The effect of temperature variability on laying birds was studied experimentally, using Japanese quail. Two aspects of temperature variability were investigated: the effects of regular daily variation in temperature, and of a sudden change in temperature. Both of these may become more common as a consequence of climate change. These manipulations were carried out at two levels of food supply. Energy expenditure increased with higher daily temperature variation, and also after a sudden change in temperature, taking several days to settle to a constant level. Manipulating daily temperature variation also resulted in smaller eggs being laid under more variable temperatures, when food quality was also low. The results demonstrate that day-to-day variation in temperature, as well as mean temperature, affects energy expenditure, which can have consequences for egg production.

There has been much interest in the impact on ecosystems of recent climate changes (McCarty, 2001; Parmesan and Yohe, 2003; Penuelas and Filella, 2001; Walther et al., 2002). Rising temperatures, driven by this climate change, have been fundamental in causing these ecological impacts (Penuelas and Filella, 2001). Correlations with increased temperatures, and changes in range, migratory pattern, reproductive success and over-winter survival, have been demonstrated for a range of animals (reviewed in McCarty, 2001). These temperature changes have included increases of annual mean temperatures by up to 3°C in northern latitudes over the last 30 years(IPCC, 2001). The range of daily temperatures has also been altered as a result of climate changes, with increases in some areas and decreases in others(Easterling et al., 1997; IPCC, 2001). However, although the effects of mean temperature on energy expenditure have been studied previously, the effects of temperature variability have received very little attention.

This paper investigates the impacts of temperature variability on energy expenditure, and the consequences for reproduction, using Japanese quail Coturnix japonica as the model species. Costs of living, which are clearly important for breeding due to their impact on reproductive decisions,can be affected by changes in thermoregulation and foraging costs(Bryant, 1997; Feist and White, 1989; Kendeigh et al., 1977). Temperature can be important in determining the amount of time and energy that can be allocated to the different stages of reproduction. For birds, this has been demonstrated for egg production(Stevenson and Bryant, 2000; Ward, 1996), incubation(Bryan and Bryant, 1999) and brood-rearing (Spencer and Bryant,2002). Food supply is also important for breeding birds, as the levels of available nutrients are important in determining resource allocation to egg production (Houston,1997).

Manipulation of roosting temperatures of breeding wild birds has demonstrated the effects of mean temperature on egg quality(Nager and van Noordwijk,1992), timing of laying(Meijer et al., 1999), the ability to maintain a daily laying schedule(Yom-Tov and Wright, 1993),and incubation behaviour (Bryan and Bryant,1999). This implies that temperature can alter thermoregulatory costs, leading to a reallocation of energy resources available for reproduction. In this way, mean temperature could directly influence fitness via the quality of eggs (Both et al., 1999; Christians,2002; Perrins,1996; Williams,1994) and the pattern of laying(Nilsson and Svensson,1993).

Temperature variability is less commonly investigated than the effects of mean temperature. However, an experiment involving captive Japanese quail found that rapid sinusoidal temperature fluctuations resulted in an increase in metabolism (Prinzinger,1982). Another experiment involving captive turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo) showed that, when they were transferred from a low to a high temperature, metabolic rate decreased slowly during an acclimatisation period of several days (MacLeod et al.,1980b).

These experiments suggest that temperature variability, as well as mean temperature, may be important in determining daily living costs. This may then have consequent effects on egg-laying ability(Perrins, 1970; Stevenson and Bryant, 2000)and fitness more generally. This paper uses two experiments to investigate independently the effects on egg production and energy expenditure of: (i)daily variation in temperature and (ii) a sudden change in temperature.

Study population

The study took place at the Roslin Institute, Scotland, using Japanese quail Coturnix japonica Temminck and Schlegel reared from stock. These quail are from a population that have not been subject to any intentional selection pressures, allowing comparisons to be made with wild birds. The first experiment was carried out from February to April 2002, and the second from September to December 2002. Two different sets of birds were used: 20 females for the first experiment, and 16 for the second. The birds were aged 14 weeks at the start of each experiment.

Four weeks prior to the experiments, each bird was randomly allocated to one of four large chambers (1 m×1 m×1.2 m high). During this time,the chambers were kept at a constant temperature of approx. 20°C, with a lighting cycle of 14 h:10 h light:dark. The birds were provided with standard quail layer feed (Savory and Gentle,1976) and water ad libitum.

Experiment 1 – Daily variation in temperature

A 2×3 factorial design was used for the experimental treatments. This involved two levels of food quality and three levels of temperature variation. The two levels of food quality were `high' (`HQ'; 100% standard food) and`low' (`LQ'; 50% standard food, 50% cellulose). These were alternated each week, with the aim of minimising changes in intestinal architecture(Starck, 1999). The three levels of temperature variation were `constant' (`C'; set at 18.3°C),`low' (`L'; set at 25°C during the day and 15°C at night) and `high'(`H'; set at 31.7°C during the day and 11.7°C at night). These treatments aimed to provide the same mean daily temperature. The temperature changes took place at 09:00 h and 17:00 h each day. The shorter warm-cycle than light-cycle gave conditions with cooler dawns and dusks, compared to the rest of the day.

Each treatment, run for a 7 day period, was randomly allocated to each chamber. This was then repeated. The experiment therefore lasted for 12 weeks. The 7 day period included an acclimatisation period of 3 days prior to measuring egg mass or gas exchange for that treatment. At the end of each 7 day period, all the birds were weighed to 0.1 g using an electronic balance(Sartorius UK, Epsom, Surrey, UK). Eggs were weighed on the day of laying to 0.01 g using an electronic balance. In each of the eight chambers, temperature was measured every 10 min using data loggers (Gemini Data Loggers Ltd., W. Sussex, England).

Each week, two pairs of birds (at different times; the same birds were used each week) from each of the four groups, were moved into smaller calorimeter chambers (600 mm×600 mm×450 mm high) for 2 days at a time. Here they were provided with the same food, temperature and lighting conditions as found in the larger chamber from which they came. Gas exchange was measured on the second day, using the system described by Lundy et al.(1978) and MacLeod et al.(1985).

Experiment 2 – Sudden temperature change

A 2×4 factorial design was used for the experimental treatments. This involved two levels of food quality and four temperature-change treatments. The same two levels of food quality were used: `high' (100% standard food) and`low' (50% standard food, 50% cellulose). The four temperature-change treatments were changes from `high to medium' (`HM'), `medium to high' (`MH'),`medium to low' (`ML') and `low to medium' (`LM') temperatures, where `high'was set at 28°C, `medium' at 20°C, and `low' at 12°C.

Two females were kept in each of the smaller calorimeter chambers. The birds were subjected to a temperature change every 7 days, and the effects on gas exchange were measured over the subsequent days. At the end of each week,all the birds were weighed to 0.1 g using an electronic balance. In each of the chambers, temperature was measured every 10 min using data loggers (Gemini Data Loggers Ltd., W. Sussex, England). After 8 weeks, each bird had been subjected to each of the treatments. The experiment was then repeated with another eight birds.

Indirect calorimetry

Indirect calorimetry was used to measure energy expenditure for both experiments. Metabolic rate was calculated from the rates of oxygen consumption and carbon dioxide production. This was done by comparing the air from the calorimeter chamber with ambient air. Energy expenditure (kJ h–1) was calculated using the equation of Romijn and Lokhorst(1961):
\[\mathrm{Energy\ expenditure}=16.18V_{\mathrm{O}_{2}}+5.02V_{\mathrm{CO}_{2}}\]
where VO2 and VCO2 are the volumes of oxygen consumed and carbon dioxide produced (l h–1). Mean energy expenditures were calculated for both day (10:00 h–16:00 h) and night (23:00 h–05:00 h).

Statistical analysis

All data were analysed using Genstat for Windows, 5th edition. The effects on egg mass and energy expenditure were analysed using a linear mixed model using restricted maximum likelihood (REML) to control for the use of different chambers for each group, and the repeated measurements on individual birds(Patterson and Thompson,1971). Values quoted in the text and tables are means ± s.e.m. Vertical bars in graphs represent s.e.m. The effect sizes for factors,presented in tables, are relative to the reference group.

Effects of daily variation in temperature

Temperature treatments

There was a significant difference in the range of temperatures for the three temperature treatment groups (C, 1.9±0.7°C; L,11.9±0.6°C; H, 19.8±0.6°C; χ2=846.46;d.f.=2; P<0.001). There was also an unplanned but significant difference in mean temperature for these three groups (C,19.7±0.3°C; L, 18.5±0.2°C; H, 19.6±0.3°C;χ 2=14.57; d.f.=2; P<0.001), but this difference was small (<1.3°C). To control for any effects of mean temperature on energy expenditure (Godfrey and Bryant,2000; Spencer and Bryant,2002) or egg mass (Nager and van Noordwijk, 1992; Nager and Zandt, 1994), mean temperature was included in the analysis as a covariate.

Energy expenditure

Energy expenditure is shown for the six treatment groups in Fig. 1A. Energy expenditure was significantly affected by the food quality treatment, temperature variation,mean temperature and time of day (Table 1). Energy expenditure was higher for the low-quality, compared to the high-quality food treatment, and increased with higher temperature variability; and increased with a decrease in mean temperature.

Fig. 1.

The effects of the temperature variation and food quality treatment groups(Experiment 1) on (A) daily energy expenditure (DEE) and (B) mean egg mass. Values are means ± s.e.m.(N=20). Hatched columns, high-quality food; open columns, low-quality food.

Fig. 1.

The effects of the temperature variation and food quality treatment groups(Experiment 1) on (A) daily energy expenditure (DEE) and (B) mean egg mass. Values are means ± s.e.m.(N=20). Hatched columns, high-quality food; open columns, low-quality food.

Table 1.

Linear mixed model (REML) of energy expenditure (from Experiment 1) in terms of food quality, temperature variation, mean temperature and time of day

Energy expenditure
Termd.f.EffectWald statistic (χ2)P value
Minimal model     
    Food quality     
        HQ 52.48 <0.001 
        LQ  89.0±12.3   
    Temperature treatment     
        C    
        L 29.7±15.0 14.83 <0.001 
        H  58.2±15.2   
    Mean temperature -3.98±1.38 8.29 0.004 
    Time of day     
        Day 75.28 <0.001 
        Night  -172.2±19.8   
Excluded terms     
    Egg mass  2.82 0.093 
    Week  0.78 0.376 
Energy expenditure
Termd.f.EffectWald statistic (χ2)P value
Minimal model     
    Food quality     
        HQ 52.48 <0.001 
        LQ  89.0±12.3   
    Temperature treatment     
        C    
        L 29.7±15.0 14.83 <0.001 
        H  58.2±15.2   
    Mean temperature -3.98±1.38 8.29 0.004 
    Time of day     
        Day 75.28 <0.001 
        Night  -172.2±19.8   
Excluded terms     
    Egg mass  2.82 0.093 
    Week  0.78 0.376 

A random term of `bird identities and calorimeter chamber' was used(χ2=37.24; d.f.=1; P<0.001) to account for the non-independence of conditions from different chambers, and repeated measures of the same birds.

For definitions of HQ, LQ, C, L and H, see text.

Egg production

Birds fed on the high-quality food laid significantly larger eggs than birds fed the low-quality food (Table 2). There was also a significant interaction between food treatment and temperature treatment (Table 2). Under the low-quality food treatment, significantly larger eggs were laid under the constant-temperature treatment compared to both the low and high temperature-variation treatments(Fig. 1B). Under the high-quality food treatment there was no such difference in egg mass between the treatments. Also, under the low-quality food treatment only, egg mass increased with maximum temperature by 0.23±0.10 g °C –1.

Table 2.

Analysis of egg mass (from Experiment 1), in terms of food quality,temperature treatment, maximum temperature, and interactions between food quality and temperature treatment, and food quality and maximum temperature

Egg mass
Termd.f.Wald statistic (χ2)P value
Minimal model    
    Food quality 41.05 <0.001 
    Temperature treatment 1.65 0.439 
    Maximum temperature 2.17 0.140 
    Food quality×Temperature treatment 9.24 0.010 
    Food quality×Maximum temperature 5.65 0.017 
Excluded terms    
    Mean temperature 2.03 0.154 
    Week 0.93 0.336 
    Minimum temperature 0.63 0.427 
    Female mass 0.03 0.872 
Egg mass
Termd.f.Wald statistic (χ2)P value
Minimal model    
    Food quality 41.05 <0.001 
    Temperature treatment 1.65 0.439 
    Maximum temperature 2.17 0.140 
    Food quality×Temperature treatment 9.24 0.010 
    Food quality×Maximum temperature 5.65 0.017 
Excluded terms    
    Mean temperature 2.03 0.154 
    Week 0.93 0.336 
    Minimum temperature 0.63 0.427 
    Female mass 0.03 0.872 

A random term of `bird identities and calorimeter chamber' was used(χ2=14.45; d.f.=1; P<0.001) to account for the non-independence of conditions from different chambers, and repeated measures of the same birds.

Effects of sudden temperature change

Temperature

There was a significant difference in mean temperature between the three temperature levels (H, 29.4±0.1°C; M, 19.8±0.1°C; L,12.5±0.1°C; χ2=10399.76; d.f.=2; P<0.001). This gave a difference of mean temperature between`medium' and `low' levels of 7.4±0.02°C, and 9.6±0.02°C between `medium' and `high' levels.

Energy expenditure

Energy expenditure was highest on the day after the change in temperature and decreased over the next 6 days (Fig. 2A), when controlling for temperature treatment, time of day, and week (Table 3). Highest energy expenditures occurred when temperature was changed from `medium' to `high' or`low', compared to `high' or `low' to `medium'(Fig. 2B).

Fig. 2.

Daily energy expenditure (DEE) (Experiment 2) (A) following a sudden change in temperature; and (B) for the sudden temperature-change treatment groups(LM, low to medium; ML, medium to low; MH, medium to high; HM, high to medium). Values are means ± s.e.m.(N=16).

Fig. 2.

Daily energy expenditure (DEE) (Experiment 2) (A) following a sudden change in temperature; and (B) for the sudden temperature-change treatment groups(LM, low to medium; ML, medium to low; MH, medium to high; HM, high to medium). Values are means ± s.e.m.(N=16).

Table 3.

Linear mixed model (REML) of energy expenditure (from Experiment 2), in relation to temperature treatment, day from temperature change, time of day and week

Energy expenditure
Termd.f.EffectWald statistic (χ2)P value
Minimal model     
    Day from change -19.81±3.10 45.85 <0.001 
    Temperature treatment     
        HM    
        MH  31.09±17.64 15.11 0.002 
        ML 58.91±17.88   
        LM  2.40±18.93   
    Time of day     
        Day 120.27 <0.001 
        Night  -135.32±12.34   
    Week -31.01±3.10 100.21 <0.001 
Excluded terms     
    Food quality  1.02 0.313 
    Mean temperature  4.25 0.120 
    Female mass  3.29 0.070 
Energy expenditure
Termd.f.EffectWald statistic (χ2)P value
Minimal model     
    Day from change -19.81±3.10 45.85 <0.001 
    Temperature treatment     
        HM    
        MH  31.09±17.64 15.11 0.002 
        ML 58.91±17.88   
        LM  2.40±18.93   
    Time of day     
        Day 120.27 <0.001 
        Night  -135.32±12.34   
    Week -31.01±3.10 100.21 <0.001 
Excluded terms     
    Food quality  1.02 0.313 
    Mean temperature  4.25 0.120 
    Female mass  3.29 0.070 

A non-significant random term of `bird identities and calorimeter chamber'was used (χ2=3.51; d.f.=1; P>0.085) to be consistent. The analysis was similar without this random term, and also if the close-to-significant term of `female mass' was controlled for.

For definitions of HM, MH, ML, LM, see text.

Temperature variability and energy expenditure

Both high daily temperature variation, and a sudden change in temperature,resulted in an increase in energy expenditure. A previous experiment has shown that rapid temperature fluctuations also increase energy expenditure(Prinzinger, 1982). In this latter experiment, temperature cycled between 7°C and 13°C, and metabolic rate increased between treatments as the cycle-time decreased from 4 h to 1 h (Prinzinger, 1982). These results suggest that temperature variability can be important in determining daily living costs. Day-to-day constancy, as well as the mean value, should therefore be studied when investigating the effects of temperature. Since the daily range of temperatures may be changing as a result of climate change (Easterling et al.,1997; IPCC, 2001),the effects of temperature variability should be investigated when studying the impacts of these changes.

The increase in energy expenditure during more variable temperatures, or after a sudden change in temperature, could be caused by the feedback system controlling metabolism `overshooting' while adjusting to new temperatures(Prinzinger, 1982). This is thought to be more important than the energy expenditure required by the increase in activity in the neurological centres of the measuring and feedback control systems, which is believed to be negligible(Prinzinger, 1982). Other possible reasons include the inefficiency of changing from fat synthesis/deposition during cold temperatures, and fat-catabolism at higher temperatures, and more variable conditions resulting in increased physical activity or feeding during warmer temperatures to compensate for increased energy expenditure during cold temperatures.

The increase in energy expenditure following a sudden change of approx. 8°C suggests that acclimatisation to such a change can take several days. This was also found for growing turkeys when temperature was changed from low to high, but not vice versa(MacLeod et al., 1980b). Nevertheless, the period of acclimatisation may be shorter for wild animals that are more familiar with variable temperatures, compared to the quail and turkeys used in these experiments. Energy expenditure also tended to be higher when the temperature was changed from `medium' to either `high' or `low',compared to a change from `high' or `low' to `medium'. This suggests that it is easier to adjust to average rather than more extreme temperatures.

Energy expenditure also showed circadian rhythmicity, being higher during the day than at night (Lundy et al.,1978; MacLeod et al.,1980a). This has been shown to be due mainly to increased activity(MacLeod et al., 1982),controlled by the lighting cycle (MacLeod et al., 1980a). Basal metabolic rates are also higher during the day than the night (Aschoff and Pohl,1970).

Food supply

Energy expenditure was also affected by food quality, with birds fed low-quality food having higher energy expenditures than birds on high-quality food. This is likely to be due to the greater time required for feeding under the low quality food treatment, and the increased processing in the gut required for a high-cellulose diet.

Food quality also had a major influence on egg mass. A positive effect of food on egg mass has previously been found from: experiments with captive birds (Yamane et al., 1979);observations of wild birds over years of differing food availability(Bryant, 1978; Hiom et al., 1991; Järvinen and Vaisanen,1984); and supplementary feeding experiments of wild birds(Hill, 1988; Hiom et al., 1991; Högstedt, 1981; Källander and Karlsson,1993; Ramsay and Houston,1997). Food is therefore likely to directly affect the amount of resources available for egg production.

Temperature variability and egg mass

Under the low-quality food treatment, significantly larger eggs were laid when temperature was constant compared to when it was varied during the day. The increased thermoregulatory costs imposed by higher temperature variability may have therefore reduced resources available for egg production. Since an increase in energy expenditure is required for egg production(Stevenson and Bryant, 2000; Ward and MacLeod, 1992),smaller eggs may therefore be produced under more variable conditions. Under the high-quality food treatment, egg mass was not influenced by temperature variability, suggesting that under good food conditions any negative effects of temperature variation can be fully compensated. Since food conditions can vary between years and areas (Bolton et al., 1992; Dijkstra et al.,1982), and within a breeding season(Birkhead and Nettleship, 1982; Gibb, 1950), our results suggest that temperature variation is likely to be more important in some situations compared to others. Alternatively, since foraging costs are higher for wild birds than captive birds provided with food ad libitum,birds in the wild may invariably experience relatively `poor' conditions, so temperature variation may be more likely to affect energy expenditure. This effect of temperature variability on reproduction is unlikely to be limited to just birds. Similar effects could also be expected for other animals.

Conclusions

We have demonstrated an effect on energy expenditure of temperature variability, in terms of daily variation and the effect of a sudden temperature change. This had subsequent effects on the resources allocated to egg production. Since daily temperature ranges have increased in some areas due to climate change (Easterling et al.,1997; IPCC, 2001),temperature variation is likely to become more important in determining daily living costs in these areas. Where day-to-day variation in temperature is decreasing, by contrast, daily living costs may also be reduced. Although recent increases in mean temperatures(IPCC, 2001), and a reduction in energy expenditure with temperature would be expected to reduce daily living costs (Kendeigh et al.,1977; Walsberg,1983), our results suggest that increasing temperature variability might counteract this affect. The effect of the exact pattern of temperature change is thus likely to be important in predicting biotic responses to future climate change.

Ji-Hyuk Kim, Kim Bernard and Lindsey McNeil are thanked for their help with the experiments. We also thank Ian Stevenson and three anonymous referees for their comments on previous versions of this manuscript. This study was funded as part of C.J.P.'s NERC research studentship (NER/S/C/2000/03642) with additional CASE support from CJ Wildbird Foods.

Aschoff, J. and Pohl, M. (
1970
). Der ruheumsatz von vögeln als funktion der tageszeit und der korpergrösse.
J. Ornithol.
111
,
38
-47.
Birkhead, T. R. and Nettleship, D. N. (
1982
). The adaptive significance of egg size and laying date in thick-billed murres Uris lomvia.
Ecology
63
,
300
-306.
Bolton, M., Houston, D. and Monaghan, P.(
1992
). Nutritional constraints on egg formation in the lesser black-backed gull: an experimental study.
J. Anim. Ecol.
61
,
521
-532.
Both, C., Visser, M. E. and Verboven, N.(
1999
). Density-dependent recruitment rates in great tits: the importance of being heavier.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
266
,
465
-469.
Bryan, S. M. and Bryant, D. M. (
1999
). Heating nest-boxes reveals an energetic constraint on incubation behaviour in great tits, Parus major.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
266
,
157
-162.
Bryant, D. M. (
1997
). Energy expenditure in wild birds.
Proc. Nutr. Soc.
56
,
1025
-1039.
Bryant, D. M. (
1978
). Establishment of weight hierarchies in the broods of House Martins Delichon urbica.
Ibis
120
,
16
-26.
Christians, J. K. (
2002
). Avian egg size:variation within species and inflexibility within individuals.
Biol. Rev.
77
,
1
-26.
Dijkstra, C., Vuursteen, L., Daan, S. and Masman, D.(
1982
). Clutch size and laying date in the kestrel Falco tinnunculus–effect of supplementary food.
Ibis
124
,
210
-213.
Easterling, D. R., Horton, B., Jones, P. D., Peterson, T. C.,Karl, T. R., Parker, D. E., Salinger, M. J., Razuvayev, V., Plummer,N., Jamason, P. et al. (
1997
). Maximum and minimum temperature trends for the globe.
Science
277
,
364
-367.
Feist, D. D. and White, R. G. (
1989
). Terrestrial mammals in cold. In
Advances in Comparative and Environmental Physiology
, vol.
4
(ed. L. C. H. Wang), pp.
327
-360. Berlin:Springer-Verlag.
Gibb, J. (
1950
). The breeding biology of the great and blue titmice.
Ibis
92
,
507
-539.
Godfrey, J. D. and Bryant, D. M. (
2000
). State-dependent behaviour and energy expenditure: an experimental study of European robins on winter territories.
J. Anim. Ecol.
69
,
301
-313.
Hill, W. L. (
1988
). The effect of food abundance on the reproductive patterns of coots.
Condor
90
,
324
-331.
Hiom, L., Bolton, M., Monaghan, P. and Worrall, D.(
1991
). Experimental-evidence for food limitation of egg-production in gulls.
Ornis Scand.
22
,
94
-97.
Högstedt, G. (
1981
). Effect of additional food on reproductive success in the magpie (Pica pica).
J. Anim. Ecol.
50
,
219
-229.
Houston, D. C. (
1997
). Nutritional constraints on egg production in birds.
Proc. Nutr. Soc.
56
,
1057
-1065.
IPCC. (
2001
). Climate change 2001: the scientific basis.
Contribution of working group I to the third assessment report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Järvinen, A. and Vaisanen, R. A. (
1984
). Reproduction of pied flycatchers (Ficedula hypoleuca) in good and bad breeding seasons in a northern marginal area.
Auk
101
,
429
-450.
Källander, H. and Karlsson, J. (
1993
). Supplemental food and laying date in the European starling.
Condor
95
,
1031
-1034.
Kendeigh, S. C., Dol'nik, V. R. and Gavrilov, V. M.(
1977
). Avian Energetics. In
Granivorous Birds in Ecosystems
, vol.
12
(ed. J. Pinowski and S. C. Kendeigh), pp.
129
-204. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lundy, H., MacLeod, M. G. and Jewitt, T. R.(
1978
). An automated multi-calorimeter system: preliminary experiments on laying hens.
Br. Poult. Sci.
19
,
173
-186.
MacLeod, M. G., Jewitt, T. R., White, J., Verbrugge, M. and Mitchell, M. A. (
1982
). The contribution of locomotor activity to energy expenditure in the domestic fowl. In
Proc. 9th Symp. Energy Metabolism
(ed. A. Ekern and F. Sundstøl),pp.
297
-300. Lillehammer, Norway: Agricultural University of Norway.
MacLeod, M. G., Lundy, H. and Jewitt, T. R.(
1985
). Heat production by the mature male turkey (Meleagris gallopavo): preliminary measurements in an automated, indirect,open-circuit multi-calorimeter system.
Br. Poult. Sci.
26
,
325
-333.
MacLeod, M. G., Tullett, S. G. and Jewitt, T. R.(
1980a
). Circadian variation in the metabolic rate of growing chickens and laying hens of a broiler strain.
Br. Poult. Sci.
21
,
155
-159.
MacLeod, M. G., Tullett, S. G. and Jewitt, T. R.(
1980b
). Effects of ambient temperature on the heat production of growing turkeys. In
Energy Metabolism of Farm Animals. European Association of Animal Production
(ed. L. E. Mount), pp.
257
-261. London: Butterworths.
McCarty, J. P. (
2001
). Ecological consequences of recent climate change.
Cons. Biol.
15
,
320
-331.
Meijer, T., Nienaber, U., Langer, U. and Trillmich, F.(
1999
). Temperature and timing of egg-laying of European starlings.
Condor
101
,
124
-132.
Nager, R. G. and van Noordwijk, A. J. (
1992
). Energetic limitation in the egg-laying period of great tits.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
249
,
259
-263.
Nager, R. G. and Zandt, H. S. (
1994
). Variation in egg size in great tits.
Ardea
82
,
315
-328.
Nilsson, J. Å. and Svensson, E. (
1993
). The frequency and timing of laying gaps.
Ornis Scand.
24
,
122
-126.
Parmesan, C. and Yohe, G. (
2003
). A globally coherent fingerprint of climate change impacts across natural systems.
Nature
421
,
37
-42.
Patterson, H. D. and Thompson, R. (
1971
). Recovery of inter-block information when block sizes are unequal.
Biometrika
58
,
545
-554.
Penuelas, J. and Filella, I. (
2001
). Responses to a warming world.
Science
294
,
793
-795.
Perrins, C. M. (
1970
). The timing of birds'breeding seasons.
Ibis
112
,
242
-255.
Perrins, C. M. (
1996
). Eggs, egg formation and the timing of breeding.
Ibis
138
,
2
-15.
Prinzinger, R. (
1982
). The energy costs of temperature regulation in birds: the influence of quick sinusoidal temperature fluctuations on the gaseous metabolism of the Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica).
Comp. Biochem. Physiol.
71A
,
469
-472.
Ramsay, S. L. and Houston, D. C. (
1997
). Nutritional constraints on egg production in the blue tit: a supplementary feeding study.
J. Anim. Ecol.
66
,
649
-657.
Romijn, C. and Lokhorst, W. (
1961
). Some aspects of energy metabolism in birds. In
2nd Sypmposium on Energy Metabolism
(ed. E. Brouwer and A. van Es), pp.
49
-58. Wageningen: E.A.A.P. Publication No. 10.
Savory, C. J. and Gentle, M. J. (
1976
). Effects of dietary dilution with fibre on the food intake and gut dimensions of Japanese quail.
Br. Poult. Sci.
17
,
561
-570.
Spencer, K. A. and Bryant, D. M. (
2002
). State-dependent behaviour in breeding barn swallows (Hirundo rustica): consequences for reproductive effort.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
269
,
403
-410.
Starck, J. M. (
1999
). Phenotypic flexibility of the avian gizzard: rapid, reversible and repeated changes of organ size in response to changes in dietary fibre content.
J. Exp. Biol.
202
,
3171
-3179.
Stevenson, I. R. and Bryant, D. M. (
2000
). Avian phenology–Climate change and constraints on breeding.
Nature
406
,
366
-367.
Walsberg, G. E. (
1983
). Avian Ecological Energetics. In
Avian Biology
, vol.
VII
(ed. D. S. Farner, J. R. King and K. C. Parkes),pp.
161
-220. New York: Academic Press.
Walther, G. R., Post, E., Convey, P., Menzel, A., Parmesan, C.,Beebee, T. J. C., Fromentin, J. M., Hoegh-Guldberg, O. and Bairlein,F. (
2002
). Ecological responses to recent climate change.
Nature
416
,
389
-395.
Ward, S. (
1996
). Energy expenditure of female barn swallows Hirundo rustica during egg formation.
Physiol. Zool.
69
,
930
-951.
Ward, S. and MacLeod, M. G. (
1992
). Energy cost of egg formation in quail.
Proc. Nutr. Soc.
51
,
41
.
Williams, T. D. (
1994
). Intraspecific variation in egg size and egg composition in birds: effects on offspring fitness.
Biol. Rev.
69
,
35
-60.
Yamane, T., Ono, K. and Tanaka, T. (
1979
). Protein requirement of laying Japanese quail.
Br. Poult. Sci.
20
,
279
-383.
Yom-Tov, Y. and Wright, J. (
1993
). Effect of heating nest boxes on egg laying in the blue tit (Parus caeruleus).
Auk
110
,
95
-99.