The visual plankivorous feeding behaviour of the shiner perch(Cymatogaster aggregata) was investigated by means of a flow tank operated at various current speeds. Artemia salina was used as prey. In a second set of experiments, Artemia was darkened with black ink,to compare the visually mediated behaviour of C. aggregata while feeding on dark prey vs feeding on natural (i.e. semi-transparent)prey. The positions of the fish and its prey at the time of the feeding reaction of C. aggregata were measured in three dimensions. Prey were on average closer and more in line with the fish's axis when feeding reactions to darkened Artemia were considered, in comparison with natural Artemia. Three potential mechanisms triggering the feeding reaction of C. aggregata were explored: the prey may trigger a reaction in C. aggregata when it reaches a threshold (1) angular size, (2)angular velocity, or (3) rate of change of the angular size (i.e. loom) of the prey as it is carried passively by the current towards the fish. Our results show that angular velocity may trigger the fish's reaction when using semi-transparent prey, while loom may trigger the reaction to darkened prey. This suggests that feeding behaviour of planktivorous fish is flexible and can use different cues to trigger a motor reaction to prey with different visual characteristics. The feeding reaction appeared to occur at longer distances for semi-transparent rather than darkened Artemia. We suggest that semi-transparent Artemia were visible at greater distances because of their higher scattering (i.e. diffuse reflectance) that made them appear brighter when viewed against a dark background.

High sensitivity to motion across the visual field is characteristic of many visual systems. For example, in many vertebrates, motion detection cells in the brain can have directional sensitivity and maximal responses to stimuli moving at different speeds (Barlow and Hill, 1963; Cronly-Dillon,1964; Jacobson and Gaze,1964; Guthrie and Banks,1974; Kawasaki and Aoki,1983). In addition, the ability to detect a lower contrast stimulus (i.e. the modulation sensitivity), usually occurs at intermediate presentation frequencies or velocities(Kelly, 1972; Sekuler et al., 1978; Sekuler et al., 1990). The detection of predator or prey by fishes, although it may involve characteristics such as shape, spatial pattern and chromatic aspects,certainly responds dramatically to targets in motion. A central part of any visual stimulus must be the contrast of the target compared with the radiance of the background space light. Because zooplankton are small and usually semi-transparent, in general, they appear as low-contrast objects under viewing conditions that minimize the effects of scatter and reflection of downwelling light into the direction of view. However, near the surface where the downwelling irradiance is high relative to the horizontal irradiance,semi-transparent plankters will scatter downwelling light from their external and internal refractive interfaces and appear as bright targets, as long as the viewing distance is relatively short and the downwelling irradiance is not diffuse (i.e. the plankters are irradiated by the solar beam) (E. R. Loew and W.N.McF., unpublished). With depth, or as the veiling brightness increases with an increase in viewing distance, this phenomenon becomes less obvious. Thus, the contrast of a semi-transparent object when viewed horizontally depends on the veiling brightness, the viewing distance, the depth, the characteristics of the downwelling irradiance (diffuse vs beam) and the differences in refractive index between any intra-organismal compartments(Videen and Ngo, 1998).

Planktivorous fishes that feed in currents provide an example where the motion between a fish and its zooplankter prey are consistent and definable(Hobson, 1972; Hamner et al., 1988). By feeding zooplanktivourous fishes in flumes, Kiflawi and Genin(1997) demonstrated that peak ingestion can occur at intermediate current speeds and McFarland and Levin(2002) showed that individuals will cease feeding at higher speeds. Four phenomena may chronologically occur before a fish reacts to a particle: (a) detection, (b) recognition (c)decision (i.e. threshold of triggering mechanism at the sensory system level)and (d) motor response. Behaviourally, only the feeding reaction can be observed (i.e. the motor response). The timing of the feeding reaction should be, however, largely dependent on the timing of the detection. In particular,the distance at which a feeding reaction is triggered is limited by the maximum detection distance.

In investigating predator–prey interactions, Dill(1974) measured the escape responses of prey and modelled the rate of change in apparent size of a predator (i.e. the loom) as it approached the prey. For fish feeding on plankters in a current, angular velocity, apparent size and loom were described mathematically for particles that approached head-on and at various distances offset from the position of the fish in the current(McFarland and Levin 2002). In that study, the temporal-resolution of a fish's reaction to an incoming plankter was limited by the 2D spatial resolution and by the low temporal resolution of the system used to capture time-sequence images for analysis(McFarland and Levin 2002). In this paper, using a high-speed video system, we examine the reaction of fish under similar conditions and evaluate, in three dimensions, what attribute(s)of a plankter appear to influence the feeding reaction of fish. We test three alternative hypotheses: (1) The feeding reaction of planktivourous fish may be triggered by a fixed apparent size (i.e. angular size) of the approaching particle. (2) Fish may react to plankton once it reaches a given threshold angular velocity as it is carried passively by the current. (3) The mechanism triggering a fish's reaction may be the loom (i.e. the rate of change of the angular size) of the prey while approaching the fish. The experiments were carried out using three different current speeds and two treatments, which provided plankton with different properties of contrast (i.e. semi-transparent and darkened Artemia) in order to test the effect of contrast on detection mechanisms.

Fish collection and maintenance

Shiner perch (Cymatogaster aggregata Gibbons) were collected using a beach seine at Jackson Beach, San Juan Island Washington, USA, during August of 2000 (total length 6.7±0.08 cm; mean±s.e.m., N=8), and April 2002 (total length 8.8±0.17 cm; N=8). Fish were held at the Friday Harbor Laboratories in flow-through seawater tanks at 12°C for one week before use. Fish were exposed to a 12h:12h light:dark cycle, provided through fluorescent illumination from above. Integrated irradiance was 5.1×1017 photons cm–2 s–1 over 300–750 nm (OCEAN OPTICS S1000 spectroradiometer; Dunedin, FL, USA).

Apparatus

Experiments were carried out in a flume tank as described in McFarland and Levin (2002) with a working section 50 cm long, 12 cm wide, and a water depth of 17 cm(Fig. 1). The experimental tank was illuminated from above with a daylight fluorescent lamp (two 20 W lights,model F20T12 GE). Integrated irradiance was 8.2×1017 photons cm–2 s–1 over 300–750 nm, measured at the water surface level of the experimental tank (OCEAN OPTICS S1000 spectroradiometer).

Fig. 1.

Flume tank used in the experiments seen from the side (as viewed by the camera). (A) working section, (B) collimators, (C) mirror at 45°,providing top view of the fish, (D) pump, (E) Catch screen. The shaded rectangular section indicates the approximate field of view of the camera.

Fig. 1.

Flume tank used in the experiments seen from the side (as viewed by the camera). (A) working section, (B) collimators, (C) mirror at 45°,providing top view of the fish, (D) pump, (E) Catch screen. The shaded rectangular section indicates the approximate field of view of the camera.

Water flow was generated by a two-bladed propeller and current speed controlled by varying the voltage via a variac to the AC/DC motor(Dayton AC/DC model 2MO338 motor; Lincolnshire, IL, USA). A catch screen was placed 10 cmdownstream from the working section to isolate fish in the flume. To assure repeatability of current speed for different trials, the voltage output of the variac was prescribed through the use of a digital voltmeter. Three layers of collimators (5 cm wide) were positioned upstream to produce a homogeneous flow through the working section. Viewed through the collimators,the upstream aperture of the collimator tubes provided a dark background to the fish. The background radiance from the collimator, as measured with an OCEAN OPTICS S1000 spectroradiometer between 300 and 750 nm, was only 3.1%when compared with the radiance from a white Teflon reflective surface and was similar to the radiance from a black surface (4.3%). Flow speed was calibrated from dye injected in the water upstream from the collimators and filmed at 125 images s–1 using a high-speed video camera (Red Lake, model 1000 S series motionscope; San Diego, CA, USA). The current flow as seen by discrete dye travel in the horizontal dimension was laminar over the working section, with minor turbulence only observed just upstream from the catch screen. To avoid wall effects, only trials in which a fish initiated a reaction towards the prey more than 2 cm from the walls, bottom and water surface, were analysed.

Fish movements were recorded at 125 images s–1 with the video camera placed perpendicular to the flume. We capture 3D-movements of each fish by positioning a 30 cm×60 cm mirror at 45° above the experimental tank (Fig. 1). Therefore, lateral and top images of the fish were produced at one time, the bottom half of the camera filming the fish laterally to determine height in the water column and position along the length of the flow tank, while the upper half of the camera filmed through the mirror and viewed the fish from above against the floor of the tank, allowing positions across the width of the flow tank to be determined. Frame-by-frame analysis of the horizontal and vertical positions of each fish at the time of first departure from a steady swimming mode and at the moment of engulfment of a plankter (Artemianauplii) was accomplished though use of the software Scion Image (NIH image analysis). The 3D-coordinate position (x, y, z) of each fish in the flume was established against two ruled grids, one placed on the backwall of the flume and another on the bottom of the flume. For horizontal positions of the fish (x, y), the midpoint between the eyes of each fish was used as reference, and for the vertical position (z) the centre of the fish's observable (right) eye.

Experimental procedure

Artemia nauplii were used as in Kiflafi and Genin (1997), since they have a shape and size similar to the prey of shiner pearch, calanoid copepods, and because they lack escape behaviour(Coughlin and Strickler, 1990; Trager et al., 1994). Individual fish did not require conditioning to swim in the flume and, even though naïve, fed on Artemia when introduced [also reported for natural zooplankton by McFarland and Levin(2002)]. As suggested by Webb(1982) for visual reaction in fish, the size of an Artemia (i.e. particle diameter) was considered as the mean between its length and width (mean size 0.053±0.002 cm; N of subsample =10).

Two sets of experiments differing in prey type were carried out. Perch collected in August of 2000 were used in the first set of trials, where naturally semi-transparent Artemia nauplii were released into the flume-tank (Fig. 2). In a second set of experiments, shiner perch collected in April 2002 were presented with darkened Artemia (Fig. 2). A stone for grinding a Sumi ink stick with water was used to make liquid black ink. Darkening was obtained by adding a suspension of black ink (0.037 g in 1 l of water) into the Artemia's tank overnight. The Sumi ink produced a darkening of the Artemia gut.

Fig. 2.

Images of semi-transparent (natural) (A, under dark-field illumination; C,under bright-field illumination) and darkened (B, under dark-field illumination; D, under bright-field illumination) Artemia. Specimens were photographed using a photomicroscope (Wild M400 Photomakroskop) onto Fuji Sensia 100 iso slide film, using two synchronised flashes (Sunpak Auto 383 Super) and a 64× magnification. Scanned (jpg) imaged were obtained using a Scanner UMAX Mirage II Se, at a 200×200 dpi resolution and 342×537 pixels. These images do not necessarily reflect the Artemia as perceived by the fish.

Fig. 2.

Images of semi-transparent (natural) (A, under dark-field illumination; C,under bright-field illumination) and darkened (B, under dark-field illumination; D, under bright-field illumination) Artemia. Specimens were photographed using a photomicroscope (Wild M400 Photomakroskop) onto Fuji Sensia 100 iso slide film, using two synchronised flashes (Sunpak Auto 383 Super) and a 64× magnification. Scanned (jpg) imaged were obtained using a Scanner UMAX Mirage II Se, at a 200×200 dpi resolution and 342×537 pixels. These images do not necessarily reflect the Artemia as perceived by the fish.

In both experiments, fish were acclimated in the flume tank at zero current speed for 40 min before each experiment. Current speed was then increased slowly over a 2 min period to the chosen trial speed. The order of trial speeds was chosen at random among the six possible combinations of orders using slow (0.52 body length s–1 for semi-transparent Artemia and 0.66 body length s–1 for darkened Artemia), intermediate (1.88 body length s–1 for semi-transparent Artemia and 1.60 body length s–1for darkened Artemia), and fast flow speeds (3.12 body length s–1 for semi-transparent Artemia and 3.01 body length s–1 for darkened Artemia). These speeds are within the range in which C. aggregata swims using pectoral fin locomotion, i.e. they are lower than the gait transition speeds (from pectoral fin to caudal fin locomotion), observed in this species and size(Mussi et al., 2002). Ten minutes after a trial speed was reached, live darkened or natural(semi-transparent) Artemia nauplii were introduced from behind the collimator baffles and in the approximate centre of the section (i.e. midway between the walls and in midwater). We assumed that, after recirculating in the flow tank (i.e. Artemia took a full trip around the flow tank before being preyed upon), the nauplii were distributed randomly over the tank cross section. The numbers of trials for the natural (semi-transparent) Artemia experiment were 15, 15 and 19 for slow, intermediate and fast current speeds, respectively, and 10 trials at each current speed were performed using darkened Artemia. Each trial involved one single fish. For each individual in both darkened and natural conditions, up to four prey captures were analysed at each current speed.

Fish first reacted to an approaching particle by a noticeable change in position at the time TR(Fig. 3). Because a latent period must occur between when the reaction is triggered by the particle and the actual visible reaction of the fish(Dill, 1974; Domenici, 2002), we have defined the theoretical time at which the particle triggers fishes' `inner reaction' as TL (the reaction at the sensory system level). The time of the fish's first `visible reaction' was defined as TR (the reaction at the motor level), and the time at which the fish captured the particle was defined as TC. The hypothesized delay between TL and TR would be due to the conductive times of the neurosensory and neuromuscular systems. While we were not able to determine TL experimentally, its definition is needed in order to facilitate further theoretical considerations.

Fig. 3.

Geometry of the particle detection and capture in three dimensions. The direction of the current is indicated by empty arrows. A fish's initial detection response is triggered at TL, and the fish's first visible reaction to an approaching food particle occurs at TR (i.e. response latency occurs between TL and TR). The arrow between TL and TR indicates the distance covered by the particle during the response latency. The food particle is captured at TC, and the distance covered between TR and TC corresponds to Yforward. Various particles are drawn at TC because the particle at TC may be upstream or downstream relative to the fish's eyes at TR(data not shown). Xlateral is the lateral distance in the horizontal plane. Xlateral is the overall lateral distance between the fish's eyes and the axis of motion of the prey along the current. Zvertical and Dtotal are the vertical distance and the total distance, in three dimensions, between fish's eyes and the particle at TR, respectively. Alpha (α)is the angle between the current direction and Dtotal. Beta (β) is the angular size of the plankton as seen by the fish.

Fig. 3.

Geometry of the particle detection and capture in three dimensions. The direction of the current is indicated by empty arrows. A fish's initial detection response is triggered at TL, and the fish's first visible reaction to an approaching food particle occurs at TR (i.e. response latency occurs between TL and TR). The arrow between TL and TR indicates the distance covered by the particle during the response latency. The food particle is captured at TC, and the distance covered between TR and TC corresponds to Yforward. Various particles are drawn at TC because the particle at TC may be upstream or downstream relative to the fish's eyes at TR(data not shown). Xlateral is the lateral distance in the horizontal plane. Xlateral is the overall lateral distance between the fish's eyes and the axis of motion of the prey along the current. Zvertical and Dtotal are the vertical distance and the total distance, in three dimensions, between fish's eyes and the particle at TR, respectively. Alpha (α)is the angle between the current direction and Dtotal. Beta (β) is the angular size of the plankton as seen by the fish.

Neither darkened nor semi-transparent Artemia nauplii were visible on the video images. However, the jaw protrusion of the fish that corresponded to the engulfing of an Artemia was visible. Previous video recordings from close up at all speeds indicated that the position of the tip of the mouth at maximum mouth protrusion was a good estimator of the position of an Artemia at the time of capture along the tank axis. The vertical position of the particle was therefore estimated as the midpoint between the two edges of the mouth tip at maximum protrusion, while the position of the particle across the width of the tank was estimated by using the top image of the tip of the mouth at the time of maximum protrusion, in accordance with previous observations. Therefore, the time TR and the position of the mouth at TC when a fish captured a particle (easily detectable by jaw protrusion), were used to estimate the position of the nauplius at TR. This assumes that nauplii moved passively within the current (TR to TC in Fig. 3). The vertical distance between a fish and a food particle at TR (Zvertical) was estimated from video side views. The video top view provided estimates of Yforward (the distance between the fish and the prey approaching along the current axis), and Xlateral(the lateral distance between the fish and the prey along the axis perpendicular to the current, in the horizontal plane).

The geometry in Fig. 3 was reconstructed for each capture event, and all analyses were done using the plane DtotalXlateralYforward, where Dtotal is the estimated total distance between the fish's eyes and the food at TR, and Xlateralis the overall lateral distance between the fish's eyes and the axis of motion of the prey along the current. Dtotal is at an angle alpha(α) from the fish's axis. Left and right responses were pooled as if they were all responses to particles approaching on the right side of the fish.

The angular size threshold (βR, i.e. β at TR) of the prey was calculated as the angle subtended by the plankton onto the fish's eye at the time of reaction(Fig. 3). The apparent loom threshold (λR, i.e. loom at TR) was calculated as in Dill's (1974)equation, modified for the case of approaching objects not necessarily in line with the fish's body axis, following McFarland and Levin(2002).

McFarland and Levin's equation (equation 26 in McFarland and Levin, 2002),for the general case of a particle coming towards the fish in any direction,corresponds to:
\[\ {\lambda}_{\mathrm{R}}=\frac{d{\beta}}{dt}=\frac{-2R}{(D_{\mathrm{total}}^{2}+R^{2})}(U_{\mathrm{y}}{\\ }\mathrm{cos}{\\ }{\alpha}+U_{\mathrm{x}}{\\ }\mathrm{sin}{\\ }{\alpha}),\]
1
where R the radius of the approaching particle, Dtotal is the total distance to the particle at TR, β is the angular size of the particle as seen by the fish, α is the angle between the line that connects the fish to the particle and the current direction at TR,Uy is particle speed along the current (with a negative sign if the particle is approaching the fish; McFarland and Levin 2002), Ux is particle speed perpendicularly to the current (note that our y and x axes correspond to the x and y axes, respectively, in McFarland and Levin, 2002). In our case, we assume that Ux is zero as the particles are carried passively by the current, which runs parallel to the axis of the fish. Therefore,
\[\ {\lambda}_{\mathrm{R}}=\frac{d{\beta}}{dt}=\frac{-2RU_{\mathrm{y}}{\\ }\mathrm{cos}{\\ }{\alpha}}{D_{\mathrm{total}}^{2}+R^{2}}.\]
2
Using particle diameter P=2R and considering current speed Uc=–Uy (i.e. considering current towards the fish having a positive speed) for particles approaching,we obtain:
\[\ {\lambda}_{\mathrm{R}}=\frac{d{\beta}}{dt}=\frac{4U_{\mathrm{c}}{\\ }P{\\ }\mathrm{cos}{\\ }{\alpha}}{4D_{\mathrm{total}}^{2}+P^{2}},\]
3
where Uc is current speed, P is the particle diameter calculated as the mean between the particle's length and width(Webb 1982), Dtotal is the total distance to the particle at TR, β is the angular size of the particle as seen by the fish, and α is the angle between the line that connects the fish to the particle and the current direction, at TR(Fig. 3).
The apparent angular velocity threshold (i.e. ωR, the angular velocity of the particle at TR) was calculated following McFarland and Levin(2002). Their equation(equation 6 in McFarland and Levin,2002) for the general case of a particle coming in any direction corresponds to:
\[\ {\omega}_{\mathrm{R}}=\frac{Y_{\mathrm{forward}}U_{\mathrm{x}}-X_{\mathrm{lateral}}U_{\mathrm{y}}}{D_{\mathrm{total}}^{2}}.\]
4
Since in our case Ux is equal to zero (see above), the equation becomes:
\[\ {\omega}_{\mathrm{R}}=\frac{-X_{\mathrm{lateral}}U_{\mathrm{y}}}{D_{\mathrm{total}}^{2}}.\]
5
The minus sign is due to the fact that McFarland and Levin(2002) consider speed as negative when approaching the fish. We switched the sign to positive, as in our case Uc=–Uy. In addition, Xlateral is equal to Dtotal sinα (equation 3 in McFarland and Levin, 2002). Therefore, the equation becomes:
\[\ {\omega}_{\mathrm{R}}=\frac{U_{\mathrm{c}}{\\ }\mathrm{sin}{\\ }{\alpha}}{D_{\mathrm{total}}}.\]
6

The effect of current speed and plankton type (natural vsdarkened) was tested using two-way ANOVAs (analysis of variance) for each feeding reaction variable (i.e. Dtotal, Xlateral, Yforward, Zvertical, α, angular size, angular velocity, loom). To account for multiple simultaneous two-way ANOVAs, the level of significance was adjusted within columns using the sequential Bonferroni technique(Rice, 1989).

Theoretical considerations on how angular size, angular velocity and loom should vary as the particle approaches

If the time at which a particle first triggers a fish's reaction (i.e. the threshold at the sensory system level at TL), is caused by a given angular size (βL), angular velocity(ωL) or loom (λL) of an approaching particle, independent of its speed, then the `apparent' angular sizeβ R, angular velocity ωR or loomλ R (i.e. at TR), should vary with current speed as the approaching object covers a longer distance between TL and TR (assuming a constant latency) when travelling at higher speeds. The potential effect of current speed on the threshold for the fish's response can be visualised by plotting how angular size, loom, and angular velocity vary as a function of Yforward (Fig. 4). Since current speed for each trial is constant, decreasing values of Yforward also correspond to time. The angular size increases continuously as the particle travels along Yforward (Fig. 4A) and its rate of increase is higher the smaller the lateral distance Xlateral is. Therefore, assuming the theoretical angular size βL to be constant at different flow speeds, the experimental angular size βR should increase with current speed.

Fig. 4.

(A) The relationship between angular size (βR) and Yforward at various theoretical values of Xlateral. The graph is valid for all speeds, because angular size is not affected by current speed (Uc). Any given value of βR is reached at decreasing values of Yforward as Xlateral increases. (B)The relationship between loom λR and Yforward (the particle distance upstream at the time of TR), at various theoretical values of Xlateral, at the intermediate current speed used in darkened conditions. At Xlateral=0, λRis always highest. Horizontal line represents the average experimental values of λR found in darkened conditions at intermediate Uc. λR is reached at smaller values of Yforward as Xlateral values increase.(C) The relationship between angular velocity ωR and Yforward at various theoretical values of Xlateral, at the intermediate current speed used for semi-transparent Artemia. At Xlateral=0,ω R would be equal to zero and it is not represented. Horizontal line represents the average experimental values ofω R found for semi-transparent Artemia at intermediate Uc. Average ωR is reached at increasing values of Yforward as Xlateral values increase.

Fig. 4.

(A) The relationship between angular size (βR) and Yforward at various theoretical values of Xlateral. The graph is valid for all speeds, because angular size is not affected by current speed (Uc). Any given value of βR is reached at decreasing values of Yforward as Xlateral increases. (B)The relationship between loom λR and Yforward (the particle distance upstream at the time of TR), at various theoretical values of Xlateral, at the intermediate current speed used in darkened conditions. At Xlateral=0, λRis always highest. Horizontal line represents the average experimental values of λR found in darkened conditions at intermediate Uc. λR is reached at smaller values of Yforward as Xlateral values increase.(C) The relationship between angular velocity ωR and Yforward at various theoretical values of Xlateral, at the intermediate current speed used for semi-transparent Artemia. At Xlateral=0,ω R would be equal to zero and it is not represented. Horizontal line represents the average experimental values ofω R found for semi-transparent Artemia at intermediate Uc. Average ωR is reached at increasing values of Yforward as Xlateral values increase.

Loom increases as the particle travels along the current up to a certain(relatively small) value of Yforward, and then decreases again (except if Xlateral=0 cm). For example, for an Xlateral value of 2, loom increases up to Yforward values around 1.5 cm(Fig. 4B). Therefore, if we assume λL to be constant at different current speeds, thenλ R should increase with current speed up to Yforward values of about 1.5 cm, as the distance covered by the particle along Yforward would increase with current speed. Loom decreases only when very close to the fish. The angular velocityω, conversely, increases continuously as the particle travels along Yforward, although this increase has different rates depending on Xlateral (Fig. 4C). In particular, if we assume ωL to be constant at different speeds, then ωR should increase with current speeds (as Yforward gets shorter). Therefore, we expect that the angular variable(s) implicated in the mechanisms triggering the feeding reaction should increase with speed.

For those angular variables that are found to increase with speed, we estimated latency as follows. To find a theoretical constant value of latency for all current speeds, an index of homogeneity (IH)between each average `angular variable' (i.e. angular size, angular velocity or loom) calculated at TL (i.e. assuming a given latency)at different current speeds was calculated using (maximum `angular variable'– minimum `angular variable') / minimum `angular variable'. Each angular variable at TL was calculated as the theoretical value that the fish would experience if, for each sequence, the particle had triggered a reaction at a position with Xlateral at TL being the same as at TR (since a straight course was assumed), while Yforward was considered longer at TL than at TR by a distance that was calculated as the current speed multiplied by the theoretical latency.

The alternative hypotheses for the three mechanisms triggering feeding reaction were further tested as follows:

  1. The frequency distributions of Xlateral were compared between darkened and semi-transparent Artemia experiments, since it can be assumed that low values of Xlateral may imply that the feeding reaction is triggered by an angular size or a loom mechanism,while high Xlateral values suggest a mechanism based on angular velocity. This is because, at any Yforward,angular size and loom should be highest for particles coming head on, while the angular velocity should be highest at low values of Xlateral only for very small values of Yforward (not observed here; see theoretical values in Fig. 4). Therefore, if an angular size or a loom threshold are used, the first particles to achieve threshold will be those with the smallest Xlateral. Conversely, if angular velocity is used, the first particles to reach threshold should always be off line from the fish's axis, i.e. at Xlateral>0.

  2. The potential roles of angular size, angular velocity and loom in the feeding reaction of the semi-transparent and darkened Artemia,respectively, was tested by comparing the Yforward and Xlateral positions of the particles at TR and the theoretical values of these positions based on the average experimental angular size, angular velocity and loom at each current speed.

Darkening (dark vs semi-transparent Artemia) had an effect on most variables measured (Table 1 and 2). In particular, the reaction distance Dtotal was significantly greater for the semi-transparent Artemia (Table 1 and 2, Fig. 5). The results from a two-way ANOVA (adjusted using the sequential Bonferroni method; Rice, 1989) revealed that current speed had no significant effect on all reaction distance parameters(i.e. Dtotal, Xlateral, Yforward, Zvertical), while darkening did (Table 2). No significant interaction effect of current speed and darkening was found. Neither current speed nor darkening had any effect on reaction angle α(P>0.5 and P>0.05, respectively), and the interaction effect was not significant according to the sequential Bonferroni technique(see Table 2).

Table 1.

Geometry of the visual response

Artemia typeCurrent speedYforward (cm)Zvertical (cm)Xlateral (cm)Dtotal (cm)α (deg.)
Natural Slow 4.0±0.5 1.1±0.2 2.1±0.3 4.6±0.5 30.1±3.9 
Natural Intermediate 5.7±0.7 1.2±0.2 2.5±0.3 6.3±0.7 25.5±3.6 
Natural Fast 6.0±0.4 1.0±0.3 2.7±0.3 6.6±0.5 23.2±1.3 
Darkened Slow 3.6±0.4 0.4±0.1 0.6±0.2 3.7±0.4 11.9±4.1 
Darkened Intermediate 3.1±0.3 0.7±0.1 1.3±0.2 3.5±0.2 24.3±6.0 
Darkened Fast 3.2±0.4 0.7±0.2 1.3±0.3 3.6±0.3 25.0±6.5 
Artemia typeCurrent speedYforward (cm)Zvertical (cm)Xlateral (cm)Dtotal (cm)α (deg.)
Natural Slow 4.0±0.5 1.1±0.2 2.1±0.3 4.6±0.5 30.1±3.9 
Natural Intermediate 5.7±0.7 1.2±0.2 2.5±0.3 6.3±0.7 25.5±3.6 
Natural Fast 6.0±0.4 1.0±0.3 2.7±0.3 6.6±0.5 23.2±1.3 
Darkened Slow 3.6±0.4 0.4±0.1 0.6±0.2 3.7±0.4 11.9±4.1 
Darkened Intermediate 3.1±0.3 0.7±0.1 1.3±0.2 3.5±0.2 24.3±6.0 
Darkened Fast 3.2±0.4 0.7±0.2 1.3±0.3 3.6±0.3 25.0±6.5 

Data shown are mean ± s.e.m.Dtotal(total distance from plankton at the time of reaction), Xlateral (lateral distance), Yforward(forward distance), Zvertical (vertical distance), α(angle between the fish's axis and the plankton).

Table 2.

Two-way ANOVA for each detection parameter measured

Variabled.f.msFP
Yforward     
   Intercept 1344.261 395.9377 <0.001 
   Speed 4.507 1.3276 >0.1 
   Darkening 67.136 19.7743 <0.001 
   Speed×darkening 10.230 3.0130 >0.05 
   Error 73 3.395 — — 
Dtotal     
   Intercept 1660.845 478.6935 <0.001 
   Speed 6.166 1.7771 >0.05 
   Darkening 91.503 26.3732 <0.001 
   Speed×darkening 7.622 2.1970 >0.05 
   Error 73 3.470 — — 
Zvertical     
   Intercept 52.27299 80.65771 <0.001 
   Speed 0.25464 0.39291 >0.5 
   Darkening 4.44736 6.86232 0.0107 
   Speed×darkening 0.18578 0.28666 >0.5 
   Error 73 0.64808 — — 
Xlateral     
   Intercept 225.2176 196.5117 <0.001 
   Speed 2.7245 2.3772 >0.05 
   Darkening 33.8909 29.5713 <0.001 
   Speed×darkening 0.0443 0.0386 >0.5 
   Error 73 1.1461 — — 
α, angle between particle and fish's axis     
   Intercept 40243.82 196.0764 <0.001 
   Speed 104.49 0.5091 >0.5 
   Darkening 632.13 3.0799 >0.05 
   Speed×darkening 714.95 3.4834 0.036 
   Error 73 205.25 — — 
ω, angular velocity     
   Intercept 145.0043 79.5432 <0.001 
   Speed 21.8438 11.9825 <0.001 
   Darkening 12.5417 6.8798 0.0106 
   Speed×darkening 7.5108 4.1201 0.020 
   Error 73 1.8230 — — 
λ, loom     
   Intercept 0.1352 160.3226 <0.001 
   Speed 0.0095 11.3796 <0.001 
   Darkening 0.0910 22.5987 <0.001 
   Speed×darkening 0.0058 6.9095 <0.005 
   Error 73 0.0008 — — 
β, angular size     
   Intercept 0.0136 261.5416 <0.001 
   Speed 0.0001 2.2211 >0.1 
   Darkening 0.0002 4.7532 0.032 
   Speed×darkening 0.0001 1.9541 >0.1 
   Error 73 0.00005 — — 
Variabled.f.msFP
Yforward     
   Intercept 1344.261 395.9377 <0.001 
   Speed 4.507 1.3276 >0.1 
   Darkening 67.136 19.7743 <0.001 
   Speed×darkening 10.230 3.0130 >0.05 
   Error 73 3.395 — — 
Dtotal     
   Intercept 1660.845 478.6935 <0.001 
   Speed 6.166 1.7771 >0.05 
   Darkening 91.503 26.3732 <0.001 
   Speed×darkening 7.622 2.1970 >0.05 
   Error 73 3.470 — — 
Zvertical     
   Intercept 52.27299 80.65771 <0.001 
   Speed 0.25464 0.39291 >0.5 
   Darkening 4.44736 6.86232 0.0107 
   Speed×darkening 0.18578 0.28666 >0.5 
   Error 73 0.64808 — — 
Xlateral     
   Intercept 225.2176 196.5117 <0.001 
   Speed 2.7245 2.3772 >0.05 
   Darkening 33.8909 29.5713 <0.001 
   Speed×darkening 0.0443 0.0386 >0.5 
   Error 73 1.1461 — — 
α, angle between particle and fish's axis     
   Intercept 40243.82 196.0764 <0.001 
   Speed 104.49 0.5091 >0.5 
   Darkening 632.13 3.0799 >0.05 
   Speed×darkening 714.95 3.4834 0.036 
   Error 73 205.25 — — 
ω, angular velocity     
   Intercept 145.0043 79.5432 <0.001 
   Speed 21.8438 11.9825 <0.001 
   Darkening 12.5417 6.8798 0.0106 
   Speed×darkening 7.5108 4.1201 0.020 
   Error 73 1.8230 — — 
λ, loom     
   Intercept 0.1352 160.3226 <0.001 
   Speed 0.0095 11.3796 <0.001 
   Darkening 0.0910 22.5987 <0.001 
   Speed×darkening 0.0058 6.9095 <0.005 
   Error 73 0.0008 — — 
β, angular size     
   Intercept 0.0136 261.5416 <0.001 
   Speed 0.0001 2.2211 >0.1 
   Darkening 0.0002 4.7532 0.032 
   Speed×darkening 0.0001 1.9541 >0.1 
   Error 73 0.00005 — — 

Significant results according to the sequential Bonferroni method(Rice, 1989) are in bold.—, not determined.

Fig. 5.

Frequency distribution of reaction distance Dtotal over all current speeds for both semi-transparent (white bars) and darkened (black bars) Artemia.

Fig. 5.

Frequency distribution of reaction distance Dtotal over all current speeds for both semi-transparent (white bars) and darkened (black bars) Artemia.

Current speed and darkening had no effect on angular size, and no interaction effect between darkening and current speed was found(Table 2 and Fig. 6). Conversely, both angular velocity and loom were affected by current speed (P<0.001 in both cases), as well as darkening (P=0.0106 and P<0.001, respectively), and the interaction of these two factors was significant for loom (P<0.005)(Table 2 and Fig. 6).

Fig. 6.

Angular velocity ωR (black bars), loomλ R (white bars) and angular size βR (grey bars) at different current speeds in the semi-transparent (A) and darkened (B)conditions; error bars show s.e.m.

Fig. 6.

Angular velocity ωR (black bars), loomλ R (white bars) and angular size βR (grey bars) at different current speeds in the semi-transparent (A) and darkened (B)conditions; error bars show s.e.m.

Angular variables involved in triggering the feeding reaction to plankton are expected to increase with current speeds, except for loom at very small distances (see above and Fig. 4). While loom may theoretically decrease at small distance from the fish, loom increases with decreasing Yforward when the Yforward and Xlateral ranges of all observed speeds are considered (only intermediate speed is shown as an example, in Fig. 4). The results show that with semi-transparent Artemia, βRdoes not vary significantly with speed(Table 2 and Fig. 6), ωRincreased with current speed, while λR was lowest at the intermediate speeds, and higher at the low and the high current speeds. Therefore, an index of homogeneity IH was calculated only for angular velocity. IH was highest when a theoretical latency of 385 ms was considered. That is, if we assume a latency of 385 ms,the values for the angular velocity when the fish's `inner reaction' occurs(ωL) are relatively constant (i.e. 0.26 rad s–1, 0.28 rad s–1 and 0.26 rad s–1 for slow, intermediate and fast speeds,respectively).

When darkened Artemia were used, the angular sizeβ R is relatively constant at different current speeds, while both ωR and λR increased with current speed in darkened Artemia (Fig. 6). Therefore, IH was calculated for both loom and angular velocity. However, a maximum IH was found only for loom, and it resulted in a latency of 230 ms. Using this theoretical latency, λL is relatively constant (i.e. 0.013 rad s–1, 0.015 rad s–1 and 0.013 rad s–1 for slow, intermediate and fast speeds, respectively). Conversely, IH increased indefinitely when increasing values of latency were applied to ωL.

The frequency distributions of Xlateral of the semi-transparent vs darkened treatments were compared using a Chi square test. Four bins (0–1, 1–2, 2–3, >3 cm) were used for the comparison (Fig. 7). The distributions of the two treatments are significantly different(Chi2 22.69; 3 d.f.; P<0.0001). The highest peak in the distribution for the darkened treatment was in the bin Xlateral=0–1 cm, i.e. for particles almost in line with the fish.

Fig. 7.

Frequency distribution of particle lateral distance(Xlateral) in the darkened (black bars) and semi-transparent conditions (white bars) over all current speeds.

Fig. 7.

Frequency distribution of particle lateral distance(Xlateral) in the darkened (black bars) and semi-transparent conditions (white bars) over all current speeds.

The comparison between the Yforward and Xlateral positions of the particles at TR and the theoretical values of these positions based on the average experimental angular size, angular velocity and loom at each current speed is shown in Fig. 8. For the semi-transparent Artemia experiments, the theoretical values of Yforward and Xlateral at TR based on the average angular velocity are closer to the experimental values than those based on the average loom or angular size for the fast current speed (ANOVA; P<0.01; Tukey post test, P<0.01 and P<0.05 for the comparisons between loom and ωR, andβ R and ωR, respectively), although differences were not significant for the intermediate and slow speeds(Fig. 8). The theoretical values of Yforward and Xlateral based on the average loom for intermediate and fast speeds are closer to the experimental values than those based on angular velocity (intermediate and fast; ANOVA; P<0.05; Tukey Post-test, P<0.05 in both cases), although there were no significant differences when values based on averages βR and ωR were compared. Differences were not significant in any case for the slow speed(Fig. 8).

Fig. 8.

Experimental values of Yforward and Xlateral at low, intermediate and high current speed in semi-transparent (A, B, C, respectively) and darkened (D, E, F, respectively). Blue, red and green lines represent values corresponding to a constant loom,angular velocity and angular size, respectively (i.e. the average loom,angular velocity and angular size for each given speed, respectively).

Fig. 8.

Experimental values of Yforward and Xlateral at low, intermediate and high current speed in semi-transparent (A, B, C, respectively) and darkened (D, E, F, respectively). Blue, red and green lines represent values corresponding to a constant loom,angular velocity and angular size, respectively (i.e. the average loom,angular velocity and angular size for each given speed, respectively).

Angular size vs loom vs angular velocity

Planktivorous fishes typically feed on transient zooplankton delivered by water currents (Stevenson,1972; Hobson and Chess, 1976, 1978; Thresher, 1983; Hobson, 1991). When a plankter is transported by the current towards a fish's visual field, angular size,loom and angular velocity change over time(McFarland and Levine, 2002). These parameters could act independently or together as attentive cues to alert a fish to the presence of particles and to trigger a sensory–motor reaction (O'Brien and Slade,1976; Dunbrack and Dill,1984; Dill et al.,1997; Raviv,2000).

The geometry of the particle position and, in particular, its lateral distance, at which the feeding reaction occurs can give us some insights on the main visual mechanism involved. A relatively distant food particle travelling towards the fish at a given lateral distance(Xlateral) from the nasal–caudal axis has a relatively low angular velocity, which is increasing very slowly. As the particle approaches, a sharp increase in angular velocity takes place, and it reaches a peak (Fig. 4)whenYforward=0 and subsequently decreases as the particle travels along its rectilinear path. By contrast, a straight-on particle(Xlateral=0) has zero angular velocity throughout its approach towards the fish. Conversely, loom and angular size are maximised when lateral distance is minimal. This scenario suggests that preference for particles coming at a lateral distance Xlateral=0 implies angular size or loom as the mechanisms triggering the feeding reaction. Conversely, if particles coming at small lateral distance are not attacked,then angular velocity may be the mechanism involved in the feeding reaction.

It is reasonable to suppose that fish would capture most particles at low Xlateral values for minimizing energy expenditure, if detection mechanisms were not limiting factors. However, in spite of possible energetic advantages of capturing prey in line with fish's nasal–caudal axis, very few particles were taken in when semi-transparent Artemiatravelled at small Xlateral values (i.e. <1 cm; Fig. 7). In fact, most particles produced a reaction when they approached off-axis with peak frequency values of Xlateral at 2–3 cm, and with values of Xlateral as high as 6 cm(Fig. 7). These results suggested that a threshold angular velocity ωR might alert a fish to approaching semi-transparent Artemia, and promote the feeding reaction to food at greater reaction distances(Fig. 5).

By contrast, with darkened Artemia, the peak response frequency occurred when prey approached closest to a fish's nasal–caudal axis. The larger reaction to darkened prey at low Xlateral(Fig. 7), suggested that loom or angular size rather than angular velocity might have triggered a fish's reaction to darkened prey (Fig. 8). However, further explanations, are needed to account for the decline in frequency at Xlateral values >3 cm in semi-transparent Artemia (Fig. 7). This decline may be related to the energetic cost of feeding at larger Xlateral and to the fact that peak values ofω R decline as Xlateral increases(Fig. 4C).

The effect of current speed on each angular variable suggests that angular velocity is the mechanism triggering the response to semi-transparent Artemia, while loom is the mechanism triggering the reaction to feed on darkened plankton. This is because angular velocity for the semi-transparent Artemia and loom for the darkened Artemiawere the only variables that both (1) increased with current speed as expected if a relatively constant latency and a constant angular threshold are assumed(2) allowed calculation of a reasonable latency. These results are in line with our comparison between the experimental Yforward and Xlateral and the expected Yforward and Xlateral based on averages angular size, angular velocity or loom. Such a comparison suggests that angular velocity may be the variable that triggers the feeding reaction to semi-transparent Artemia,although differences are significant only for high current speed. Conversely,the fitting of the experimental data with the model for the darkened Artemia treatment is not significantly different between angular size and loom. This is because the shapes of the curve for angular size and loom are very similar (Fig. 8). However, as discussed above (Materials and methods section), if angular size was the mechanism triggering the feeding reaction, one would expect the position of the particle at the apparent reaction time(TR) to be different for different current speeds. Let us consider a realistic example in which the sensory threshold occurs at a given angular size corresponding to a distance of 10 cm at TL. Let us consider a given latency of 200 ms. In this case, at TR, high-speed particles should be closer to the fish, at approximately 5 cm distance from the fish vs 9 cm distance for slow speed particles. This assumes the same time interval (200 ms) between when the sensory threshold is reached (i.e. TL) and the motor response (i.e. TR) in the fish at all current speeds. Conversely, if a loom mechanism is considered, higher current speed would cause both (1) higher rate of change of the looming angle and (2) a longer distance travelled between TL and TR. Higher loom in high speed would cause reaching the sensory threshold from a larger distance. As a realistic example, let us consider that sensory threshold (i.e. at TL) of loom may be reached at 10 cm for high speed particles vs 6 cm for slow speed particles. However, this difference may be in large part cancelled out, at least in our experimental conditions, by the longer distance travelled by the high speed particles. Following our example, during a given latency period of 200 ms, higher speed would allow particles to travel approximately 5 cm vs 1 cm travelled at slow speed. This would cause slow and fast particles to be at a similar distance (i.e. 5 cm according to our example) from the fish at the time of the fish's response. Therefore, loom, but not angular size, can explain particles travelling at different speeds to be in similar positions(Fig. 8D–F) relative to the fish at the time of reaction (TR). We conclude that loom is the most likely variable involved in triggering the feeding reaction of fish preying on darkened Artemia.

Latency and speed

The reaction models based on average angular velocity ωRor loom λR fit better at high, rather than at low, current speed for darkened and semi-transparent Artemia, respectively(Fig. 8). An explanation could be that variability in the time for reaction may decrease with current speed(i.e. when a fish needs to minimize latency to successfully capture the particle). Previous work on escape response in zebra danio (Brachidanio rerio) showed that predator speed had no effect on the apparent loom threshold, while it had an effect on the reaction distance and, therefore,latencies were considered negligible (Dill,1974). However, latencies of escape responses are likely to be shorter than those of feeding reactions, as escape responses are triggered by the Mauthner cells, a pair of giant neurons in the hindbrain of most fish species (Eaton and Hackett, 1984). Batty(1989) reported escape response latency to visual stimuli to be about 40 ms. The reaction to an approaching prey (e.g. plankton), may have a longer latency of the order of 100–300 ms, because more time is needed to process a prey's movement and trajectory,and then to direct an attack. This range corresponds approximately at the values found for maximizing IH (i.e. assuming that fish respond to a given value of angular velocity or loom at all current speeds).

At higher current speeds (e.g. 40 cm s–1) than those used here, C. aggregata swam in a flume but did not attempt to capture plankton (McFarland and Levin,2002). This may have been due to sensory–motor constraints,which influenced the fish's reaction time preventing successful capture when plankton approached at high speeds. Furthermore, feeding may become energetically costly at very high current speeds, even if fish may be capable of capturing the prey.

Plankton visibility

Transparency in the aquatic environment represents a significant adaptive cryptic mechanism (Johnsen 2001a,b, 2002). However, visual adaptations, such as UV and polarization vision, have evolved among planktivores to increase apparent visual contrast(Lythgoe and Hemmings, 1967; Lythgoe, 1979; Loew et al., 1993; McFarland and Loew, 1994; Cronin et al., 1994; Shashar et al., 1998). Foraging success in planktivores is also strongly dependent on the light,turbidity, background space light, and visibility of pigmented parts, such as the eyes or the gut of the prey (Vinyard,1980; McFall-Ngai,1990; Thetmeyer and Kils,1995; Johnsen and Widder,1998; Tsuda et al.,1998; Utne-Palm,1999; Utne-Palm and Stiansen,2002). Under the same background light conditions, fish predation was shown to be higher when transparent prey with larger eyes were used vs prey with small eyes (Zaret 1972), and reaction distances for transparent prey were significantly lower than those for pigmented prey(Thetmeyer and Kils, 1995; Tsuda et al., 1998; Utne-Palm 1999). However, it was shown by video imaging in the near field that transparent zooplankton irradiated from above when viewed horizontally against a darker background appeared as bright targets due to light scattering off their bodies(Loew and McFarland, 2000). Some planktivorous fish search for prey outside Snell's window (i.e. a cone angle of about 97.2° through which the terrestrial hemisphere can be seen), because prey are seen against a dark background(Lythgoe, 1979).

In our study, feeding reaction to zooplankton appeared to occur at longer distances for semi-transparent Artemia than for a darkened one. This result would be in line with Loew and McFarland's(2000) observations, and is in agreement with the general observation that changes in prey contrast may cause differences in reaction distances, although most observations are on pigmented prey against a light background (e.g. Utne-Palm, 1999). In our study, the dark background of the collimator tubes would enhance the visibility of semi-transparent plankton; therefore, semi-transparent Artemia should be visible at a greater distance due to their higher light scattering (diffuse reflectance) against the dark background. Because light was weakly reflected from a dark Artemia's body there would be less contrast between the target and the background in darkened Artemia (Fig. 2 and E. R. Loew and W.N.McF., unpublished).

The higher visibility of the natural (semi-transparent) plankton at longer distances, however, does not explain why shiner perch did not react to semi-transparent particles coming straight on. At small Xlateral, fish could theoretically react to a threshold angular velocity, although such a threshold would be reached at very small values of Yforward(Fig. 4) (i.e. when it may be too late for the fish to react in time to catch the prey). Alternatively, fish could have used loom as a mechanism triggering their feeding reaction. A threshold loom would be reached at relatively high values of Yforward for the case of small Xlateral (Fig. 4). If we assume that the apparent size of semi-transparent and darkened particles was the same, then a Yforward of about 6 cm should trigger a reaction when Xlateral=0, as in the semi-transparent Artemia at intermediate speed. Because relatively few fish reacted to natural plankton at small Xlateralvalues, it is possible that the apparent size of the darkened plankton was greater than that of the natural plankton, although there we have no evidence that darkening can cause a change in apparent size. If semi-transparent Artemia had a relatively small apparent size, for any value of Xlateral and Yforward, loom for a darkened Artemia would be greater than for a semi-transparent Artemia. In this case, it would be possible that when using semi-transparent Artemia, a threshold loom may have occurred when the prey was too close to the fish to react in time for prey capture. Alternatively, it may be possible that shiner perch do not have enough behavioural flexibility in order to shift from a reaction based on angular velocity mechanism to one based on loom within a single feeding session, and it may be `locked' in using the most favourable mechanism of detection for any given contrast conditions. However, we are not aware of any supporting evidence from other sources, and therefore this suggestion remains highly speculative.

Under the conditions of our experiments, we may be dealing with both positive and negative contrast situations. For the natural prey irradiated from above viewed against the darker background there may be positive contrast due to internal scatter. However, for the darkened prey the contrast may be negative. Since on- and off-channels use separate pathways that may differ in properties, it is possible that some of the observed differences in feeding reaction mechanisms may be due to the way the visual system handles positive and negative contrast (Wheeler,1982).

Limitations of our study

We have examined a fish's response to food in a flume tank where turbulence in the water flow was minimized, and where the detection of plankton was constrained by the dimensions of the flume. Therefore, this study was simplified compared with foraging in natural water currents. In nature,turbulence (Aksnes and Giske,1993; Kiorboe and Saiz,1995; McFarland and Levin,2002), as well as schooling can affect foraging behaviour in planktivores (Ryer and Olla,1991; O'Driscoll,1998; Lachlan et al.,1998; Foster et al.,2001). Nevertheless, individual responses in a flume to approaching zooplankton allowed us to standardize feeding reaction parameters and to define a fish's reaction to plankton.

In our study, fish reaction to approaching zooplankton was assumed to be independent of the particle shape or its intrinsic motion. In order to standardize prey size and behaviour, we used only Artemia nauplii. Artemia is a non-evasive prey since no significant escape motion has been recorded (Coughlin and Strickler,1990) and, generally, planktivores preferentially select non-evasive prey items, even when the evasive prey are larger(Drenner et al., 1978; Vinyard 1980, 1982). Although Artemia is not a natural prey of shiner perch, perch showed similar behavioural patterns when feeding on natural plankton(McFarland and Levin, 2002). In addition, by staining Artemia with black ink, we provide an artificial set-up that may have affected the behaviour of Artemia or its physiology (e.g. any chemical cues it may provide). However, as suggested by its lack of significant evasive behaviour(Coughlin and Strickler, 1990), Artemia was most likely just passively carried by the current and therefore its behaviour should not have affected our results.

The dark background we used does not correspond to the most common environment where plankton is found. However, certain environmental conditions, i.e. twilight, as well as in certain conditions of turbidity,cloudiness and depth, may also provide a relatively dark background. Nevertheless, conditions of visibility created in laboratory set-ups such as ours are quite different from natural conditions, and it would be interesting to investigate the feeding reactions of planktivorous fishes to a variety of plankton types in natural conditions.

Conclusions

Our results showed that at high current speeds, angular velocity was the likely cue for triggering the feeding reaction when semi-transparent Artemia were used. Loom appeared to be the main mechanism when prey were darkened. It is possible that both mechanisms act in concert during some of the feeding phases (detection, pursuit, capture), although one mechanism might eventually prevail in detecting certain prey characteristics (e.g. motion type, prey contrast).

As our major conclusion, we suggest that prey contrast dramatically affects detection and the subsequent feeding reaction. When prey contrast was high due to scattering (semi-transparent prey), feeding reactions occurred at greater distances and at increasing Xlateral values where angular velocity tends to be higher. When prey contrast was low (darkened treatment),reaction distances diminished and feeding reactions occurred for prey approaching at smaller lateral distance (Xlateral). This study showed that visual behaviour of planktivores is highly flexible because fish appear to be able to utilize different visual properties of the approaching object for their reaction to prey. Although angular velocity was the main cue for triggering feeding reactions to semi-transparent plankton at high current speeds, it is still possible that during the tracking of a plankter after a prey's detection, loom may have a role in determining the time of engulfment. This and other issues will be explored in a further paper dealing with the pursuit and capture of plankton.

We thank Friday Harbor Laboratory, its director Dennis Willows and all the staff, for the space and assistance provided. We thank also Simon Levin,Samuel Ramsden, Lyle Britt, Mike Burrows, Bruno Pernet and Ellis Loew for their valuable advice. The useful comments of two anonymous referees are greatly acknowledged. This paper is dedicated to Mac.

Aksnes, D. L. and Giske, J. (
1993
). A theoretical model of aquatic visual feeding.
Ecol. Modelling
67
,
233
-250.
Barlow, H. P. and Hill, R. M. (
1963
). Selective sensitivity to direction of movement in ganglion cells of the rabbit retina.
Science
139
,
412
-414.
Batty, R. S. (
1989
). Escape responses of herring larvae to visual stimuli.
J. Mar. Biol. Ass. UK
69
,
647
-654.
Coughlin, D. J. and Strickler, J. R. (
1990
). Zooplankton capture by a coral reef fish: an adaptive response to evasive prey.
Env. Biol. Fish.
29
,
35
-42.
Cronin, T. W., Marshall, N. J., Caldwell, R. L. and Shashar,N. (
1994
). Specialization of retinal function in the compound eyes of mantis shrimp.
Vision Res.
34
,
2639
-2656.
Cronly-Dillon, J. R. (
1964
). Units sensitive to direction of movement in goldfish optic tectum.
Nature
2203
,
214
-215.
Dill, L. M. (
1974
). The escape response of the zebra danio (Brachidanio rerio). I. The stimulus for escape.
Anim. Behav.
22
,
710
-721.
Dill, L. M., Holling, C. S. and Palmer, L. H.(
1997
). Predicting the 3D structure of animal aggregations from functional consideration: the role of information. In
Animal Groups in Three Dimensions
, ch. 14 (ed. J. K. Parrish and W. M. Hamner),pp.
207
-224. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Domenici, P. (
2002
). The visually mediated escape response in fish: Predicting prey responsiveness and the locomotor behaviour of predators and prey.
Mar. Fresh. Behav. Physiol.
35
,
87
-110.
Drenner, R. F., Strickler, J. R. and O'Brien, W. J.(
1978
). Capture probability: the role of the zooplankter escape in the selective feeding of planktivorous fish.
J. Fish. Res. Bd Can.
35
,
1370
-1373.
Dunbrack, R. L. and Dill, L. M. (
1984
). Three dimensional prey reaction field of the juvenile coho salmon (Onchorynchus kisutch).
Can. J. Fish. Aqua. Sci.
41
,
1176
-1182.
Foster, E. G., Ritz, D. A., Osborn, J. E. and Swadling, K. M. (
2001
). Schooling affects the feeding success of Australian salmon (Arripis trutta) when preying on mysid swarms(Paramesopodopsis rufa).
J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol.
261
,
93
-106.
Guthrie, D. M. and Banks, J. R. (
1974
). Input characteristics of the intrinsic cells of the optic tectum of teleost fish.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol.
47A
,
83
-92.
Hamner, W. M., Jones, M. S., Carleton J. H., Hauri, I. R. and Williams D. McB. (
1988
). Zooplankton, planktivorous fish, and water currnets on a windward reef face: Great Barrier Reef, Australia.
Bull. Mar. Sci.
42
,
459
-479.
Hobson, E. S. (
1972
). Activity of Hawaiian reef fishes during the evening and morning transitions between daylight and darkness.
Fish. Bull.
70
,
715
-740.
Hobson, E. S. (
1991
). Trophic relationships of fishes specialized to feed on zooplankters above coral reefs. In
The Ecology of Fishes on Coral Reefs
(ed. P. F. Sale),pp.
69
-93. Academic Press.
Hobson, E. S. and Chess, J. R. (
1976
). Trophic intearctions among fishes and zooplankters nearshore at Santa Catalina Island,California.
Fish. Bull.
74
,
567
-598.
Hobson, E. S. and Chess, J. R. (
1978
). Trophic interactions among fishes and plankton in the lagoon at Enewetak Atoll,Marshall Islands.
Fish. Bull.
76
,
133
-153.
Jacobson, M. and Gaze, R. M. (
1964
). Types of visual response from single units in the optic tectum and optic nerve of goldfish.
Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B
49
,
199
-209.
Johnsen, S. (
2001a
). Hidden in plain sight:cryptic and conspicuous coloration in the pelagic environment.
Am. Zool.
41
,
1487
-1487.
Johnsen, S. (
2001b
). Hidden in plain sight: the ecology and physiology of organismal transparency.
Biol. Bull.
201
,
301
-318.
Johnsen, S. (
2002
). Cryptic and conspicuous coloration in the pelagic environment.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B.
269
,
243
-256.
Johnsen, S. and Widder, E. A. (
1998
). Transparency and visibility of gelatinous zooplankton from the northwestern Atlantic and Gulf of Mexico.
Biol. Bull.
195
,
337
-348.
Kawasaki, M. and Aoki, K. (
1983
). Visual responses recorded from the optic tectum of Japanese dace, Tribolodon hakonensis.
J. Comp. Physiol.
152
,
147
-153.
Kelly, D. H. (
1972
). Flicker. In
Handbook of Sensory Physiology
vol.
VII/4
, ch. 11 (ed. D. Jameson and L. M. Hurvich), pp.
273
-302. Berlin, Germany:Springer-Verlag.
Kerker, M. (
1969
).
The scattering of light and other electromagnetic radiation
. New York, USA: Academic Press.
Kiflawi, M. and Genin, A. (
1997
). Prey flux manipulation and the feeding rates of reef-dwelling planktivorous fish.
Ecology
78
,
1062
-1077.
Kiorboe, T. and Saiz, E. (
1995
). Planktivorous feeding in calm and turbulent environments, with emphasis on copepods.
Mar. Ecol. Progr. Ser.
122
,
135
-145.
Lachlan, R. F., Crooks, L. and Laland, K. N.(
1998
). Who follows whom? Shoaling preferences and social learning of foraging information in guppies.
Anim. Behav.
56
,
181
-190.
Loew, E. R. and McFarland, W. N. (
2000
). Zooplankton as visible targets for planktivores. In
Second Workshop on Ultraviolet and Polarization Vision
, June 20–24, 2000(ed. C. Hawryshyn and H. Browman), p.
19
. University of Victoria, BC Canada.
Loew, E. R., McFarland, W. N., Mills, E. L. and Hunter, D.(
1993
). A chromatic action spectrum for planktonic predation by juvenile yellow perch, Perca flavescens.
Can. J. Zool.
71
,
384
-386.
Lythgoe, J. N. (
1979
).
The Ecology Of Vision
. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.
Lythgoe, J. N. and Hemmings, C. C. (
1967
). Polarized light and underwater vision.
Nature
213
,
893
-894.
McFall-Ngai, M. J.
1990
. Crypsis in the pelagic environment.
Am. Zool.
30
,
175
-188.
McFarland, W. N. and Loew, E. R. (
1994
). Ultraviolet visual pigments in marine fishes of the family Pomacentridae.
Vision Res.
34
,
1393
-1396.
McFarland, W. and Levin, S. A. (
2002
). Modelling the effects of current on prey acquisition in planktivorous fishes.
Mar. Fresh. Behav. Physiol.
35
,
69
-85.
Mussi, M., Summers, A. P. and Domenici, P.(
2002
). Gait transition speed, pectoral fin-beat frequency and amplitude in Cymatogaster aggregata, Embiotoca lateralis and Damalichthys vacca.
J. Fish Biol.
61
,
1282
-1293.
O'Brien, W. J., Slade, N. A. and Vinyard, G. L.(
1976
.). Apparent size as the determinant of prey selection by bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus).
Ecology
57
,
1304
-1310.
O'Driscoll, R. L. (
1998
). Feeding and schooling behaviour of barracouta (Thyrsites atun) off Otago, New Zealand.
Mar. Fresh. Res.
49
,
19
-24.
Raviv, D. (
2000
). The visual looming navigation cue: a unified approach.
Comp. Vis. Image Under.
79
,
331
-363.
Rice, W. R. (
1989
). Analysing tables of statistical tests.
Evolution
43
,
223
-225.
Ryer, C. H. and Olla, B. L. (
1991
). Information-transfer and the facilitation and inhibition of feeding in a schooling fish.
Env. Biol. Fish.
30
,
317
-323.
Sekuler, R., Pantle, A. and Levinson, E.(
1978
). Physiological basis of motion perception. In
Perception Handbook of Sensory Physiology
, vol.
VIII
, ch. 3 (ed. R. Held, H. W. Leibowitz and H. L. Teuber), pp.
67
-96. Berlin, Germany:Springer-Verlag.
Sekuler, R., Anstis, S. Braddick, O. J., Brandt, T., Movshon J. A. and Orban, G. (
1990
). The perception of motion. In
Visual Perception The Neurophysiological Foundations
,ch. 9 (ed. L. Spillmann and J. S. Werner), pp.
205
-230. San Diego, USA: Academic Press.
Shashar, N., Hanlon, R. T. and Petz, A. D.(
1998
). Polarization vision helps detect transparent prey.
Nature
393
,
222
-223.
Stevenson, R. A. (
1972
). Regulation of feeding behavior of the bicolour damselfish (Eupomacentrus partitus Poey) by environmental factors. In
Behavior of Marine Animals
,vol.
2
(ed. H. E. Winn and B. L. Olla). pp.
278
-302. New York: Plenum.
Thetmeyer, H. and Kils, U. (
1995
). To see and not be seen: the visibility of predator and prey with respect to feeding behavior.
Mar. Ecol. Progr. Ser.
126
,
1
-8.
Thresher, R. E. (
1983
). Environmental correlates of the distribution of planktivorous fishes in One Tree Reef lagoon.
Mar. Ecol. Progr. Ser.
10
,
137
-145.
Trager, G., Achituv, Y. and Genin, A. (
1994
). Effects of prey escape ability, flow speed, and predator feeding mode on zooplankton capture by barnacles.
Mar. Biol.
120
,
251
-259.
Tsuda, A., Saito, H. and Hirose, T. (
1998
). Effect of gut content on the vulnerability of copepods to visual predation.
Limn. Ocean.
43
,
1944
-1947.
Utne-Palm, A. C. (
1999
). The effect of prey mobility, prey contrast, turbidity and spectral composition on the reaction distance of Gobiusculus flavescens to its planktonic prey.
J. Fish Biol.
54
,
1244
-1258.
Utne-Palm, A. C. and Stiansen, J. E. (
2002
). Effect of larval ontogeny, turbulence and light on prey attack rate and swimming activity in herring larvae.
J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol.
268
,
147
-170.
Videen, G. and Ngo, D. (
1998
). Light scattering multipole solution for a cell.
J. Biomed. Optics
3
,
212
-220.
Vinyard, G. L. (
1980
). Differential prey vulnerability and predator selectivity: effects of evasive prey on blue gill(Lepomis macrochirus) and pumpkinseed (L. gibbosus)predation.
Can. J. Fish. Aqua. Sci.
37
,
2294
-2299.
Vinyard, G. L. (
1982
). Variable kinematics of Sacramento perch (Archoplites interruptus) capturing evasive and nonevasive prey.
Can. J. Fish. Aqua. Sci.
39
,
208
-211.
Webb, P. W. (
1982
). Avoidance responses of fathead minnow to strikes by four teleost predators.
J. Comp. Physiol.
147
,
371
-378.
Wheeler, T. G. (
1982
). Color vision and retinal chromatic information processing in teleost: a review.
Brain. Res. Rev.
4
,
177
-235.
Zaret, T. M. (
1972
). Predators, invisible prey,and the nature of polymorphisms in the Cladocera (Class Crustacea).
Limn. Ocean.
17
,
171
-184.