and dynamic viscosity of freshwater, respectively. The model treats the cupula as two beams joined end-to-end. Assuming small deflections(<10% of cupula height), the motion of each beam may be calculated with the following general equation (McHenry et al., in press):
\[\ {\nu}(z)=-\frac{iU_{{\infty}}}{{\omega}}\left[1-\frac{b{\delta}^{4}}{4EI+b{\delta}^{4}}\mathrm{exp}\left(\frac{-(1+i)z}{{\delta}}\right)\right]+{{\sum}_{j=0}^{3}}C_{j}\mathrm{exp}\left(i^{j}z\sqrt[4]{\frac{b}{EI}}\right),\]
(2)
where bωπ(2ρa2ω–4μki–πμk/L), k=L/[L2+(π/4)2], L=γ+ln[a(2δ)–0.5], Eis Young's modulus, I is the second moment of area, Cj is a sequence of four integration constants, ais the radius of the beam and γ is Euler's constant. The general equation assumes that the density of the cupula is equal to that of the surrounding water.
The flexural stiffness of each part of the cupula was calculated from our morphological measurements and published values for material properties. Flexural stiffness is equal to the product of Young's modulus and the beam's second moment of area. For a cylinder, the second moment of area is calculated with the following equation (Gere,2001):
\[\ I=\frac{{\pi}}{4}a^{4}.\]
(3)
The region of the cupula distal to the kinocilia is composed entirely of matrix material. Therefore, the flexural stiffness of the distal beam was calculated as follows (Gere,2001):
\[\ (EI)_{\mathrm{dist}}=E_{\mathrm{matrix}}I_{\mathrm{dist}},\]
(4)
where (EI)dist and Idist are,respectively, the flexural stiffness and second moment of area for the distal cupula, and Ematrix is Young's modulus of the matrix[Ematrix is 21 Pa in D. rerio(McHenry and van Netten,2007)]. The flexural stiffness of the proximal region,(EI)prox, may be calculated by the following relationship:
\[\ (EI)_{\mathrm{prox}}=E_{\mathrm{matrix}}I_{\mathrm{prox}}+n(EI)_{\mathrm{kino}},\]
(5)
where (EI)kino is the flexural stiffness for an individual kinocilium [(EI)kino is 2.4×10–21Nm2 (McHenry and van Netten,2007)], n is the number of hair cells and Iprox is the second moment of area for the proximal cupula.
Fig. 2.

The method of 3D micromorphometrics. (A) In this set-up the larva is positioned beneath the water-immersion objective of a compound microscope. The microscope is free to translate in three dimensions to interrogate microscopic features within the specimen. (i–iv) The position of these features is measured with the aid of a custom-designed computer program that first (i)captures digital photographs of the microscope field of view. Each photograph captures morphology at a particular optical plane with the z-position determined by the microscope focus. (ii) The user selects landmarks from this image. Once the user has entered the position of the microscope objective in global coordinates, (iv) the 3D positions of landmarks are calculated (see Materials and methods for details). (B) These coordinates are described with respect to the central axis of the body (dashed line), which is defined by points at the rostrum and tail tip. (C) The seven landmarks from a neuromast were used to calculate the cupular height (hc), kinocilia height (hk), base diameter (db) and diameter at the kinocilia height (dk).

Fig. 2.

The method of 3D micromorphometrics. (A) In this set-up the larva is positioned beneath the water-immersion objective of a compound microscope. The microscope is free to translate in three dimensions to interrogate microscopic features within the specimen. (i–iv) The position of these features is measured with the aid of a custom-designed computer program that first (i)captures digital photographs of the microscope field of view. Each photograph captures morphology at a particular optical plane with the z-position determined by the microscope focus. (ii) The user selects landmarks from this image. Once the user has entered the position of the microscope objective in global coordinates, (iv) the 3D positions of landmarks are calculated (see Materials and methods for details). (B) These coordinates are described with respect to the central axis of the body (dashed line), which is defined by points at the rostrum and tail tip. (C) The seven landmarks from a neuromast were used to calculate the cupular height (hc), kinocilia height (hk), base diameter (db) and diameter at the kinocilia height (dk).

Fig. 3.

Mathematical modeling of the mechanics of superficial neuromasts. (A) The cupula of the neuromast is modeled as two beams joined end-to-end. The distal beam (light grey) is rigidly fixed to the proximal beam (dark grey), which is anchored to the body with a pinned joint and torsion spring with a stiffness equal to that of the hair bundles. This cupula is excited by a boundary layer of flow acting over the surface of the body, which is modeled as a flat plate. This model predicts the frequency response (B,C) of the sensitivity of cupular deflections to flow. (B) The amplitude of sensitivity(Eqn 7) was used to find the peak amplitude and cut-off frequency of the frequency response (see Materials and methods for details). (C) The phase of cupular sensitivity is defined with respect to the local flow velocity.

Fig. 3.

Mathematical modeling of the mechanics of superficial neuromasts. (A) The cupula of the neuromast is modeled as two beams joined end-to-end. The distal beam (light grey) is rigidly fixed to the proximal beam (dark grey), which is anchored to the body with a pinned joint and torsion spring with a stiffness equal to that of the hair bundles. This cupula is excited by a boundary layer of flow acting over the surface of the body, which is modeled as a flat plate. This model predicts the frequency response (B,C) of the sensitivity of cupular deflections to flow. (B) The amplitude of sensitivity(Eqn 7) was used to find the peak amplitude and cut-off frequency of the frequency response (see Materials and methods for details). (C) The phase of cupular sensitivity is defined with respect to the local flow velocity.

Specific solutions to this equation require definitions for the boundary conditions at the two ends of each beam within the cupula. At the tip of the distal beam, it may be assumed that zero bending moment
\([(EI)_{\mathrm{dist}}{\nu}_{\mathrm{dist}}^{{^{\prime\prime}}}(h_{\mathrm{c}})=0]\)
and shearing force
\([(EI)_{\mathrm{dist}}{\nu}_{\mathrm{dist}}^{{^{\prime\prime\prime}}}(h_{\mathrm{c}})=0]\)
are generated. At the junction between the beams, the two may be assumed to be equal in deflection[νprox(hk)=νdist(0)],orientation
\([{\nu}_{\mathrm{prox}}^{{^\prime}}(h_{\mathrm{k}})={\nu}_{\mathrm{dist}}^{{^\prime}}(0)]\)
,bending moment
\([(EI)_{1}{\nu}_{\mathrm{prox}}^{{^{\prime\prime}}}(h_{\mathrm{k}})=(EI)_{\mathrm{dist}}{\nu}_{\mathrm{dist}}^{{^{\prime\prime}}}(0)]\)
,and shear force
\([(EI)_{\mathrm{prox}}{\nu}_{\mathrm{prox}}^{{^{\prime\prime\prime}}}(h_{\mathrm{k}})=(EI)_{\mathrm{dist}}{\nu}_{\mathrm{dist}}^{{^{\prime\prime\prime}}}(0)]\)
(McHenry et al., in press). Finally, the cupula may be assumed to be pinned at the base[νprox(0)=0] with the hair bundles acting as a torsion spring that resists changes in orientation, as defined by:
\[\ {\nu}_{\mathrm{prox}}^{{^\prime}}(0)=\frac{(EI)_{\mathrm{prox}}{\nu}_{\mathrm{prox}}^{{^{\prime\prime}}}(0)}{nq_{\mathrm{t}}},\]
(6)
where n is the number of hair cells and qt is the hair bundle torsion stiffness [2.9×10–14 N m rad–1 in Acerina cernua(van Netten and Kroese,1987)]. We used the values of n reported by Harris and colleagues for each neuromast locus(Harris et al., 2003). These boundary conditions define eight simultaneous linear equations that were used to numerically solve for the four integration constants(Eqn 2) for each of the two beams. This calculation was performed within Matlab after defining all parameter values to yield a specific solution to the model.
We calculated a frequency response of cupular deflection from specific solutions to the model. Deflections were normalized by the stimulus intensity(i.e. flow velocity) to provide a measure of sensitivity. Therefore, the sensitivity S of a neuromast was calculated as:
\[\ S=\frac{{\nu}(h_{\mathrm{b}})}{U(h_{\mathrm{b}})},\]
(7)
where hb is the height of the hair bundles[hb is 5.2 μm in D. rerio(Dinklo, 2005)]. This measure of sensitivity is a complex number with a modulus equal to the amplitude(Fig. 3B) and a phase calculated from its argument as: 180°arg(S)/π(Fig. 3C). To find the frequency response, we calculated sensitivity from specific solutions to the model for hundreds of frequencies for 0.001 Hz<f<1000 Hz. For each frequency response, we calculated the cut-off frequency and peak amplitude from the relationship between frequency and the amplitude of sensitivity. This was achieved by first finding a least-squares linear curve fit in the 0.001 Hz<f<0.1 Hz range and a second line constrained to a slope of –20 dB decade–1 at 10 Hz<f<1000 Hz. Cut-off frequency was taken as the frequency of the intersection between these lines and peak amplitude was calculated as the amplitude of the intersection (Fig. 3B).

Statistics

Statistical tests were used to assess whether the morphology and predicted sensitivity of neuromasts varied with body position or age. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to determine whether morphological parameters, the peak amplitude of sensitivity and cut-off frequency were dependent upon either body position or age. Differences between neuromast locations were explored with a post-hoc analysis of location using the Bonferroni method to adjust for multiple comparisons (Sokal and Rohlf, 1995). This method conducts t-test pair-wise comparisons between each group in the ANOVA, adjusting the level of significance (α) such that α=0.05/K where K is the number of comparisons. When comparing larvae of different ages, mean values for each larva were used and groups (by day of development) were compared post-hoc using Tukey's least significant difference procedure (Sokal and Rohlf,1995). This procedure determines the minimum that is significant and determines whether each comparison exceeds that difference. Finally,coefficients of determination were calculated to assess the proportion of variability in frequency response explained by each morphological character. All statistical tests were preformed in Matlab (v. 7.4 with the statistics toolbox).

We successfully measured the morphology of nearly all neuromasts in the lateral line system. Measurements were conducted on a total of 495 neuromasts from 37 larvae. Some neuromasts were easier to locate than others, which created differences in sampling among loci(Fig. 4). Overall, a large number of measurements were generated for cupula height (N=386),kinocilia height (N=354), and the cupula diameter at the base(N=492) and kinocilia tips (N=274). The first two neuromasts of the infraorbital (IO) line were obscured by the eyes and consequently were the only cupulae for which there were no measurements.

Cupular morphology exhibited a broad range of variation among lateral line neuromasts. The values for cupula height spanned a 9-fold range (8.7 μm to 79.1 μm, Fig. 5A). Both kinocilia height (6.7 μm to 34.6μm, Fig. 5B) and cupula diameter(4.2μm to 25.1μm at kinocilia tips, Fig. 5C; 3.7 μm to 21.8μm at the base, Fig. 5D)spanned more than a 5-fold range. On average, the heights of cupulae (mean± 1 s.d., 40±14 μm; N=386) were around twice that of the kinocilia (mean ± 1 s.d., 20±4μm; N=354) and four times the cupular diameter at the base (mean ± 1 s.d., 11±3μm; N=492). The diameter at the kinocilia tips (mean ± 1 s.d., 11±3 μm; N=274) was not significantly different from the diameter at the cupular base (Student's unpaired t-test, P=0.056).

Fig. 4.

The frequency distribution of morphological measurements. The total number of measurements for each region of the (A) cranial and (B) trunk regions of the body. The color-coded regions of the lateral line system correspond to the neuromast locations illustrated in Fig. 1.

Fig. 4.

The frequency distribution of morphological measurements. The total number of measurements for each region of the (A) cranial and (B) trunk regions of the body. The color-coded regions of the lateral line system correspond to the neuromast locations illustrated in Fig. 1.

Under the assumptions of our model, variation in cupular morphology was predicted to create large differences in the frequency responses of neuromasts. Peak amplitude values spanned a 38-fold range(5.7×10–4 to 220×10–4; Fig. 5E) and cut-off frequencies spanned more than a 200-fold range (0.90 to 200 Hz) among all neuromasts (Fig. 5F). The form of these differences is revealed by the predicted frequency responses of neuromasts (Fig. 6). All neuromasts behaved as velocity detectors with low-pass filtering. They exhibited a nearly flat response (2 dB decade–1) in the amplitude of sensitivity to local flow velocity up to the cut-off frequency. Although the form of the frequency response is similar in all neuromasts,sensitivity varied greatly at low frequencies due to morphological differences(Fig. 6A). At the lowest frequencies, the near-zero phase of sensitivity indicates that the cupulae deflect nearly synchronously with the velocity of flow close to the body(Fig. 6A). However, the transition in phase with frequency differed broadly due to the influence of morphology on cut-off frequency. At frequencies above the cut-off, amplitude attenuates at a rate of 17 dB decade–1 and a phase lag around 75° emerges in all neuromasts, irrespective of morphological variation. Beyond 200 Hz, our model suggests that all neuromasts exhibited similar mechanical sensitivities. In total, we found that a large variation in mechanical response was predicted among neuromasts within individual fish(Fig. 6B) and at particular loci among individuals (Fig. 6C).

The results of our mathematical modeling suggest that cupular height is the dominant parameter in determining the frequency response of a neuromast. This result was formulated by first examining correlations between morphological parameters, cut-off frequency and peak amplitude (blue dots in Fig. 7). For each correlation,we considered the effect of the independent variable by running a series of simulations that differed only in values for the independent variable and maintained all other parameters at their mean measured values (green lines in Fig. 7). Finally, we calculated a coefficient of determination [r2(Sokal and Rolf, 1995)] that evaluated the proportion of variation in the correlations that were predicted by the independent variable. We found that most variation in peak amplitude(r2=0.76) and cut-off frequency(r2=0.90) may be attributed to variation in cupular height(Fig. 7A). Other parameters individually accounted for only as much as 15%(Fig. 7D) of the variation in peak amplitude and 8% of the variation in cut-off frequency(Fig. 7B,C).

Our findings suggest that variation in cupular morphology and frequency response is not related to the body position of a neuromast. Our ANOVA found that all morphological parameters and cut-off frequency, but not peak sensitivity, depended on neuromast location. ANOVA d.f. among locations was 28 for every parameter and ranged from 199 for peak sensitivity and 331 for cupula width at the base within locations. However, our post-hocanalysis (d.f. ranged from 0 for IO5 × P13 to 50 for MI1 × MI2 for cupula width at the base) found that the neuromasts at most loci were indistinguishable when significance levels were properly adjusted using the Bonferroni method. This is illustrated in Table 1 where two neuromasts that do not share a group are significantly different. For example, all but two neuromasts (SO1 and IO5) were indistinguishable by cupular height within a single group (`a' in hc, Table 1). The two neuromasts outside this group were not outliers in cupular height because they were still indistinguishable from most other neuromasts (groups `b' and `c' in hc, Table 1). A similar lack of distinction was found among all morphometric parameters, cut-off frequency and peak amplitude(Table 1). Values of peak amplitude were particularly homogeneous, as all loci were found to belong to the same statistical group.

Table 1.

Morphology and sensitivity of each neuromast

Morphology and sensitivity of each neuromast
Morphology and sensitivity of each neuromast
Fig. 5.

Morphological measurements and predicted frequency responses for different regions of the body. The distribution of data is shown for each locus with box and whisker plots. In each box, the center line represents the median value,the upper and lower bounds of the box represent the interquartile range, and the whiskers represent the total range. Outliers defined as exceeding 1.5 times the interquartile range are denoted by a plus sign. Data are shown for(A) cupula height (hc), (B) kinocilia height(hk), (C) cupula diameter at kinocilia tips(dk), (D) cupula diameter at its base(db), (E) peak amplitude and (F) cut-off frequency of the predicted frequency responses. Sample sizes and other statistics from these data are provided in Table 1.

Fig. 5.

Morphological measurements and predicted frequency responses for different regions of the body. The distribution of data is shown for each locus with box and whisker plots. In each box, the center line represents the median value,the upper and lower bounds of the box represent the interquartile range, and the whiskers represent the total range. Outliers defined as exceeding 1.5 times the interquartile range are denoted by a plus sign. Data are shown for(A) cupula height (hc), (B) kinocilia height(hk), (C) cupula diameter at kinocilia tips(dk), (D) cupula diameter at its base(db), (E) peak amplitude and (F) cut-off frequency of the predicted frequency responses. Sample sizes and other statistics from these data are provided in Table 1.

Cupulae exhibited significant differences in morphology and frequency response during the first days of larval development. Newly hatched larvae had shorter cupulae (Fig. 8A) and kinocilia (Fig. 8B) than unhatched larvae of the same age (3 d.p.f.). For all tested parameters ANOVA d.f. was 3 among age groups and 23 within age groups. The d.f. for our post-hoc analysis ranged from 6 for 4 d.p.f. × 3 d.p.f. and 16 for 3 d.p.f. × 5–20 d.p.f. We observed that the shorter cupulae of hatched larvae frequently exhibited an irregularly notched edge instead of the tapered tip that was common to longer cupulae. Among hatched larvae, long and tapered cupula were more common after the 1 or 2 days of growth that followed hatching. Cupulae recovered after hatching but never again achieved embryonic height values (Fig. 8A). Kinocilia, however, recovered to prehatching lengths within 1 day(Fig. 8B). The diameter of the cupula was not significantly different between hatched and prehatched larvae(Fig. 8C,D). Cbhanges in height were predicted to cause a significant increase in cut-off frequency and reduction in peak amplitude with hatching(Fig. 8E,F). Although cut-off frequency attained the prehatching level after hatching(Fig. 8F), peak sensitivity was higher in prehatching larvae than in any subsequent stage sampled(Fig. 8E).

Fig. 6.

The frequency responses modeled from morphological measurements. The predicted amplitude (upper panels) and phase (lower panels) of sensitivity are shown by transparent gray lines for all recorded neuromasts. Therefore dark regions of the drawn lines demonstrate a high degree of overlap in the frequency responses of neuromasts. (B) All neuromasts for a representative individual (11 d.p.f.) are highlighted (red lines) to demonstrate the degree of variation that may be exhibited within a larva. (C) Neuromasts at a particular locus (P8) are highlighted (red lines) for all individuals sampled.

Fig. 6.

The frequency responses modeled from morphological measurements. The predicted amplitude (upper panels) and phase (lower panels) of sensitivity are shown by transparent gray lines for all recorded neuromasts. Therefore dark regions of the drawn lines demonstrate a high degree of overlap in the frequency responses of neuromasts. (B) All neuromasts for a representative individual (11 d.p.f.) are highlighted (red lines) to demonstrate the degree of variation that may be exhibited within a larva. (C) Neuromasts at a particular locus (P8) are highlighted (red lines) for all individuals sampled.

Fig. 7.

The effects of morphological parameters on peak amplitude and cut-off frequency. In each plot, blue dots indicate predictions of peak amplitude(upper panels) and phase (lower panels) made from each neuromast measured. The green line shows the model predictions where only the independent variable is permitted to vary. The coefficient of determination (r2)was calculated from a comparison of these relationships to indicate the degree of variation that is caused by the independent variable. The independent variables examined were (A) cupula height (hc), (B)kinocilia height (hk), (C) cupula diameter at kinocilia tips (dk), (D) cupula diameter at its base(db) and (E) the number of kinocilia.

Fig. 7.

The effects of morphological parameters on peak amplitude and cut-off frequency. In each plot, blue dots indicate predictions of peak amplitude(upper panels) and phase (lower panels) made from each neuromast measured. The green line shows the model predictions where only the independent variable is permitted to vary. The coefficient of determination (r2)was calculated from a comparison of these relationships to indicate the degree of variation that is caused by the independent variable. The independent variables examined were (A) cupula height (hc), (B)kinocilia height (hk), (C) cupula diameter at kinocilia tips (dk), (D) cupula diameter at its base(db) and (E) the number of kinocilia.

Fig. 8.

Differences in cupular morphology and frequency response for larvae of different ages. In each box, the center line represents the median value, the upper and lower bounds of the box represent the interquartile range, and the whiskers indicate the total range. Outliers defined as exceeding 1.5 times the interquartile range are denoted by a plus sign. Measurements for (A) cupula height (hc), (B) kinocilia height(hk), (C) cupula diameter at kinocilia tips(dk), (D) cupula diameter at its base(db), (E) peak sensitivity and (F) cut-off frequency are presented for multiple individuals. These data are shown for larvae at 3 d.p.f. both (i) prior to (N=5) and (ii) after hatching(N=5), and at (iii) 4 d.p.f. (N=3) and (iv) 5–20 d.p.f. (N=13) using the mean values among neuromasts for individual larvae. The letters (a, b or c) indicate statistical groups as determined by one-way ANOVA with a post-hoc comparison using Tukey's least significant difference procedure such that two ages must not share any statistical groups to be considered statistically different.

Fig. 8.

Differences in cupular morphology and frequency response for larvae of different ages. In each box, the center line represents the median value, the upper and lower bounds of the box represent the interquartile range, and the whiskers indicate the total range. Outliers defined as exceeding 1.5 times the interquartile range are denoted by a plus sign. Measurements for (A) cupula height (hc), (B) kinocilia height(hk), (C) cupula diameter at kinocilia tips(dk), (D) cupula diameter at its base(db), (E) peak sensitivity and (F) cut-off frequency are presented for multiple individuals. These data are shown for larvae at 3 d.p.f. both (i) prior to (N=5) and (ii) after hatching(N=5), and at (iii) 4 d.p.f. (N=3) and (iv) 5–20 d.p.f. (N=13) using the mean values among neuromasts for individual larvae. The letters (a, b or c) indicate statistical groups as determined by one-way ANOVA with a post-hoc comparison using Tukey's least significant difference procedure such that two ages must not share any statistical groups to be considered statistically different.

Our results suggest that the sensitivity of lateral line neuromasts is greatly affected by the height of the cupulae. In zebrafish larvae, height is the most variable aspect of cupular morphology(Fig. 5) and it has the largest effect on frequency response (McHenry et al, in press). Under the assumptions of our modeling, these factors cause cupular height to generate 90% of the variation in cut-off frequency and 76% of the variation in peak amplitude among neuromasts(Fig. 7A). This suggests that cupular height is the major determinant of mechanical sensitivity in the lateral line system of zebrafish larvae.

These results are consistent with previous research on Mexican blind cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus). Teyke(Teyke, 1990) proposed that the taller cupulae (up to 300μm) of the blind morphotype creates a lateral line system with heightened sensitivity compared with that of their sighted relatives (up to 42μm). Greater height presents more surface area and exposes the neuromast to more rapid flow to create larger bending moments at the hair bundles (Teyke, 1990)(McHenry et al., in press). This probably contributes to the blind morphotype's ability to distinguish stationary surfaces without the aid of touch or sight(Weissert and von Campenhausen,1981; von Campenhausen et al.,1981). Therefore, the morphology and behavior of Mexican blind cavefish is consistent with our prediction that lateral line sensitivity is modulated by cupular height.

Our measurements provide indirect evidence that cupular height varies greatly because they are frequently damaged. The cupulae of larvae that hatched through the chorion were about half the height of unhatched larvae(Fig. 8A). The distal margins of many cupulae possessed irregularly notched edges in post-hatched larvae that contrasted with the smooth edges of cupulae from unhatched larvae. The notched edges provide evidence of damage from breaking through the chorion during hatching, as found in another cyprinid species (Gnathopogon elongates) (Mukai, 1995)and in herring (Clupea harengus)(Blaxter and Fuiman, 1989). We found that larvae began to recover their cupulae immediately following hatching, but never attained the height of prehatching fish(Fig. 8). If larvae persistently secrete the mucopolysaccharide material that composes the matrix(Blaxter, 1984a; Blaxter, 1984b; Mukai and Kobayashi, 1992),then cupular height may be regulated by the continuous growth and wear of the delicate cupular matrix (McHenry and van Netten, 2007). This suggests that damage caused by incidental contact with the environment and hydrodynamic forces may create high variability in cupular height throughout the larval stage (Figs 5 and 8).

We found that the large variation in cupular height does not follow a consistent pattern with body position (Fig. 5, Table 1). In contrast with reports on other species, this lack of a morphological pattern suggests that no region of the body is consistently more sensitive than the others (Fig. 5F). The cupulae of both adult Mexican blind cavefish [A. mexicanus(Teyke, 1990)] and larval glass knife fish [Eigenmannia sp.(Vischer, 1989)] are taller at anterior body positions. This pattern suggests the cranial region of the fish is more sensitive than the trunk. Given the few species for which cupular height has been reported, it is unclear whether sensitivity in superficial neuromasts typically varies with body position among fishes.

Our predictions of frequency response have implications for the dynamic range of a neuromast. The range of stimulus intensities that a neuromast may detect is dictated by the physiology of its hair cells. For a weak stimulus,detection requires that the kinocilia of the hair cells exceed a deflection threshold. At high intensities, the hair cells may saturate if the kinocilia deflections are too great (Hudspeth,1989). For intensities within these extremes, the relationship between deflection and the receptor potentials of a neuromast is anticipated to reflect the sigmoidal curve (Fig. 9) that is characteristic of individual hair cells(Hudspeth and Corey, 1977). The dynamic range of a neuromast may be defined as the range of flow velocities over which differences in velocity may be detected. As demonstrated in canal neuromasts (Kroese and van Netten, 1989), dynamic range is largely determined by cupular mechanics. These mechanics dictate how much the kinocilia within a neuromast deflect for a given flow stimulus. Our results suggest that the greater mechanical sensitivity of tall superficial neuromasts creates a smaller dynamic range.

Fig. 9.

The proposed effect of variation in cupular height on the dynamic range of the lateral line system. (A) A region of the body encompassing three neuromasts is focused on (box) for a comparison of responses for a lateral line with cupulae of (Bii,Cii) variable height and (Bi,Ci) uniform height.(Bi,Bii) An oscillatory stimulus (blue arrow) causes greater deflection in neuromasts with taller cupulae. (Cii) The tallest and most sensitive neuromast(blue line) is anticipated to produce a transducer potential that saturates at a relatively low flow velocity. The gray lines indicate sensitivity that is dominated by the mechanics of the cupula. In contrast to the tallest cupula,the shortest cupula (green line) is less sensitive, but encodes flow at higher velocity. Therefore, the dynamic range of the entire system (gray region) is large compared with that of a lateral line system composed of neuromasts with uniform morphology (Ci). (Bi,Ci) Neuromasts having similar cupular height will deflect to the same degree and produce similar transducer potentials. As a consequence, the dynamic range for the system will be relatively narrow.

Fig. 9.

The proposed effect of variation in cupular height on the dynamic range of the lateral line system. (A) A region of the body encompassing three neuromasts is focused on (box) for a comparison of responses for a lateral line with cupulae of (Bii,Cii) variable height and (Bi,Ci) uniform height.(Bi,Bii) An oscillatory stimulus (blue arrow) causes greater deflection in neuromasts with taller cupulae. (Cii) The tallest and most sensitive neuromast(blue line) is anticipated to produce a transducer potential that saturates at a relatively low flow velocity. The gray lines indicate sensitivity that is dominated by the mechanics of the cupula. In contrast to the tallest cupula,the shortest cupula (green line) is less sensitive, but encodes flow at higher velocity. Therefore, the dynamic range of the entire system (gray region) is large compared with that of a lateral line system composed of neuromasts with uniform morphology (Ci). (Bi,Ci) Neuromasts having similar cupular height will deflect to the same degree and produce similar transducer potentials. As a consequence, the dynamic range for the system will be relatively narrow.

The inverse relationship between cupular height and dynamic range has implications for the flow velocities that may be detected by the lateral line system. The dynamic range for the entire system may be defined as the range of intensities that can be detected among all neuromasts. Variable cupular morphology should generate a wide range of mechanical sensitivities among the neuromasts and thereby cause the system to be sensitive to a broad range of flow velocities (Fig. 9). Tall cupulae in the system would provide high sensitivity, but saturate at relatively low flow velocities. Short cupulae would be relatively insensitive,but encode stimuli of high intensity. Therefore, the dynamic range of a system of variable neuromast morphologies (Fig. 9Bii,Cii) may be much greater than would be possible if all neuromasts were uniform (Fig. 9Bi,Ci).

The role of neuromast morphology in flow sensing is mediated by the neurophysiology of the lateral line system. The receptor potentials generated by neuromast hair cells are encoded as a train of action potentials within afferent neurons (Dijkgraaf,1963). This encoding and its integration at the central nervous system filters signals beyond the mechanical filtering of the neuromasts. Integration begins in the afferent neurons, which may innervate multiple neuromasts within a section of the lateral line(Teyke, 1990; Ledent, 2002) and thereby average the responses of a group of receptors. Our findings suggest that an afferent neuron that innervates a group of neuromasts with taller cupulae will be more sensitive, and have a lower cut-off frequency and a smaller dynamic range than a neuron that innervates a group of short neuromasts. It is unlikely that such differences between neurons exist in zebrafish larvae because cupular heights do not correlate with body position(Fig. 5). The averaging of inputs by afferent neurons may help to explain why the frequency responses of afferent nerves are similar in some species despite variation in neuromast morphology (Coombs and Montgomery,1992; Coombs and Montgomery,1994; Montgomery et al.,1994).

Our findings suggest that the lateral line system of larval fish serves a functional role that is distinct from that of adults. Adult fish are large compared with prey that function as a stimulus source. This difference in scale allows adult fish to detect spatial patterns in flow along their body. It is thought that adult fish are capable of sensing stimulus direction and proximity by analyzing sensory cues from spatial variation in pressure gradients along the trunk (Coombs and Conley, 1997; Ćurčič-Blake and van Netten, 2006). Larvae, however, are much smaller than the flow generated by predators (Higham et al.,2006) and are therefore capable of sampling only a small portion of spatial gradients in flow. Zebrafish larvae are anticipated to have further difficulty in sensing flow gradients because of the high variability in the frequency responses predicted among neuromasts(Fig. 6). The variability in neuromast sensitivity that may assist in detecting a wide range of flow velocities (Fig. 9) may also hinder an ability to sense spatial cues. Therefore, the central nervous system of fish may process flow signals differently at different stages of their life history as a consequence of changes in body size relative to stimuli.

Katherine Yip assisted with data collection throughout the study. James Strother and members of the UCI biomechanics group provided thoughtful comments and insight on the project and earlier versions of this manuscript. This research was supported by National Science Foundation grants to M.J.M.(IOS-0723288 and IOB-0509740).

Batchelor, G. K. (
1967
).
An Introduction to Fluid Dynamics
. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Blaxter, J. H. S. (
1984a
). Cupular growth in herring neuromasts.
J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. U.K.
64
,
935
-938.
Blaxter, J. H. S. (
1984b
). Neuromasts and cupular growth of cod larvae.
Flodevigen Rapportserie
1
,
183
-188.
Blaxter, J. H. S. and Fuiman, L. A. (
1989
). Function of the free neuromasts of marine teleost larvae. In
The Mechanosensory Lateral Line: Neurobiology and Evolution
(ed. S. Coombs, P. Görner and H. Münz), pp.
481
-499. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Bleckmann, H. (
1994
). Reception of hydrodynamic stimuli in aquatic and semiaquatic animals.
Prog. Zool.
41
,
1
-115.
Brand, M., Granato, M. and Nuesslein-Volhard, C.(
2002
). Keeping and raising zebrafish. In
Zebrafish
(ed. C. Nusslein-Volhard and R. Dahm), pp.
7
-38. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cahn, P. H. and Shaw, E. (
1962
). The first demonstration of lateral line cupulae in the mugiliformes.
Copeia
1962
,
109
-114.
Carton, A. G. and Montgomery, J. C. (
2004
). A comparison of lateral line morphology of blue cod and torrentfish: two sandperches of the family Pinguipedidae.
Environ. Biol. Fishes
70
,
123
-131.
Coombs, S. and Conley, R. A. (
1997
). Dipole source localization by mottled sculpin. 1. Approach strategies.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
180
,
387
-399.
Coombs, S. and Montgomery, J. (
1992
). Fibers innervating different parts of the lateral line system of antarctic notothenioid, Trematomus bernacchii, have similar frequency responses, despite large variation in the peripheral morphology.
Brain Behav. Evol.
40
,
217
-233.
Coombs, S. and Montgomery, J. (
1994
). Function and evolution of superficial neuromasts in Antarctic notothenoid fish.
Brain Behav. Evol.
44
,
287
-298.
Coombs, S. and van Netten, S. M. (
2006
). The hydrodynamics and structural mechanics of the lateral line system. In
Fish Biomechanics
(ed. R. E. Shadwick and G. V. Lauder), pp.
103
-139. New York: Elsevier.
Corey, D. P., Garcia-Anoveros, J., Holt, J. R., Kwan, K. Y.,Lin, S. Y., Vollrath, M. A., Amalfitano, A., Cheung, E. L. M., Derfler, B. H.,Duggan, A. et al. (
2004
). TRPA1 is a candidate for the mechanosensitive transduction channel of vertebrate hair cells.
Nature
432
,
723
-730.
Ćurčič-Blake, B. and van Netten, S. M.(
2006
). Source location encoding in the fish lateral line canal.
J. Exp. Biol.
209
,
1548
-1559.
Dijkgraaf, S. (
1963
). The functioning and significance of the lateral-line organs.
Biol. Rev.
38
,
51
-105.
Dinklo, T. (
2005
).
Mechano- and electrophysiological studies on cochlear hair cells and superficial lateral line cupulae
. PhD thesis, University of Groningen, The Netherlands.
Faucher, K., Fichet, D., Miramand, P. and Lagardere, J. P.(
2006
). Impact of acute cadmium exposure on the trunk lateral line neuromasts and consequences on the “C-start” response behaviour of the sea bass (Dicentrarchus labrax L.; Teleostei,Moronidae).
Aquat. Toxical.
76
,
278
-294.
Gere, J. M. (
2001
).
Mechanics of Materials
. Cheltenham: Nelson Thornes.
Gibbs, M. A. and Northcutt, R. G. (
2004
). Development of the lateral line system in the shovelnose sturgeon.
Brain Behav. Evol.
64
,
70
-84.
Gillespie, P. G. and Walker, R. G. (
2001
). Molecular basis of mechanosensory transduction.
Nature
413
,
194
-202.
Harris, J. A., Cheng, A. G., Cunningham, L. L., MacDonald, G.,Raible, D. W. and Rubel, E. W. (
2003
). Neomycin-induced hair cell death and rapid regeneration in the lateral line of zebrafish (Danio rerio).
J. Assoc. Res. Otolaryngol.
4
,
219
-234.
Hassan, E. S. (
1986
). On the discrimination of spatial intervals by the blind cave fish (Anoptichthys jordani).
J. Comp. Physiol. A
159
,
701
-710.
Higgs, D. M. and Fuiman, L. A. (
1998
). Associations between sensory development and ecology in three species of clupeoid fish.
Copeia
1
,
133
-144.
Higham, T. E., Day, S. W. and Wainwright, P. C.(
2006
). Multidimensional analysis of suction feeding performance in fishes: fluid speed, acceleration, strike accuracy and the ingested volume of water.
J. Exp. Biol.
209
,
2713
-2725.
Hudspeth, A. J. (
1989
). How the ear's works work.
Nature
341
,
397
-404.
Hudspeth, A. J. and Corey, D. P. (
1977
). Sensitivity, polarity, and conductance change in the response of vertebrate hair cells to controlled mechanical stimuli.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
74
,
2407
-2411.
Kroese, A. B. A. and van Netten, S. M. (
1989
). Sensory transduction in lateral line hair cells. In
The Mechanosensory Lateral Line: Neurobiology and Evolution
(ed. S. Coombs, P. Gorner and H. Munz), pp.
265
-284. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Ledent, V. (
2002
). Postembryonic development of the posterior lateral line in zebrafish.
Development
129
,
597
-604.
Ma, E. Y., Rubel, E. W. and Raible, E. W.(
2008
). Notch signaling regulates the extent of hair cell regeneration in the zebrafish lateral line.
J. Neurosci.
28
,
2261
-2273.
McHenry, M. J. and Lauder, G. V. (
2006
). Ontogeny in form and function: locomotor morphology and drag in zebrafish(Danio rerio).
J. Morphol.
267
,
1099
-1109.
McHenry, M. J. and van Netten, S. M. (
2007
). The flexural stiffness of superficial neuromasts in the zebrafish (Danio rerio) lateral line.
J. Exp. Biol.
210
,
4244
-4253.
McHenry, M. J., Strother, J. A. and van Netten, S. M.(
2008
). Mechanical filtering by the boundary layer and fluid-structure interaction in the superficial neuromast of the fish lateral line system.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
(in press).
Metcalfe, W. K., Kimmel, C. B. and Schabtach, E.(
1985
). Anatomy of the posterior lateral line system in young larvae of the zebrafish.
J. Comp. Neurol.
233
,
377
-389.
Mogdans, J. and Bleckmann, H. (
2001
). The mechnosensory lateral line of jawed fishes. In
Sensory Biology of Jawed Fishes
(ed. B. G. Kapoor and T. J. Hara), pp.
181
-213. Enfield, NH: Science Publishers.
Montgomery, J., Coombs, S. and Janssen, J.(
1994
). Form and function relationships in lateral line systems:comparative data from six species of Antarctic notothenioid fish.
Brain Behav. Evol.
44
,
299
-306.
Montgomery, J. C., Baker, C. F. and Carton, A. G.(
1997
). The lateral line can mediate rheotaxis in fish.
Nature
389
,
960
-963.
Mukai, Y. (
1995
). Cupular growth of embryonic free neuromasts in the Willow Shiner Gnathopogon Elongatus Caerulescens and the pattern in the changes in cupular length after hatching.
Fish. Sci.
61
,
521
-522.
Mukai, Y. and Kobayashi, H. (
1992
). Cupular growth-rate of free neuromasts in 3 species of cyprinid fish.
Nippon Suisan Gakkai Shi
58
,
1849
-1853.
Münz, H. (
1989
). Functional organization of the lateral line periphery. In
The Mechanosensory Lateral Line
(ed. S. Coombs, P. Görner and H. Münz), pp.
285
-298. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Raible, D. W. and Kruse, G. J. (
2000
). Organization of the lateral line system in embryonic zebrafish.
J. Comp. Neurol.
421
,
189
-198.
Rouse, G. W. and Pickles, J. O. (
1991
). Ultrastructure of free neuromasts of Bathygobius fuscus (Gobiidea)and canal neuromasts of Apogon cyanosoma (Apogonidea).
J. Morphol.
209
,
111
-120.
Satou, M., Takeuchi, H. A., Tanabe, M., Kitamura, S., Okumoto,N., Iwata, M. and Nishii, J. (
1994
). Behavioral and electrophysiological evidences that the lateral-line is involved in the inter-sexual vibrational communication of the hime salmon (landlocked red salmon, Oncorhynchus-nerka).
J. Comp. Physiol. A
174
,
539
-549.
Sidi, S., Friedrich, R. W. and Nicolson, T.(
2003
). NompC TRP channel required for vertebrate sensory hair cell mechanotransduction.
Science
301
,
96
-99.
Sokal, R. R. and Rohlf, F. J. (
1995
).
Biometry
. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Teyke, T. (
1990
). Morphological differences in neuromasts of the blind cave fish Astyanax hubbsi and the sighted river fish Astyanax mexicanus.
Brain Behav. Evol.
35
,
23
-30.
van Netten, S. M. and Kroese, A. B. A. (
1987
). Laser interferometric measurements on the dynamic behavior of the cupula in the fish lateral line.
Hear. Res.
29
,
55
-61.
Vischer, H. A. (
1989
). The development of lateral-line receptors in Eigenmannia (Teleostei, Gymnotiformes). 1. The mechanoreceptive lateral-line system.
Brain Behav. Evol.
33
,
205
-222.
von Campenhausen, C., Reiss, I. and Weissert, R.(
1981
). Detection of stationary objects by the blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani (Characidae).
J. Comp. Physiol. A
143
,
369
-374.
Webb, J. F. (
1989
). Neuromast morphology and lateral line trunk canal ontogeny in two species of cichlids: an SEM study.
J. Morphol.
202
,
53
-68.
Webb, J. F. and Shirey, J. E. (
2003
). Postembryonic development of the cranial lateral line canals and neuromasts in zebrafish.
Dev. Dyn.
228
,
370
-385.
Weissert, R. and von Campenhausen, C. (
1981
). Discrimination between stationary objects by the blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani (Characidae).
J. Comp. Physiol. A
143
,
375
-381.
Westerfield, M. (
1993
).
The zebrafish Book: A Guide for the Laboratory use of Zebrafish (Brachydanio rerio)
. Eugene, OR: University of Oregon Press.
Wiersinga-Post, J. E. C. and van Netten, S. M.(
2000
). Temperature dependency of cupular mechanics and hair cell frequency selectivity in the fish canal lateral line organ.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
186
,
949
-956.