While morphological and behavioral feeding specializations are obvious in many vertebrate groups, among lizards there appear to be few dietary specialists. By comparing the prey capture kinematics and overall feeding behavior in two highly specialized ant-eating lizards (Moloch horridus and Phrynosoma platyrhinos) with those of two closely related dietary generalists (Pogona vitticeps and Uma notata), we investigate whether dietary specialization has been accompanied by changes in the function and use of the feeding system. We quantified kinematic variables from high-speed video recordings (200-250 frames s-1) of each species feeding on ants. Prey capture was strikingly different in M. horridus to that of other species, being characterized by a suite of unusual behaviors including the lack of a body lunge, faster tongue protrusion, reduced prey processing and, most notably,the ability to modulate the slow open phase of the gape cycle. In concert,these traits make a single feeding event in M. horridus faster than that in any other iguanian lizard studied to date. Prey capture behavior in P. platyrhinos is kinematically more similar to U. notataand P. vitticeps than to M. horridus, but the ant specialists are similar in that both lack distinct prey processing behaviors,resulting in faster overall capture and feeding events. While ant feeding in P. vitticeps is faster than feeding on other prey, the duration of a single feeding event is still four times longer than in either ant specialist,because of extensive prey processing. Additionally, a phylogenetic comparison of ant specialist lizards with dietary generalists revealed that ant-eating lizards require significantly less time to capture and process prey. Thus there are not only significant behavioral modifications in these ant-eating lizards, but also multiple strategies among specialists, suggesting differing selective pressures or phylogenetic constraints in the evolution of ant eating in lizards.

Over the past two decades, the feeding behavior of lizards has received increasing attention (reviewed by Schwenk,2000; Bels, 2003). Much of the focus of this body of research has been on testing the generalized tetrapod feeding model of Bramble and Wake(1985), which suggested a general pattern underlying feeding behavior in `lower' tetrapods. Most studies of lizard feeding behavior have since supported the presence of a basic pattern, but have also identified significant variation in feeding movements(e.g. Schwenk and Throckmorton,1989; Kraklau,1991; Urbani and Bels,1995; Delheusy and Bels,1999; McBrayer and Reilly,2002a). While these studies have laid a foundation for comparisons of lizard groups, they also reveal a striking gap in the literature regarding the feeding movements of dietarily specialized lizards. Whereas studies on generalized lizards are ideal for providing insights into basic patterns,studies on feeding systems associated with ecological specialization may allow us to identify evolutionary innovations or point towards constraints on the evolution of phenotypic diversity.

In many organisms, ecological specialization is reflected in morphological and behavioral adaptations (e.g. Losos,1990; Grant,1999). Although feeding specializations are obvious in many vertebrate groups such as mammals (e.g. herbivores, anteaters, carnivores,etc...), few dietary specialist groups appear to exist among lizards. While reviewing lizard diets, Greene(1982) found it difficult to describe a set of common features characterizing dietary specialists among lizards, primarily because of the preponderance of morphological generalists that appear to have specialized diets. Although Greene(1982) and later Schwenk(2000) suggested there were likely few adaptive characters in the feeding system of lizards related to ecological specialization, they agreed with Pianka and Parker(1975) that many of the unique morphological features exhibited by the North American horned lizards(Phrynosoma) and the Australian thorny devil Moloch horridusGray 1841 are likely adaptive traits, related to a specialized ant-eating diet.

Because most lizards include a diversity of prey (in both size and type) in their diet, relatively few items need to be eaten daily. However, due to the relatively small size and low caloric value of ants(Withers and Dickman, 1995),large quantities have to be consumed daily (e.g. up to 2000 ants per day by M. horridus). With the time constraints inherent in eating so many prey, we might expect ant-eating lizards to show distinct specializations,allowing them to minimize the duration of a feeding bout. Thus, myrmecophagous lizards appear ideally suited for investigating functional specializations of the feeding system in lizards.

Lizards of the genera Phrynosoma and Moloch have been considered a classic example of convergent evolution, both genera sharing a suite of morphological and behavioral characters considered to be`adaptations' to their myrmecophagous diet(Pianka and Parker, 1975; Pianka, 1986). Strikingly,relatively few of these `adaptations' (e.g. spiny body, cryptic coloration,reduced dentition, large stomach, dorso-ventrally flattened body) have been examined quantitatively in an explicit comparative or experimental framework. Only Montanucci's examination of Phrynosoma dentition(Montanucci, 1989) found any relationship between morphology and diet. Besides Montanucci's study, and a statement that Phrynosoma exhibit diet-based tongue modifications(Schwenk, 2000; Schwenk and Sherbrooke, 2003),there have been few predictions of what morphological or behavioral modifications of the feeding system might be exhibited by ant-eating lizards.

In this study, we examine the kinematics of prey capture and some aspects of ant-eating feeding behavior in four species of lizards. Two of the species, Moloch horridus (Agamidae) and the desert horned lizard Phrynosoma platyrhinos Girard 1852 (Phrynosomatidae) are considered ant specialists. Whereas the diet of M. horridus is exclusively ants(Withers and Dickman, 1995; Pianka and Pianka, 1970),non-ant prey may at times dominate the diet of P. platyrhinos(Pianka and Parker, 1975; J. J. Meyers and A. Herrel, personal observation). In addition, we include kinematic data for two non-ant-specialized lizards to test specifically for differences between each specialist and a closely related generalist species. The Australian bearded dragon Pogona vitticeps Ahl T1926 (Agamidae)and the fringe-toed lizard Uma notata Baird 1858 (Phrynosomatidae)are typical dietary generalists, including ants only occasionally in their diet (Greer, 1989; Cogger, 2000; Turner, 1998). By comparing the specialist species with these generalists, we will attempt to identify functional novelties in the feeding system that have allowed these lizards to successfully exploit ant prey as their dominant dietary resource.

Specimens and high-speed videography

The Moloch horridus Gray 1841 (N=4; snout-vent length,SVL=88.2, 94.7, 95.0, 99.1 mm) used in this study were housed at the Alice Springs Reptile Centre, Alice Springs, NT, Australia. Animals were kept in a large outdoor enclosure where ant prey were attracted using dead cockroaches. Unrestrained prey capture events were recorded using a portable JVC high-speed digital camera (Victory Company, Japan; 200 frames s-1). A ruler serving as a scale bar was placed in view whenever the animal or camera changed position. Juvenile Pogona vitticeps Ahl T1926 (N=3;SVL=72.0, 73.5, 77.5) and Phrynosoma platyrhinos Girard 1852(N=3; SVL=71.0, 77.5, 78.0) were purchased from commercial animal dealers, while the Uma notata Baird 1858 (N=2; SVL 85,97.25) were collected in the Yuma sand dunes in Arizona, USA. Unrestrained prey capture events of these individuals feeding on ants were filmed using a digital high-speed camera (Redlake Motionscope, San Diego, CA, USA; 250 frames s-1).

Kinematic analyses

A total of 27 kinematic variables were computed from the x,ycoordinates obtained from seven anatomical landmarks digitized on each frame. Landmarks were located at the eye, the tip of the upper jaw, the jaw vertex,the tip of the lower jaw, the throat surface directly below the jaw vertex,the tongue tip and a fixed point on the prey(Fig. 1). Feeding sequences were digitized using Didge Image Digitizing software (A. J. Cullum, 1999;Parthenogenetic Products). Only sequences in which the lateral aspect of the animal's head was approximately perpendicular to the camera were digitized.

Fig. 1.

M. horridus: points digitized and used to calculate kinematic variables. 1, eye; 2, tip of the upper jaw; 3, jaw vertex; 4, tip of lower jaw; 5, throat below jaw vertex; 6, tongue tip; 7, prey. Note the relatively long neck, especially when compared to the other species (see Figs 3A, 4A, 5A).

Fig. 1.

M. horridus: points digitized and used to calculate kinematic variables. 1, eye; 2, tip of the upper jaw; 3, jaw vertex; 4, tip of lower jaw; 5, throat below jaw vertex; 6, tongue tip; 7, prey. Note the relatively long neck, especially when compared to the other species (see Figs 3A, 4A, 5A).

All timing variables were computed relative to the onset of mouth opening,and included (1) the time until the onset of tongue protraction, (2) the time of prey contact, (3) the time of maximum tongue protrusion, (4) the time of maximum gape, (5) the time of the onset of tongue retraction, (6) the time of the onset of mouth closing, (7) the time to completion of tongue retraction,(8) and the time to completion of mouth closing. A total of 7 durations were calculated from timing variables: (1) the duration of mouth opening,calculated as the time from the onset of mouth opening until the time of maximum gape, (2) the duration of tongue protraction, calculated as the time from the onset of tongue protraction until the time of maximum tongue protraction, (3) the duration of tongue retraction, calculated as the time from the onset of tongue retraction until the completion of tongue retraction,(4) the duration of mouth closing, calculated as the time from the onset of mouth closing until the completion of mouth closing, and (5) the duration of prey capture, calculated from the onset of mouth opening until completion of mouth closing. In addition to these variables associated specifically with prey capture, we also calculated two duration variables associated with the duration of the entire feeding event: (6) the duration of prey processing,from the completion of prey capture to the completion of mouth closing after the last processing event, and (7) the duration of the entire feeding sequence, calculated as before, but including the duration of the capture event.

Maximal excursions and displacements were calculated directly from the x,y coordinates of the digitized points: (1) maximum gape angle,defined as the maximum angle between the upper jaw tip, vertex, and lower jaw tip, (2) maximal gape distance, calculated as the maximal linear distance between upper and lower jaw tips, (3) tongue reach, calculated as the maximal distance from the lower jaw tip to the tongue tip, (4) prey distance,calculated as the rectilinear distance from the upper jaw tip to the prey at the onset of the lunge, and (5) number of processing movements, calculated as the total number of gape cycles after prey capture. In addition, we calculated maximum velocities and accelerations of the lower jaw during opening (1,2) and closing (7,8) and of the tongue during protraction (3,4) and retraction (5,6). Raw displacement profiles were smoothed using a low-pass filter employing a zero phase shift, fourth-order Butterworth digital filter at 10 Hz(Winter, 1990). Velocities and accelerations were calculated from the filtered displacement data by taking the first and second derivative.

Statistical analysis

To visualize species differences in feeding kinematics, we performed a principal components analyses on the kinematic data set for all four species. We excluded the timing variables (variables 1-8 in Table 1) since they are mainly used to derive relevant duration variables. The Broken Stick method(Jackson, 1993) was used to determine the number of factors explaining a significant amount of variation in kinematic space. To determine how species differed in behavioral space, a MANOVA and subsequent univariate F-tests coupled with simultaneous Bonferonni post-hoc tests were performed on the factor scores using all significant axes. All variables were logarithmically transformed(log10) prior to analysis. Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS-PC v10.0.5 (Maria J. Norusis/SPSS Inc.).

Table 1.

Kinematic variables for each species feeding on ant prey

Moloch horridusPogona vitticepsPhrynosoma platyrhinosUma notata
Time from onset of mouth opening (s)     
(1) Onset of tongue protraction 0.025±0.018 0.108±0.018 0.113±0.074 0.095±0.021 
(2) Prey contact 0.055±0.019 0.241±0.061 0.218±0.100 0.166±0.028 
(3) Maximum tongue protrusion 0.059±0.019 0.244±0.062 0.220±0.101 0.166±0.031 
(4) Maximum gape 0.065±0.017 0.269±0.070 0.235±0.101 0.179±0.035 
(5) Onset of tongue retraction 0.064±0.017 0.249±0.065 0.228±0.102 0.174±0.029 
(6) Onset of mouth closing 0.074±0.017 0.277±0.072 0.241±0.100 0.185±0.034 
(7) Completion of tongue retraction 0.087±0.017 0.282±0.018 0.247±0.103 0.191±0.032 
(8) Completion of mouth closing 0.116±0.017 0.307±0.078 0.263±0.102 0.200±0.031 
Duration of stage     
(1) Mouth opening 0.064±0.018 0.239±0.088 0.216±0.107 0.179±0.035 
(2) Tongue protraction 0.035±0.004 0.136±0.043 0.106±0.031 0.071±0.010 
(3) Tongue retraction 0.023±0.003 0.033±0.006 0.019±0.004 0.017±0.002 
(4) Mouth closing 0.040±0.006 0.030±0.007 0.022±0.003 0.016±0.003 
(5) Prey capture 0.115±0.018 0.277±0.095 0.244±0.107 0.200±0.031 
(6) Prey processing 0.257±0.058 1.764±0.276 0.164±0.061 1.148±0.439 
(7) Feeding sequence 0.350±0.054 2.109±0.339 0.416±0.130 1.353±0.441 
Maximal excursions and displacements     
(1) Gape angle (degrees) 47.21±2.82 29.45±8.06 50.87±3.93 24.11±0.58 
(2) Absolute gape (cm) 0.529±0.045 0.752±0.165 0.657±0.036 0.609±0.020 
(3) Tongue reach (cm) 0.375±0.040 0.547±0.025 0.459±0.048 0.440±0.038 
(4) Distance to prey (cm) 0.684±0.045 1.757±0.287 1.462±0.398 3.434±1.394 
(5) Number processing/transport movements 2.350±0.054 8.125±1.552 0.222±0.666 6.900±1.197 
Maximal speed variables     
(1) Mouth opening velocity (cm s-111.92±1.69 10.16±4.22 9.00±0.43 7.44±0.24 
(2) Mouth opening acceleration (cm s2578.99±92.12 531.57±109.81 509.46±132.21 554.32±400.65 
(3) Tongue protraction velocity (cm s-116.20±0.82 9.39±1.24 10.00±1.94 9.232±0.05 
(4) Tongue protraction acceleration (cm s22135.29±365.49 376.86±161.49 651.55±95.60 595.32±4.90 
(5) Tongue retraction velocity (cm s-115.38±2.21 25.13±14.24 19.03±7.41 7.06±1.06 
(6) Tongue retraction acceleration (cm s22385.62±188.17 3094.70±2081.95 2703.98±800.25 915.45±202.78 
(7) Mouth closing velocity (cm s-113.63±2.01 29.28±5.71 28.570±2.12 22.872±4.20 
(8) Mouth closing acceleration (cm s2825.88±247.35 1701.69±752.71 2530.30±60.25 1469.46±510.66 
Moloch horridusPogona vitticepsPhrynosoma platyrhinosUma notata
Time from onset of mouth opening (s)     
(1) Onset of tongue protraction 0.025±0.018 0.108±0.018 0.113±0.074 0.095±0.021 
(2) Prey contact 0.055±0.019 0.241±0.061 0.218±0.100 0.166±0.028 
(3) Maximum tongue protrusion 0.059±0.019 0.244±0.062 0.220±0.101 0.166±0.031 
(4) Maximum gape 0.065±0.017 0.269±0.070 0.235±0.101 0.179±0.035 
(5) Onset of tongue retraction 0.064±0.017 0.249±0.065 0.228±0.102 0.174±0.029 
(6) Onset of mouth closing 0.074±0.017 0.277±0.072 0.241±0.100 0.185±0.034 
(7) Completion of tongue retraction 0.087±0.017 0.282±0.018 0.247±0.103 0.191±0.032 
(8) Completion of mouth closing 0.116±0.017 0.307±0.078 0.263±0.102 0.200±0.031 
Duration of stage     
(1) Mouth opening 0.064±0.018 0.239±0.088 0.216±0.107 0.179±0.035 
(2) Tongue protraction 0.035±0.004 0.136±0.043 0.106±0.031 0.071±0.010 
(3) Tongue retraction 0.023±0.003 0.033±0.006 0.019±0.004 0.017±0.002 
(4) Mouth closing 0.040±0.006 0.030±0.007 0.022±0.003 0.016±0.003 
(5) Prey capture 0.115±0.018 0.277±0.095 0.244±0.107 0.200±0.031 
(6) Prey processing 0.257±0.058 1.764±0.276 0.164±0.061 1.148±0.439 
(7) Feeding sequence 0.350±0.054 2.109±0.339 0.416±0.130 1.353±0.441 
Maximal excursions and displacements     
(1) Gape angle (degrees) 47.21±2.82 29.45±8.06 50.87±3.93 24.11±0.58 
(2) Absolute gape (cm) 0.529±0.045 0.752±0.165 0.657±0.036 0.609±0.020 
(3) Tongue reach (cm) 0.375±0.040 0.547±0.025 0.459±0.048 0.440±0.038 
(4) Distance to prey (cm) 0.684±0.045 1.757±0.287 1.462±0.398 3.434±1.394 
(5) Number processing/transport movements 2.350±0.054 8.125±1.552 0.222±0.666 6.900±1.197 
Maximal speed variables     
(1) Mouth opening velocity (cm s-111.92±1.69 10.16±4.22 9.00±0.43 7.44±0.24 
(2) Mouth opening acceleration (cm s2578.99±92.12 531.57±109.81 509.46±132.21 554.32±400.65 
(3) Tongue protraction velocity (cm s-116.20±0.82 9.39±1.24 10.00±1.94 9.232±0.05 
(4) Tongue protraction acceleration (cm s22135.29±365.49 376.86±161.49 651.55±95.60 595.32±4.90 
(5) Tongue retraction velocity (cm s-115.38±2.21 25.13±14.24 19.03±7.41 7.06±1.06 
(6) Tongue retraction acceleration (cm s22385.62±188.17 3094.70±2081.95 2703.98±800.25 915.45±202.78 
(7) Mouth closing velocity (cm s-113.63±2.01 29.28±5.71 28.570±2.12 22.872±4.20 
(8) Mouth closing acceleration (cm s2825.88±247.35 1701.69±752.71 2530.30±60.25 1469.46±510.66 

The numbers in each section refer to text descriptions.

Values are means ± s.d. The number of individuals (number of sequences digitized) for all individuals are 4(20) for M. horridus, 3(8) for P. vitticep, 3 (9) P. platyrhinos and 2 (10) for U. notata.

To address whether the feeding kinematics of ant-eating lizards has diverged from that of dietary generalists, we analyzed a subset of variables within an explicit phylogenetic framework. We assembled a data set for a select set of variables (Duration variables in Table 1), including the duration of mouth opening, duration of mouth closing, duration of prey capture and the duration of the entire feeding event. These variables were chosen because they loaded highly on the PC axes and most importantly, we could obtain similar values in the literature for other species. In addition to the species examined in this study, we were able to extract data for the following taxa using both literature data and personal, previously unpublished data: Anolis carolinensis, Anolis sagrei, Chamaeleo jacksonii, Crotaphytus collaris, Dipsosaurus dorsalis, M. horridus, Phrynosoma cornutum, P. platyrhinos, P. vitticeps, Pseudotrapelus sinaitus, Sceloporus undulatus, U. notata and Urosaurus ornatus (see Fig. 2). Since our goal is to compare ant-eaters to non-ant eaters, in the subsequent analyses we coded the ant-eaters (M. horridus, P. cornutum, P. platyrhinos) and non-anteaters (all others) differently. For many other species, we were able to find data for all variables except total feeding duration. While these species were not included in the phylogenetic analysis, we included them in Table 3 for comparative purposes.

Fig. 2.

Composite phylogeny used to address the evolution of feeding behavior (see Table 3) between ant specialists and dietary generalists. Broken branches denote ant specialists,while all others are considered dietary generalists.

Fig. 2.

Composite phylogeny used to address the evolution of feeding behavior (see Table 3) between ant specialists and dietary generalists. Broken branches denote ant specialists,while all others are considered dietary generalists.

Table 3.

Summary of the duration of each kinematic phase during prey capture,prey capture duration, total feeding duration, prehension mode and prey type for feeding studies of lizards

Duration (ms)
Kinematic phase (%)
SpeciesSOFOFCCaptureFeedingPMPrey typeReference
Rhyncocephalia         
Sphenodon punctatus 280 (39) 200 (28) 150 (21) 720  Schwenk, 2000 
Iguania         
Agamidae         
Agama agama 460 (85) 40 (7) 45 (8) 545  cr Kraklau, 1991 
Agama stellio 265 (71) 38 (10) 32 (9) 373  cr Herrel et al., 1995 
Pogona vitticeps 230 (75) 34 (12) 39 (13) 277 2100 ant This study 
     6500 cr J.J. Meyers and A. Herrel unpublished data 
Pseudotrapelus sinaitus 333 (84) 33 (8) 33 (8) 399 2400 cr Meyers and Nishikawa, 2000 
Moloch horridus  65 (61) 43 (39) 108 350 ant This study 
Phrynocephalus helioscopus 155 (64) 40 (16) 50 (20) 245  mw Schwenk and Throckmorton, 1989 
Chamaeleontidae         
Chamaeleo jacksonii 1000 (84) 92 (8) 100 (8) 1192 10990 cr Meyers and Nishikawa, 2000 
Crotaphtyidae         
Crotaphytus collaris 117 (59) 50 (25) 33 (16) 200 2200 cr A. Herrel, unpublished data 
Iguanidae         
Dipsosaurus dorsalis 290 (80) 45 (12) 30 (8) 365  ww Schwenk and Throckmorton, 1989 
 317 (68) 83 (18) 67 (14) 467 18500 cr A. Herrel, unpublished data 
Phrynosomatidae         
Phrynosoma cornutum 115 (62) 30 (16) 40 (22) 185 410 cr Schwenk, 2000 
Phrynosoma platyrhinos 202 (72) 33 (14) 32 (14) 244 416 ant This study 
Sceloporus undulatus 167 (67) 33 (13) 50 (20) 250 3570 cr Meyers et al., 2002 
Uma notata 119 (56) 69 (32) 25 (13) 203 1353 ant This study 
Urosaurus ornatus 233 (74) 33 (10) 50 (16) 317 8900 cr A. Herrel, unpublished data 
Polychrotidae         
Anolis equestris 150 (46) 70 (22) 105 (32) 325  gh Bels, 1990 
Anolis carolinensis 220 (65) 80 (24) 40 (11) 340  J? Bels and Goosse, 1990 
     17200 cr Herrel unpublished data 
Anolis sagrei 350 (84) 33 (8) 33 (8) 416 6330 cr A. Herrel, unpublished data 
Opluridae         
Oplurus cuvieri 370 (82) 40 (9) 40 (9) 450  cr Delheusy and Bels, 1992 
Duration (ms)
Kinematic phase (%)
SpeciesSOFOFCCaptureFeedingPMPrey typeReference
Rhyncocephalia         
Sphenodon punctatus 280 (39) 200 (28) 150 (21) 720  Schwenk, 2000 
Iguania         
Agamidae         
Agama agama 460 (85) 40 (7) 45 (8) 545  cr Kraklau, 1991 
Agama stellio 265 (71) 38 (10) 32 (9) 373  cr Herrel et al., 1995 
Pogona vitticeps 230 (75) 34 (12) 39 (13) 277 2100 ant This study 
     6500 cr J.J. Meyers and A. Herrel unpublished data 
Pseudotrapelus sinaitus 333 (84) 33 (8) 33 (8) 399 2400 cr Meyers and Nishikawa, 2000 
Moloch horridus  65 (61) 43 (39) 108 350 ant This study 
Phrynocephalus helioscopus 155 (64) 40 (16) 50 (20) 245  mw Schwenk and Throckmorton, 1989 
Chamaeleontidae         
Chamaeleo jacksonii 1000 (84) 92 (8) 100 (8) 1192 10990 cr Meyers and Nishikawa, 2000 
Crotaphtyidae         
Crotaphytus collaris 117 (59) 50 (25) 33 (16) 200 2200 cr A. Herrel, unpublished data 
Iguanidae         
Dipsosaurus dorsalis 290 (80) 45 (12) 30 (8) 365  ww Schwenk and Throckmorton, 1989 
 317 (68) 83 (18) 67 (14) 467 18500 cr A. Herrel, unpublished data 
Phrynosomatidae         
Phrynosoma cornutum 115 (62) 30 (16) 40 (22) 185 410 cr Schwenk, 2000 
Phrynosoma platyrhinos 202 (72) 33 (14) 32 (14) 244 416 ant This study 
Sceloporus undulatus 167 (67) 33 (13) 50 (20) 250 3570 cr Meyers et al., 2002 
Uma notata 119 (56) 69 (32) 25 (13) 203 1353 ant This study 
Urosaurus ornatus 233 (74) 33 (10) 50 (16) 317 8900 cr A. Herrel, unpublished data 
Polychrotidae         
Anolis equestris 150 (46) 70 (22) 105 (32) 325  gh Bels, 1990 
Anolis carolinensis 220 (65) 80 (24) 40 (11) 340  J? Bels and Goosse, 1990 
     17200 cr Herrel unpublished data 
Anolis sagrei 350 (84) 33 (8) 33 (8) 416 6330 cr A. Herrel, unpublished data 
Opluridae         
Oplurus cuvieri 370 (82) 40 (9) 40 (9) 450  cr Delheusy and Bels, 1992 

Percent of capture duration is noted in parentheses.

Durations of kinematic profiles are taken from generalized feeding sequences in the cited studies. In addition, from our own studies, we have provided feeding durations of other lizards.

Prehension mode (PM) refers to (J) jaw or (T) tongue prehension.

Prey types are cricket (cr), waxworm (ww), mealworm (mw), grasshopper (gh)and ant.

Because species share a phylogenetic history, they cannot be considered independent data points and thus species cannot be compared using standard F-distributions. To address this concern we used the Phenotypic Diversity Analysis Programs (PDAP; Garland et al., 1992), which allows us to first construct a phylogenetic tree with trait values at the tips, run simulations of character evolution taking these phylogenetic relationships into account, perform ANOVAs on the simulations to create an empirical null distribution of F-statistics and, lastly, compute the 95th percentile of the null distribution to compare with the results of traditional analysis of variance (ANOVA).

The method described first requires construction of a phylogenetic tree,which was made in PDTREE and based on literature data depicting the relationships of the species in our analysis(Macey et al., 2000; Schulte et al., 2003; see Fig. 2). Relationships among iguanid clades were considered a hard polytomy (see Schulte et al., 2003). Because divergence times between all groups/species in our analysis were not available, we set all branch lengths to unity(Diaz-Uriarte and Garland,1998). While errors in branch lengths usually do not have a substantial effect on the analysis(Martins and Garland, 1991; Diaz-Uriarte and Garland,1998), we confirmed that our branch lengths were adequately standardized using the diagnostic tests in PDTREE(Garland et al., 1992).

Simulation analyses were performed using the PDSIMUL and PDANOVA programs of Garland et al. (1993). The tree, with trait values at the tips, was input into the PDSIMUL program where we then selected the Brownian motion model of evolutionary change using the default values (means and variances of the original data). Note that, when the branch lengths are set to unity, this corresponds to a speciational model of evolution, which assumes sudden `jumps' along the tree during speciation events. We ran 1000 unbounded simulations of character evolution and then performed ANOVAs on these simulations using PDANOVA. F-statistics from the phylogenetic ANOVAs were used to create an empirical null distribution from which we computed the 95th percentile and used these phylogenetically corrected critical values (α=0.05) to determine significance. A univariate ANOVA was then performed on the original data and the Ftrad-values from this analysis were compared to the critical Fphy-values obtained from the simulation analyses. Values were considered statistically significant if the non-phylogenetic Ftrad-value was greater than the critical Fphyl-value of the empirical F-distribution.

General feeding behavior

Here we provide a brief description of prey capture in P. vitticeps and then describe how the other three species compare to this general pattern. The gape profile during prey capture in P. vitticepsis similar to that described for other iguanian lizards (e.g. Schwenk and Throckmorton,1989; Delheusy and Bels,1992; Herrel et al.,1995) and displays the kinematic phases characteristic of the generalized lower tetrapod feeding model(Bramble and Wake, 1985). Although this model was originally developed to describe prey processing and transport, it has been aptly applied to prey capture as well (Delheusy and Bels, 1992, 1999; Herrel et al., 1995; Smith et al., 1999) and will be used here to describe the general features of the prey capture cycle in the four species. However, in our descriptions, we combined the slow open I (SOI)and slow open II (SOII) phases, as they were not always clearly distinguishable during prey capture events. To help delineate the kinematic phases, McBrayer and Reilly(2002b) proposed a method using slope analysis. While useful, the magnitude of differences noted in the animals used in this study was so great that this method was deemed unnecessary.

A typical feeding event in P. vitticeps is initiated when the animal sees, recognizes and orients itself toward a prey item. The mouth is then opened slowly and the tongue is protracted slowly (i.e. start of slow open phase, SO). The duration of the first part of this phase can be highly variable while the animal observes the prey and determines whether to capture it or not. During the SO phase, capture events are sometimes aborted. Once the animal decides to capture the prey, however, it lunges forward, rotating over the forelimbs and simultaneously protruding the tongue at increasing speeds. The tongue is protruded maximally at, or near the time of prey contact (end of SO). As tongue retraction is initiated, the mouth opens rapidly (fast open phase, FO). Maximum gape or mouth opening typically occurs as the prey enters the buccal cavity, after which the mouth closes rapidly around the prey (fast close phase, FC). During the FC phase, the animal moves back to its resting position. When the teeth come into contact with the prey, mouth closing slows(slow close phase, SC) and the prey is crushed(Fig. 3A).

Fig. 3.

Typical ant feeding event in P. vitticeps. (A) P. vitticeps capturing ant prey. Time (s) is given in the upper right of each frame. (B) Selected kinematic profiles. The gape distance profile is similar to all iguanians but very different than M. horridus. Colored dots denote onset of mouth opening (red), prey contact with the tongue(yellow) and maximum gape (blue). Onset of slow open (SO), fast open (FO) and fast close phases (FC) are denoted by dotted lines.

Fig. 3.

Typical ant feeding event in P. vitticeps. (A) P. vitticeps capturing ant prey. Time (s) is given in the upper right of each frame. (B) Selected kinematic profiles. The gape distance profile is similar to all iguanians but very different than M. horridus. Colored dots denote onset of mouth opening (red), prey contact with the tongue(yellow) and maximum gape (blue). Onset of slow open (SO), fast open (FO) and fast close phases (FC) are denoted by dotted lines.

Prey processing and transport occur in the buccal cavity, and these behaviors continue in a rhythmic fashion until the prey is finally transported to the back of the buccal cavity and swallowed. We combined prey processing and transport into one behavioral category because of the lack of these behaviors in ant specialists and because of the difficulties in adequately distinguishing between these behaviors without the use of cineradiography.

Of the other three species, U. notata is the most similar in feeding movements to P. vitticeps, exhibiting the same capture and processing movements. However, U. notata is substantially faster during prey capture as well as prey processing(Fig. 4A), making a single feeding event shorter in duration when compared to P. vitticeps. Like in U. notata, the prey capture movements of P. platyrhinosare faster than those of P. vitticeps, with the most pronounced difference in P. platyrhinos occurring near the end of prey capture and during prey processing. Unlike P. vitticeps and U. notata, which close their mandibles around the prey, P. platyrhinos transports the prey directly into the posterior buccal cavity after the capture event, and no crushing bites are performed(Fig. 5A). Because processing does not occur in P. platyrhinos the overall duration of the feeding sequence is highly reduced (see Table 1).

Fig. 4.

Typical ant feeding event in U. notata. (A) U. notatacapturing ant prey. Time (s) is given in the lower right of each frame. (B)Selected kinematic profiles. The gape distance profile is similar to P. vitticeps and P. platyrhinos but very different than M. horridus. Colored dots denote onset of mouth opening (red), prey contact with the tongue (yellow) and maximum gape (blue). Onset of slow open (SO),fast open (FO) and fast close phases (FC) are denoted by dotted lines.

Fig. 4.

Typical ant feeding event in U. notata. (A) U. notatacapturing ant prey. Time (s) is given in the lower right of each frame. (B)Selected kinematic profiles. The gape distance profile is similar to P. vitticeps and P. platyrhinos but very different than M. horridus. Colored dots denote onset of mouth opening (red), prey contact with the tongue (yellow) and maximum gape (blue). Onset of slow open (SO),fast open (FO) and fast close phases (FC) are denoted by dotted lines.

Fig. 5.

Typical ant feeding event in P. platyrhinos. (A) P. platyrhinos capturing ant prey. Time (s) is given in the lower right of each frame. As in all iguanian feeding events, prey capture occurs using the tongue. Upon tongue retraction, the rotation of the dorsal papillae is visible as the prey is transported into the mouth. Note the characteristic shape of the tongue, which is very similar to P. vitticeps and U. notata but unlike M. horridus. (B) Selected kinematic profiles. The gape distance profile is typical of iguanians showing the slow open phase before prey contact. Colored dots denote onset of mouth opening (red), prey contact with the tongue (yellow) and maximum gape (blue). Onset of slow open(SO), fast open (FO) and fast close phases (FC) are denoted by dotted lines.

Fig. 5.

Typical ant feeding event in P. platyrhinos. (A) P. platyrhinos capturing ant prey. Time (s) is given in the lower right of each frame. As in all iguanian feeding events, prey capture occurs using the tongue. Upon tongue retraction, the rotation of the dorsal papillae is visible as the prey is transported into the mouth. Note the characteristic shape of the tongue, which is very similar to P. vitticeps and U. notata but unlike M. horridus. (B) Selected kinematic profiles. The gape distance profile is typical of iguanians showing the slow open phase before prey contact. Colored dots denote onset of mouth opening (red), prey contact with the tongue (yellow) and maximum gape (blue). Onset of slow open(SO), fast open (FO) and fast close phases (FC) are denoted by dotted lines.

A feeding event in M. horridus is strikingly different from those observed for the other three species. First, M. horridus does not lunge during prey capture. Instead, the body generally remains stationary while the long neck swivels the head within range of the prey(Fig. 6A). Furthermore, the jaws open quickly and the tongue is protruded rapidly. During the initial stages of mouth opening, M. horridus frequently modulates its behavior. In most cases, the SO phase was not discernable (more than 80% of the sequences) and the gape cycle merely consisted of FO and FC phases. When present, the slow open phase was typically associated with stationary ant prey(e.g. ants that were slightly injured or attempting to move a food item),which appears atypical of most ant prey eaten by M. horridus (see Discussion). Interestingly, in the few capture events in which a distinct SO phase was present, we did not see simultaneous slow tongue protraction, as in the other species. In M. horridus, prey contact always occurred close to maximum gape and was not typically followed by the initiation of the FO phase. Once the prey is moved into the buccal cavity, one or two rapid`palatal crushing' movements typically occur before swallowing. As in P. platyrhinos, the lack of traditional prey processing reduces the overall duration of the feeding sequence in M. horridus compared to the two generalist species (see Table 1).

Fig. 6.

Typical ant feeding event in Moloch horridus. (A) M. horridus capturing ant prey. Time (s) is given in the lower right of each frame. Note the pointy shape of the tongue at full extension, uncharacteristic of iguanians during prey capture but very similar to iguanians when tongue flicking. The entire capture event occurs in just over 100 ms. (B) Selected kinematic profiles, showing the abbreviated slow open phase (only 20% of the cylces contained a slow open phase) and the longer fast open and fast close phases. Colored dots denote the onset of mouth opening (red), prey contact with the tongue (yellow) and maximum gape (blue). Onset of fast open (FO) and fast close phases (FC) are denoted by dotted lines. Note the difference in time scale between Figs 2 and 3.

Fig. 6.

Typical ant feeding event in Moloch horridus. (A) M. horridus capturing ant prey. Time (s) is given in the lower right of each frame. Note the pointy shape of the tongue at full extension, uncharacteristic of iguanians during prey capture but very similar to iguanians when tongue flicking. The entire capture event occurs in just over 100 ms. (B) Selected kinematic profiles, showing the abbreviated slow open phase (only 20% of the cylces contained a slow open phase) and the longer fast open and fast close phases. Colored dots denote the onset of mouth opening (red), prey contact with the tongue (yellow) and maximum gape (blue). Onset of fast open (FO) and fast close phases (FC) are denoted by dotted lines. Note the difference in time scale between Figs 2 and 3.

Variation in feeding kinematics

Prey capture occurs most quickly in M. horridus, with the total prey capture cycle completed in less than 115 ms, but taking up to 2.5 times longer in the other species (Table 1). The SO phase is highly variable in duration and is the predominant reason for variation in prey capture duration. It may comprise anywhere between 50% and 89% of the total prey capture sequence in P. vitticeps, U. notata and P. platyrhinos. However, in M. horridus prey capture duration is shortened by the lack, or shortening,of the SO phase. In the few cases in which an SO phase is present in M. horridus, it occurs at a lower gape and proceeds more rapidly than in the other species, representing only 38% of the prey capture sequence(Table 2).

Table 2.

Kinematic phases (SO, FO, FC) during prey capture

Moloch horridus
Pogona vitticeps
Phrynosoma platyrhinos
Uma notata
PhaseTime (s)%Time (s)%Time (s)%Time (s)%
Slow open (SO) 0.055±0.018 38 0.230±0.066 75 0.202±0.124 72 0.119±0.028 56 
Fast open (FO) 0.057±0.009 35 0.034±0.013 12 0.033±0.014 14 0.069±0.017 32 
Fast close (FC) 0.043±0.008 27 0.039±0.005 13 0.032±0.004 14 0.025±0.007 13 
Duration of prey capture (s) 0.151±0.029  0.277±0.095  0.244±0.107  0.203±0.040  
Slow open (SO)       
Fast open (FO) 0.065±0.006 61       
Fast close (FC) 0.043±0.006 39       
Duration of prey capture (s) 0.104±0.013        
Moloch horridus
Pogona vitticeps
Phrynosoma platyrhinos
Uma notata
PhaseTime (s)%Time (s)%Time (s)%Time (s)%
Slow open (SO) 0.055±0.018 38 0.230±0.066 75 0.202±0.124 72 0.119±0.028 56 
Fast open (FO) 0.057±0.009 35 0.034±0.013 12 0.033±0.014 14 0.069±0.017 32 
Fast close (FC) 0.043±0.008 27 0.039±0.005 13 0.032±0.004 14 0.025±0.007 13 
Duration of prey capture (s) 0.151±0.029  0.277±0.095  0.244±0.107  0.203±0.040  
Slow open (SO)       
Fast open (FO) 0.065±0.006 61       
Fast close (FC) 0.043±0.006 39       
Duration of prey capture (s) 0.104±0.013        

Values are means ± S.D. The number of individuals (number of sequences digitized) for all individuals are 3 (8) for P. vitticeps,3 (9) for P. platyrhinos and 2 (10) for U. notata.

For M. horridus, values of profiles exhibiting a SO phase (4 trials) and those without a slow open phase (16 trials) are given.

The FO phase of prey capture typically commences after prey contact, but the timing of the FO phase varies considerably among species. In P. vitticeps, prey contact always occurs before the FO phase(Fig. 3B), whereas in U. notata and P. platyrhinos the timing of prey contact and the FO phase are more variable (Figs 4B, 5B). In M. horridus,prey contact always occurs near the end of the FO phase(Fig. 6B). The lack of an SO phase in M. horridus seems to have resulted in a somewhat longer FO phase, comprising 55% of the cycle, compared to only 12-32% in other species(Table 2). Mean duration of the FO phase in M. horridus is 63 ms, which is similar to U. notata but almost twice as long as that of the other two species. The end of FO and start of FC occur as tongue retraction ends and the animal reaches maximum gape. The FC phase exhibits the least variation within and among all species and appears to be more `stereotyped' than the opening phases (Tables 2, 3). A SC phase was not noticeable in most sequences and may not be characteristic of prey capture events associated with small prey.

For many of the timing variables, M. horridus exhibits shorter durations than the other three species. Yet, surprisingly the shorter timing events do not appear to be the result of more rapid feeding movements. While mouth opening duration was shorter in M. horridus, maximum mouth opening speeds were not different among species(Table 1). Tongue protraction occurs more quickly in M. horridus (16.20 cm s-1) than any of the other species, P. platyrhinos (10.00 cm s-1), U. notata (9.23 cm s-1) and P. vitticeps (9.39 cm s-1). However, tongue retraction and mouth closing velocities and accelerations either did not differ between species or were generally faster in P. platyrhinos (28.57 cm s-1) and P. vitticeps(29.28 cm s-1).

Although prey capture in U. notata is faster than that of all other species except M. horridus, the processing movements and duration of prey processing result in a feeding event that is three times longer than either of the ant specialists. Processing in P. vitticepsis even slower, requiring nearly eight times longer than M. horridusto process prey (Table 1). The duration of prey processing appears to separate the ant specialist from the generalist lizard species. On average, M. horridus takes only 257 ms to process prey after capture. P. platyrhinos is even faster, taking only 164 ms to process and transport prey. In contrast, both of the generalist species take more than 1000 ms. The lack of this rapid processing behavior significantly reduces the duration of the entire feeding sequence and makes the total duration of a feeding attempt in P. platyrhinos (416 ms)similar to M. horridus (350 ms). The primary differences in processing times are due to a complete lack of prey crushing and a decrease in the number of processing/transport cycles in both M. horridus and P. platyrhinos (only 0-3 cycles in the ant specialists). Unlike the ant specialist species, P. vitticeps and U. notata crush the ants extensively before transporting them into the posterior buccal cavity for swallowing (processing and transport can take 7-11 cycles).

While all species capture prey using the tongue, there are distinct differences in the part of the tongue that contacts the prey. Of the four species, only M. horridus appears to be lacking the robust, fleshy tongue characteristic of iguanian lizards. During normal tongue protrusion,the dorsal surface of the tongue rotates forward, such that the dorsal tongue pad and not the anatomical tongue tip contacts the prey. Rotation of the tongue pad is easy to see during tongue retraction, when the prey rotates posteriorly on the dorsal surface of the tongue and is transported into the mouth (see Figs 3A, 4A, 5A). M. horridusexhibits a strikingly different tongue prehension behavior. First, the shape of the tongue during protrusion is different in M. horridus,appearing more slender and pointed. In addition, the tongue pad does not appear to rotate, and instead, the prey is flicked back into the mouth using the tongue tip (Fig. 6A). Observations of M. horridus trying to capture ants held lightly by forceps suggest limited adhesive properties of the tongue.

Quantitative analysis

A principal components analysis on the kinematic data revealed three significant factors which explained 71% of the variation in feeding kinematics(Fig. 7). The first factor explains 45% of the variation and separates the ant specialist from the dietary generalists. This factor is mainly composed of duration events, but also includes gape distance and mouth opening/closing speeds(Table 4). The other two axes,while also significant, are defined by lower factor loadings that consist of mouth closing and tongue retraction events. Results of a MANOVA on the factor scores revealed highly significant species effects (Wilks' Lambda F=83.74, P<0.0001). Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed significant differences among ant specialists and between the specialist and generalist species for factor 1 (P<0.0001), yet the generalist species did not differ from one another (P=1.00). The other two factors appear to separate the species phylogenetically with all species comparisons being different (P<0.006) except for the agamids P. vitticeps and M. horridus on factor 2(P=0.078) and the phrynosomatids U. notata and P. platyrhinos on factor 3 (P=0.496).

Fig. 7.

Plot of the three significant axes from the factor analysis showing the position of the different species in kinematic space. The ant-eating species(Moloch and Phrynosoma) are significantly different than the generalists (Pogona and Uma) on axis 1. The other two axes appear to separate the species phylogenetically, with all species comparisons being different except for the agamids P. vitticeps and M. horridus on axis 2 and the phrynosomatids U. notata and P. platyrhinos on axis 3.

Fig. 7.

Plot of the three significant axes from the factor analysis showing the position of the different species in kinematic space. The ant-eating species(Moloch and Phrynosoma) are significantly different than the generalists (Pogona and Uma) on axis 1. The other two axes appear to separate the species phylogenetically, with all species comparisons being different except for the agamids P. vitticeps and M. horridus on axis 2 and the phrynosomatids U. notata and P. platyrhinos on axis 3.

Table 4.

Eigenvalues, percentage of the variation explained and component loadings of the three significant factors based on a principal components analysis of the kinematic data

Factor
123
Eigenvalue 9.075 3.024 2.218 
% variation explained 45.373 15.119 11.088 
Duration of mouth opening 0.871 0.329 -0.045 
Duration of tongue protraction 0.832 0.435 0.050 
Duration of tongue retraction 0.030 0.371 0.661 
Duration of mouth closing -0.633 0.194 0.507 
Duration of prey capture 0.817 0.379 0.058 
Maximum gape angle -0.742 0.519 -0.136 
Absolute gape 0.602 0.497 0.044 
Maximum tongue reach 0.427 0.246 0.203 
Distance to prey 0.818 -0.191 -0.209 
Mouth opening velocity -0.655 0.225 -0.017 
Mouth opening acceleration -0.204 0.136 -0.374 
Tongue protraction velocity -0.883 -0.013 0.036 
Tongue protraction accleration -0.926 -0.156 -0.072 
Tongue retraction velocity -0.342 0.609 0.523 
Tongue retraction acceleration -0.501 0.545 0.392 
Mouth closing velocity 0.712 0.489 -0.291 
Mouth closing acceleration 0.389 0.562 -0.507 
Duration of prey processing 0.733 -0.387 0.419 
Duration of feeding sequence 0.872 -0.195 0.320 
Number of processing/transport movements 0.599 -0.537 0.465 
Factor
123
Eigenvalue 9.075 3.024 2.218 
% variation explained 45.373 15.119 11.088 
Duration of mouth opening 0.871 0.329 -0.045 
Duration of tongue protraction 0.832 0.435 0.050 
Duration of tongue retraction 0.030 0.371 0.661 
Duration of mouth closing -0.633 0.194 0.507 
Duration of prey capture 0.817 0.379 0.058 
Maximum gape angle -0.742 0.519 -0.136 
Absolute gape 0.602 0.497 0.044 
Maximum tongue reach 0.427 0.246 0.203 
Distance to prey 0.818 -0.191 -0.209 
Mouth opening velocity -0.655 0.225 -0.017 
Mouth opening acceleration -0.204 0.136 -0.374 
Tongue protraction velocity -0.883 -0.013 0.036 
Tongue protraction accleration -0.926 -0.156 -0.072 
Tongue retraction velocity -0.342 0.609 0.523 
Tongue retraction acceleration -0.501 0.545 0.392 
Mouth closing velocity 0.712 0.489 -0.291 
Mouth closing acceleration 0.389 0.562 -0.507 
Duration of prey processing 0.733 -0.387 0.419 
Duration of feeding sequence 0.872 -0.195 0.320 
Number of processing/transport movements 0.599 -0.537 0.465 

The results of our phylogenetic ANOVAs comparing the feeding duration variables of ant-eating and non ant-eating lizards are summarized in Table 5. F-statistics from the traditional analysis were compared to the empirical null distributions obtained from the 1000 simulations of character evolution along the phylogenetic tree (Fig. 2). The analyses revealed significant differences between ant-eaters and other lizards for the duration of mouth opening and total feeding duration. In ant-eaters, the mean duration of mouth opening and total feeding duration (149 and 392 ms) are much shorter than for non ant-eating lizards (361 and 7354 ms). Although not quite significant (P=0.051), prey capture duration also appeared to differ, being more than twice as fast in ant-eaters (181 ms) versus non ant-eaters (408 ms). The phylogenetic analysis thus confirms the results of our multivariate analysis and suggests that ant-eating lizards exhibit divergent feeding behaviors associated with their diet.

Table 5.

Results of phylogenetic analysis comparing the kinematic feeding durations of ant-eating lizards and lizards that are dietary generalists

VariableFtradPtradFphylPphyl
Mouth opening duration* 5.31 0.042 5.01 ∼0.039 
Mouth closing duration 0.59 0.459 4.80 ∼0.426 
Prey capture duration 4.79 0.051 5.20 <0.051 
Feeding duration 20.68 <0.001 4.80 <0.001 
VariableFtradPtradFphylPphyl
Mouth opening duration* 5.31 0.042 5.01 ∼0.039 
Mouth closing duration 0.59 0.459 4.80 ∼0.426 
Prey capture duration 4.79 0.051 5.20 <0.051 
Feeding duration 20.68 <0.001 4.80 <0.001 

Ftrad and Ptrad are the results of traditional ANOVAs on the original data. Fphyl is the F-value at α=0.05 from the empirical F-distribution obtained from the simulation analysis. Pphyl is calculated from the empirical F-distribution using the Ftrad-value. Variables are significantly different if the traditional ANOVA values (Ftrad) are higher than F-values from the simulation analysis (Fphyl).

*

Combined SO and FO durations.

Our results indicate that the feeding behavior of M. horridusdiffers strikingly from P. vitticeps, U. notata and P. platyrhinos feeding on ant prey. While the latter species exhibit the typical kinematic phases that are characteristic of iguanian lizards, M. horridus is capable of significant modulation, resulting in the absence of the SO phase in most cycles. By eliminating the SO phase during capture, M. horridus substantially shortens the overall time needed to capture ant prey. Although kinematically different in their capture profile, both ant-eating species exhibit similarities in prey processing behaviors. Neither species crushes the prey, but instead transports it to the back of the oral cavity in as few as two cycles.

Variation from the feeding model

Studies of lizard feeding behavior have documented similarities in jaw and tongue movements among groups, revealing a basic dichotomy in prey capture behavior that appears to have a phylogenetic basis(Schwenk and Throckmorton,1989; Schwenk,2000). Whereas all iguanian lizards use the tongue to capture prey, most scleroglossans do not (Schwenk,2000). In addition, all iguanians studied to date show gape profiles during capture that are composed of all the phases defined in the Bramble and Wake (1985) model(SO, FO, FC). Scleroglossans, on the other hand, appear to have lost the SO phases of the gape profile during capture(Bels and Goosse, 1990;Delheusy and Bels, 1992, 1999; Goosse and Bels, 1992; Delheusy et al., 1995; Urbani and Bels, 1995). Interestingly, while the presence of the phases is rather static, the duration of the SO phase appears to vary dramatically within and among species(Table 3).

Our results suggest that among the species studied most of the variation in the duration of prey capture is due to variation in the duration of the SO phase. Although the SO phases of P. vitticeps, U. notata and P. platyrhinos (Table 2) are roughly comparable to those of other iguanian species(Table 3), aphylogenetic ANOVA showed significant differences in the duration of the entire opening phase (SO and FO combined) when comparing ant-eaters with other iguanian lizards(Table 5). There appears to be significant modulation of the SO phase, however, with the generalist U. notata exhibiting a much faster SO phase than the specialized P. platyrhinos. When the SO phase is present in M. horridus, the duration of a single prey capture event is similar to the fastest events recorded for P. platyrhinos and U. notata. However, when M. horridus eliminates the SO phase, the prey capture event as a whole becomes more than 30% faster than that in P. platyrhinos and at least twice as fast as any other iguanian(Table 3). Because scleroglossans lack an SO phase, the duration of prey capture (using the jaws)tends to be faster than that of most iguanians and is generally comparable to that of M. horridus. However, when using tongue prehension, prey capture in scleroglossans is remarkably slower (75%) than when using jaw prehension alone (Urbani and Bels,1995).

The longer duration of tongue prehension cycles in scleroglossans makes the prey capture event in M. horridus all the more interesting. In the absence of a SO phase, the time in which to protract the tongue during mouth opening is significantly reduced. Indeed, the longer durations exhibited by scleroglossans attempting tongue prehension(Urbani and Bels, 1995)suggest that it may be difficult to coordinate fast tongue and jaw movements. It is possible that the rapid tongue protraction exhibited by M. horridus is necessary to accommodate rapid mouth opening(Table 1). However, tongue retraction and mouth closing velocities are both slower in M. horridus than in the other species examined here. The observed reduction in the speed of tongue retraction and mouth closing may be responses to the timing of prey contact. If tongue retraction is only initiated near the onset of mouth closing, as in M. horridus, then it may indeed be more advantageous to close the mouth more slowly to reduce the risk of dislodging the prey, especially considering the apparent limited adhesive capacity of the tongue of M. horridus.

In most studies of prey capture in iguanians, the fast mouth opening phase coincides with tongue retraction and transport of prey into the mouth(Herrel et al., 1995; Schwenk, 2000). The timing of FO and tongue retraction is important because rapid mouth opening will allow larger prey items to clear the jaw margins as the tongue is retracted,reducing the risk of dislodging the prey as it is transported into the mouth. Indeed, P. vitticeps always initiates the FO phase just after prey contact, but the timing varies considerably in P. platyrhinos and U. notata. In M. horridus, tongue retraction is initiated near the end of the FO phase. Based on our data, we suggest that the timing of prey contact and FO are likely to reflect prey type and more generally diet. The diet of the generalist P. vitticeps and U. notataincludes a diversity of prey (Greer,1989; Cogger, 2000; Turner, 1998), whereas the diet of M. horridus consists of only relatively small prey(Pianka and Pianka, 1970; Withers and Dickman, 1995),requiring a minimal gape to be safely transported past the mandible tips. While predominately an ant eater, P. platyrhinos also includes other prey its diet (Pianka and Parker,1975), and modulation of prey contact and the timing of jaw closure may be a strategy to accommodate larger prey (e.g. coleopterans,lepidopteran larvae).

Tongue function during capture

The tongue serves an important function in prey capture among iguanians,providing a protrusible, adhesive surface with which to apprehend prey. During typical iguanian tongue protrusion, the sticky dorsal surface of the tongue is rolled forward to contact the prey. Thus, the tongue is positioned so that the`adhesive' area of high papillary density is in contact with the prey item(Schwenk, 2000). Unlike in the other species, which exhibit the typical iguanian pattern, the tongue of M. horridus is extended straight with the anatomical tip, instead of the dorsal surface, contacting the prey. Not only does this tongue shape resemble a `tasting' tongue flick of iguanian lizards, but the kinematic profile also shares similarities. Kinematically, prey capture in M. horridus and the tongue flick in Uromastix acanthinurus and Plocederma stellio (Herrel et al., 1998) are similar in that both behaviors exhibit only two kinematic phases. However, it should be noted that while the duration of the gape cycle during tongue flicking is considerably longer, the duration of the actual tongue displacement is similar to that seen during prey capture. These similarities in tongue shape and kinematic profile are suggestive of the use of a different mechanism of tongue protrusion in M. horridus.

It is widely thought that tongue flicking is accomplished using predominately a muscular hydrostatic mechanism rather than being mainly the result of hyobranchial protraction (e.g. Herrel et al., 1998; Schwenk, 2000; De Groot et al., 2004). Because muscles maintain a constant volume as they change shape(Kier and Smith, 1985),shortening of intrinsic muscle fibers must be directed into either lateral expansion or forward elongation of the tongue, or both. The long, pointy shape and the apparent lack of anterior attachment of the tongue in M. horridus (Fig. 6A) are suggestive of a muscular hydrostatic mechanism.

Why use a muscular hydrostatic mechanism for tongue protrusion? One possibility is that this mechanism may require less total movement of the hyobranchial system. The SO phase of iguanian prey capture is typified by hyobranchial protraction associated with intrinsic movement of the tongue as it is shaped and positioned for lingual prehension. However, the lack of an SO phase in M. horridus precludes these preparatory movements, and its fast tongue movements may require rapid protrusion with limited hyobranchial movement. In addition, muscular hydrostats are defined by finely controlled movements that may facilitate more coordinated movements and overall accuracy(Ritter and Nishikawa, 1995; Nishikawa et al., 1999). While this protrusion mechanism may allow more rapid and accurate tongue protrusion,it may come as a tradeoff. Prey contact with the tongue tip instead of the tongue's dorsal surface significantly reduces the area contacting the prey,and hence tongue prehensibility. In fact, we observed that M. horridus is incapable of prehending ant prey held lightly with forceps. Thus, it appears that although a hydrostatic mechanism of protrusion may facilitate the rapid feeding behavior of M. horridus, it may also reduce the area of prey contact and in effect reduce dietary breadth to small,lightweight prey.

Rapid tongue protrusion may be facilitated by another unique characteristic of the feeding behavior of M. horridus. In general, lizards tend to lunge while capturing prey, rotating forward over the forelimbs to bring the tongue or jaws in contact with the prey item. However, in M. horridusthere is a distinct lack of the lunge behavior and the initial prey distance is accounted for as the animal positions itself near a column of ants(Withers and Dickman, 1995; Pianka et al., 1998). Once in close proximity to the ants the animal remains relatively stationary and,instead of lunging toward each ant, swivels its head within range using the long, mobile neck. The neck of M. horridus is 3-10 times longer than either P. vitticeps or P. platyrhinos, respectively (neck length is approximately 35% of body length in M. horridus, 3% in P. platyrhinos, 10% in U. notata 11% and P. vitticeps, compare Figs 3A, 4A, 5A, 6A). The loss of the forward lunge may beneficial for several reasons; the rapid tongue flick could be accommodated by the significantly more mobile head, which could track rapidly moving prey; it may decrease the amount of movement invested in each prey capture event; and it would allow the animal to remain relatively cryptic during a feeding bout.

Evolutionary considerations

Although more different than similar, the feeding behavior of the two ant-specialized lizards exhibits several convergent aspects. First, prey processing has diverged drastically from the primitive condition found in Sphenodon and most iguanian lizards, where prey are reduced extensively before swallowing(Throckmorton, 1976; Gorniak et al., 1982; Smith, 1984; Bels and Baltus, 1988, 1989; Schwenk and Throckmorton,1989; Kraklau,1991; Herrel et al.,1996; So et al.,1992). Whereas the generalist species studied here (P. vitticeps and U. notata) exhibit the typical feeding pattern by processing the prey before swallowing, neither M. horridus nor P. platyrhinos appear to perform any puncture crushing bites using the mandibles. Schwenk (2000)noted a lack of processing in another species of horned lizard (Phrynosoma cornutum) and suggested that this may be a general aspect of lizards feeding on small prey. It is possible that a behavioral repertoire including prey processing is absent in some dietary specialists (as appears to be the case for P. platyrhinos feeding on multiple prey types; J. J. Meyers,personal observation), especially if the predominant diet consists of prey that require minimal reduction.

While there is a distinct lack of puncture crushing bites in M. horridus, there appears to be a fast `chewing' movement that occurs after ingestion. McBrayer and Reilly(2002a) described a processing behavior they termed palatal crushing, during which the prey is thought to be crushed between the tongue and the palate. While it is possible that M. horridus may use a type of palatal crushing as a method of immobilizing ant prey, we cannot confirm this behavior. To adequately describe these intra-oral movements it will be necessary to record feeding behavior using high-speed cineradiography. It is unclear if and how P. platyrhinosimmobilizes prey upon ingestion, since the prey is transported directly into the posterior buccal cavity (Fig. 5A). Sherbrooke(2003) suggested that lizards of the genus Phrynosoma immobilize prey by coating them with a thick mucous secreted in the buccal cavity. While adequate for ants, mucosal secretions may not immobilize larger potentially dangerous prey(Sherbrooke, 2002) and the lack of processing may represent a costly tradeoff. Both palatal crushing and mucous secretions are possible mechanisms of disabling potentially dangerous prey items such as ants and both should be investigated more thoroughly.

As a result of a reduction in prey processing, ant specialists have achieved significantly shorter feeding events than generalists(Table 5). If we compare the duration of a feeding event in P. platyrhinos and M. horridus, we see that on average P. platyrhinos takes only 50 ms longer (Table 1). This result may seem unexpected given the more rapid capture phase of M. horridus. However, M. horridus and P. platyrhinosemploy different strategies to reduce the feeding duration. While M. horridus is routinely faster at the capture stage, it performs`processing'-like behaviors that increase the duration of the feeding event. In contrast, the slower capture phase of P. platyrhinos is overcome by the complete lack of prey processing and the immediate swallowing of prey upon ingestion.

The capture of a prey item is generally the fastest stage of a feeding event, and the majority of the feeding event in generalized lizards is composed of processing and transport of the prey. While modulation of the capture phase can decrease the duration of a feeding event, as it does in M. horridus and many scleroglossans, reducing the processing and transport phases appears to have a more significant effect on feeding duration. This is exemplified by a comparison of feeding durations in various lizards (Table 3), which revealed that the fastest feeding event (M. horridus) occurred 52 times faster than the slowest. That is, a feeding event in a specialist can occur in less than half a second, while it can take up to 18 s in generalist lizards (see Table 3). Prey size clearly influences the duration of a feeding event and we might expect lizards to feed more rapidly when feeding on small prey. Our observations support this assertion; P. vitticeps is nearly three times faster when feeding on ants versus crickets(Table 3). Yet, P. vitticeps is still 4-6 times slower than both P. platyrhinos and M. horridus feeding on ants, suggesting significant modification of the overall feeding behavior in these ant specialists.

Our data clearly show that ant specialists have undergone significant phenotypic evolution associated with novel feeding behaviors and diverging function of the hyobranchial system. While M. horridus is decidedly divergent both behaviorally and kinematically from the other species, the ant-specialized species exhibit convergent behaviors that minimize feeding movements and reduce overall feeding time. It appears that, like herbivorous lizards (Herrel et al., 1999), myrmecophagous lizards do not exhibit a universal solution to dietary specialization, but instead have evolved a variety of strategies. Since most myrmecophagous lizards do not show the extreme morphological modifications exhibited by Moloch and Phrynosoma, it will be interesting to document their behavioral strategies when feeding on ants. In particular, will scleroglossan lizards use tongue or jaw prehension when feeding on ants? Based on our observations, we would predict that like M. horridus and P. platyrhinos,other myrmecophagous lizards will exhibit behaviors that reduce the amount of time invested in each feeding event.

We wish to thank Rex Neindorf of Alice Springs Reptile Center for allowing us to film animals in his care. J. Glass and K. Lappin, L. McBrayer, K. Metzger, and K. C. Nishikawa provided comments on an earlier draft. J.M. was supported by NIH MSD grant GM 56931. A.H. would like to thank the Journal of Experimental Biology for a travel grant, which allowed him to attend the satellite meeting on specialized muscle function at the occasion of the 34th International Congress of Physiological Sciences, and subsequently to conduct field work. A.H. is a postdoctoral fellow of the fund for scientific research - Flanders (FWO-Vl).

Bels, V. L. (
2003
). Evaluating the complexity of the trophic system in reptilians. In
Vertebrate Biomechanics and Evolution
(ed. V. L. Bels, J.-P. Gasc and A. Casinos), pp.
185
-202. Oxford: Bios Scientific Publishers.
Bels, V. L. and Baltus, I. (
1988
). The influence of food items on the feeding cycle of Anolis equestris(Reptilia: Iguanidae).
Copeia
1988
,
479
-488.
Bels, V. L. and Baltus, I. (
1989
). First analysis of feeding in anolis lizards. In
Fortschritte der Zoologie
(ed. H. Splechtna and H. Hilgers), pp.
141
-145. New York: Gustav Fisher Verlag.
Bels, V. L. and Goosse, V. (
1990
). Comparative kinematic analysis of prey capture in Anolis carolinensis (Iguania)and Lacerta viridis (Scleroglossa).
J. Exp. Zool.
255
,
120
-124.
Bramble, D. M. and Wake, D. B. (
1985
). Feeding mechanisms in lower tetrapods. In
Functional Vertebrate Morphology
(ed. M. Hildebrand, D. M. Bramble, K. F. Liem, and D. B. Wake), pp.
230
-261. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.
Cogger, H. G. (
2000
).
Reptiles and Amphibians of Australia
, pp.
298
-356. Sydney, Australia: New Holland Publishers.
De Groot, J. H., van der Sluijs, I., Snelderwaard, P. Ch. and Van Leeuwen, J. L. (
2004
). A three-dimensional kinematic analysis of tongue flicking in Python molurus.
J. Exp. Biol.
207
,
827
-839.
Delheusy, V. and Bels, V. L. (
1992
). Kinematics of feeding behavior in Oplurus cuvieri (Reptilia: Iguanidae).
J. Exp. Biol.
170
,
155
-186.
Delheusy, V. and Bels, V. L. (
1999
). Feeding kinematics of Phelsuma madagascariensis (Reptilia: Gekkonidae):Testing differences between Iguania and Scleroglossa.
J. Exp. Biol.
202
,
3715
-3730.
Delheusy, V., Brillet, C. and Bels, V. L.(
1995
). Etude cinematique de la prise de nourriture chez Eublepharis macularius (Reptilia, Gekkonidae) et comparaison au sein des geckos.
Amphibia Reptilia
16
,
185
-201.
Diaz-Uriarte, R. and Garland, T., Jr (
1998
). Effects of the branch lengths errors on the performance of phyllogenetically independent contrasts.
Syst. Biol.
47
,
654
-672.
Garland, T., Jr, Harvey, P. H. and Ives, A. R.(
1992
). Procedures for the analysis of comparative data using phylogenetically independent contrasts.
Syst. Biol.
41
,
18
-32.
Garland, T., Jr, Dickerman, A. W., Janis, C. M. and Jones, J. A. (
1993
). Phylogenetic analysis of covariance by computer simulation.
Syst. Biol.
42
,
265
-292.
Goosse, V. and Bels, V. L. (
1992
). Kinematic analysis of tongue movements during chemosensory behavior in the European green lizard, Lacerta viridis (Reptilia, Lacertidae).
Can. J. Zool.
70
,
1886
-1896.
Gorniak, G. C., Rosenberg, H. I. and Gans, C.(
1982
). Mastication in the Tuatara, Sphenodon punctatus(Reptilia: Rhynchocephalis): structure and activity of the motor system.
J. Morph.
171
,
321
-353.
Grant, P. R. (
1999
).
Ecology and Evolution of Darwin's Finches
. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press.
Greene, H. W. (
1982
). Dietary and phenotypic diversity in lizards: why are some organisms specialized? In
Environmental Adaptation and Evolution
(ed. G. Mossakowski and G. Roth), pp.
107
-128. Stuttgart:Gustav Fisher.
Greer, A. E. (
1989
).
Biology and Evolution of Australian Lizards
. Sydney, Australia: Surrey Beatty and Sons. p.
264
.
Herrel, A. and De Vree, F. (
1999
). Kinematics of intraoral transport and swallowing in the herbivorous lizard Uromastix acanthinurus.
J. Exp. Biol.
202
,
1127
-1137.
Herrel, H., Cleuren, J. and De Vree, F. (
1995
). Prey capture in the lizard Agama stellio.
J. Morph.
224
,
313
-329.
Herrel, A., Cleuren, J. and De Vree, F. (
1996
). Kinematics of feeding in the lizard Agama stellio.
J. Exp. Biol.
199
,
1727
-1742.
Herrel, A., Timmermans, J. P. and De Vree, F.(
1998
). Tongue flicking in agamid lizards: morphology, kinematics and muscle activity patterns.
Anat. Rec.
252
,
102
-116.
Jackson, D. A. (
1993
). Stopping rules in principal component analysis: a comparison of heuristical and statistical approaches.
Ecology
74
,
2204
-2214.
Kier, W. M. and Smith, K. K. (
1985
). Tongues,tentacles and trunks: the biomechanics of movement in muscular hydrostats.
Zool. J. Linn. Soc. Lond.
83
,
307
-324.
Kraklau, D. M. (
1991
). Kinematics of prey capture and chewing in the lizard Agama agama (Squamata: Agamidae).
J. Morph.
210
,
195
-212.
Losos, J. B. (
1990
). Ecomorphology, performance capability, and scaling of west Indian anolis lizards: an evolutionary analysis.
Ecol. Monog.
60
,
369
-388.
Macey, J. R., Schulte, J. A., II, Larson, A., Ananjeva, N. B.,Wang, Y., Pethiyagoda, R., Rastegar-Pouyani, N. and Papenfuss, T. J. (
2000
). Evaluating trans-tethys migration: an example using acrodont lizard phylogenies.
Syst. Biol.
49
,
233
-256.
Martins, E. P. and Garland, T., Jr (
1991
). Phylogenetic analyses of the correlated evolution of continuous characters: A simulations study.
Evolution
45
,
534
-557.
McBrayer, L. D. and Reilly, S. M. (
2002a
). Prey processing in lizards: behavioral variation in sit-and-wait and widely foraging taxa.
Can. J. Zool.
80
,
882
-892.
McBrayer, L. D. and Reilly, S. M. (
2002b
). Testing the amniote models of prey transport kinematics: a quantitative analysis of mouth opening patterns in lizards.
Zoology
105
,
71
-81.
Meyers, J. J. and Nishikawa, K. C. (
2000
). Comparative study of tongue protrusion in three iguanian lizards: Sceloporus undulatus, Pseudotrapelus sinaitus and Chamaeleo jacksoni.
J. Exp. Biol.
203
,
2833
-2849.
Meyers, J. J., Herrel, A. and Birch, J. (
2002
). Scaling of morphology, bite force and feeding kinematics in an Iguanian and Scleroglossan lizard. In
Topics in Functional and Ecological Vertebrate Morphology
(ed. P. Aerts, K. D'Aout, A. Herrel and R. Van Damme), pp.
47
-62. Maastricht: Shaker Publishing.
Montanucci, R. R. (
1989
). The relationship of morphology to diet in the horned lizard genus Phrynosoma.
Herpetologica
45
,
208
-216.
Nishikawa, K. C., Kier, W. M. and Smith, K. K.(
1999
). Morphology and mechanics of tongue movement in the African pig-nosed frog Hemisus marmoratum: a muscular hydrostatic model.
J. Exp. Biol.
202
,
771
-780.
Pianka, E. R. (
1986
).
The ecology and natural history of desert lizards: analyses of the ecological niche and community structure
. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Pianka, E. R. and Parker, W. S. (
1975
). Ecology of horned lizards: a review with special reference to Phrynosoma platyrhinos.
Copeia
1975
,
141
-162.
Pianka, E. R. and Pianka, H. D. (
1970
). The ecology of M. horridus (Lacertilia: Agamidae) in Western Australia.
Copeia
1970
,
90
-103.
Pianka, G. A., Pianka, E. R. and Thompson, G. G.(
1998
). Natural history of thorny devils Moloch horridus(Lacertilia: Agamidae) in the Great Victoria Desert.
J. R. Soc. W. Aust.
181
,
183
-190.
Ritter, D. and Nishikawa, K. C. (
1995
). The kinematics and mechanism of prey capture in the African pig-nosed frog(Hemisus marmoratum): description of a radically divergent anuran tongue.
J. Exp. Biol.
198
,
2025
-2040.
Schulte, J. A., II, Vallardes, J. P. and Larson, A. L.(
2003
). Phylogenetic relationships within iguanidae inferred using molecular and morphological data and a phylogenetic taxonomy of iguanian lizards.
Herpetologica
59
,
399
-419.
Schwenk, K. (
2000
). Feeding in Lepidosaurs. In
Feeding
(ed. K. Schwenk), pp.
175
-291. New York: Academic Press.
Schwenk, K. and Sherbrooke, W. C. (
2003
). Mucus-binding of dangerous prey by horned lizards.
Soc. Int. Comp. Biol.
43a
,
MS-2.11
.
Schwenk, K. and Throckmorton, G. S. (
1989
). Functional and evolutionary morphology of lingual feeding in squamate reptiles: phylogenetics and kinematics.
J. Zool. Lond.
219
,
153
-175.
Sherbrooke, W. C. (
2002
). Phrynosoma modestum (Round-tailed horned lizard): Death due to beetle ingestion.
Herp. Rev.
33
,
312
.
Sherbrooke, W. C. (
2003
).
Introduction to Horned Lizards of North America
. Berkeley, Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press.
Smith, K. K. (
1984
). The use of the tongue and hyoid apparatus during feeding in lizards (Ctenosaura similes and Tupinambis nigropunctatus).
J. Zool.
202
,
115
-143.
Smith, T. L., Kardong, K. K. and Bels, V. L.(
1999
). Prey capture behavior in the blue-tongued skink, Tiliqua scincoides.
J. Herp.
33
,
362
-369.
So, K. K. J., Wainwright, P. C. and Bennet, A. F.(
1992
). Kinematics of prey processing in Chamaeleo jacksonii: conservation of function with morphological specialization.
J. Zool.
226
,
47
-64.
Throckmorton, G. S. (
1976
). Oral food processing in two herbivorous lizards, Iguana iguana (Iguanidae) and Uromastix aegypticus (Agamidae).
J. Morphol.
148
,
363
-390.
Turner, D. S. (
1998
). Ecology of the fringe-toed lizard, Uma notata, in Arizona's Mohawk Dunes.
Ms thesis
, University of Arizona, Tucson, USA.
Urbani, J. M. and Bels, V. L. (
1995
). Feeding behavior in two scleroglossan lizards: Lacerta viridis (Lacertidae)and Zonosaurus laticaudatus (Cordylidae).
J. Zool. Lond.
236
,
265
-290.
Vitt, L. J., Pianka, E. R., Coopers, W. E. and Schwenk, K.(
2003
). History and the global ecology of squamate reptiles.
Am. Nat.
162
,
44
-60.
Winter, D. A. (
1990
).
Biomechanics and Motor Control of Human Movement
. 2nd edition. New York:Wiley.
Withers, P. C. and Dickman, C. R. (
1995
). The role of diet in determining water, energy and salt intake in the thorny devil Moloch horridus (Lacertilia: Agamidae).
J. R. Soc. W. Aust.
78
,
3
-11.

Supplementary information