The evolution of trichromatic colour vision by the majority of anthropoid primates has been linked to the efficient detection and selection of food,particularly ripe fruits among leaves in dappled light. Modelling of visual signals has shown that trichromats should be more efficient than dichromats at distinguishing both fruits from leaves and ripe from unripe fruits. This prediction is tested in a controlled captive setting using stimuli recreated from those actually encountered by wild tamarins (Saguinus spp.). Dietary data and reflectance spectra of Abuta fluminum fruits eaten by wild saddleback (Saguinus fuscicollis) and moustached(Saguinus mystax) tamarins and their associated leaves were collected in Peru. A. fluminum leaves, and fruits in three stages of ripeness,were reproduced and presented to captive saddleback and red-bellied tamarins(Saguinus labiatus). Trichromats were quicker to learn the task and were more efficient at selecting ripe fruits than were dichromats. This is the first time that a trichromatic foraging advantage has been demonstrated for monkeys using naturalistic stimuli with the same chromatic properties as those encountered by wild animals.

As an order, primates are among the most frugivorous of mammals. Indeed,with the exception of tarsiers (Tarsius spp.), all primate species have been recorded to eat fruit, and many eat it in large quantities(Richard, 1985); it even accounts for 25–50% of the diet of `folivorous' species such as howler monkeys (Alouatta seniculus;Guillotin et al., 1994;Julliot, 1994). Whilst some species are specialized seed predators, the majority of primates act as dispersers for the species that they consume. Indeed, primate-mediated endozoochory may be the primary method of dispersal for many tropical plant species (Julliot, 1994). Given the importance of fruit to primates, and of primates to plant species in their dispersal, co-evolution has produced a suite of associated characteristics on both sides of this relationship. Trichromatic colour vision and the colour changes shown by fruits during maturation may be examples of such co-evolved characters.

Within placental mammals, trichromacy is unique amongst primates: all other species so far examined are either dichromats or monochromats(Jacobs, 1993;Ahnelt and Kolb, 2000;Arrese et al., 2002). It has been hypothesized that the evolution of trichromatic colour vision by the majority of primate species is a direct result of the chromatic signals produced by fruits (Regan et al.,2001) or leaves (Dominy and Lucas, 2001). For an animal to feed on fruits it has first to detect them against a background of leaves. Vision and olfaction are probably the principal senses employed. Theoretically, trichromacy has been predicted to be more efficient than dichromacy when detecting and identifying fruits against a leaf background (Osorio and Vorobyev, 1996; Sumner and Mollon, 2000a; Regan et al.,2001). In addition to detecting fruiting trees, an animal has to select ripe from unripe fruits. Physical and chemical defences may protect fruits until their seeds are ready to be dispersed. The ripening process is often characterized by a colour change that can give a clear visual signal to potential dispersers of the increased palatability of the ripe fruits(Regan et al., 2001). Theoretically, trichromats have also been predicted to be capable of distinguishing a greater number of ripe from unripe fruit species(Sumner and Mollon, 2000b;Regan et al., 2001).

Despite its theoretical advantages, trichromacy is not uniform within the primates. Whilst all catarrhines so far studied are trichromatic, all platyrrhines, with the two exceptions of howler (Alouatta spp.– uniformly trichromatic; Jacobs et al., 1996a) and night monkeys (Aotus spp. –uniformly monochromatic; Jacobs et al.,1996b; Jacobs,1984; Mollon et al.,1984), and some strepsirhines(Tan and Li, 1999;Jacobs et al., 2002) have a polymorphic colour vision system. All males and homozygous females are dichromats, whilst heterozygous females are trichromats. In platyrrhines, two loci code for the visual pigment proteins or opsins. The first, an autosomal locus, has a single allele that codes for the short wavelength (S) opsin and is common to all individuals. The second, on the X chromosome, codes for opsins within the long to medium wavelength (LM) range. A single X-linked locus model, with three alleles, explains the visual polymorphism observed in callitrichids (Mollon et al.,1984).

For non-human species it is necessary to take account of the animal's perceptual abilities. Thus, we should not relate our verbal classification of colours to colour discriminability or memorability for another species; even one with the same set of photopigments. A good starting point for understanding how other species might discriminate colours is to measure spectral stimuli and estimate the responses of their photoreceptors(Table 1).

Table 1.

Sex and visual status of experimental animals

SpeciesID #SexVisual statusOpsins (nm)
Saddleback 2422 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
tamarin 3894 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
 3948 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
 2214 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
 1045 Female Dichromat 423, 543 
 3895 Male Dichromat 423, 543 
 989 Male Dichromat 423, 563 
 2365 Male Dichromat 423, 563 
Red-bellied 3782 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
tamarin 3873 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
 2972 Female Dichromat 423, 563 
 2666 Female Dichromat 423, 563 
 657 Female Dichromat 423, 563 
 874 Male Dichromat 423, 543 
 3201 Male Dichromat 423, 563 
 3874 Male Dichromat 423, 563 
SpeciesID #SexVisual statusOpsins (nm)
Saddleback 2422 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
tamarin 3894 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
 3948 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
 2214 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
 1045 Female Dichromat 423, 543 
 3895 Male Dichromat 423, 543 
 989 Male Dichromat 423, 563 
 2365 Male Dichromat 423, 563 
Red-bellied 3782 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
tamarin 3873 Female Trichromat 423, 543, 563 
 2972 Female Dichromat 423, 563 
 2666 Female Dichromat 423, 563 
 657 Female Dichromat 423, 563 
 874 Male Dichromat 423, 543 
 3201 Male Dichromat 423, 563 
 3874 Male Dichromat 423, 563 

The perceptual capabilities of various primate visual systems have been modelled to examine the potential advantages of trichromacy in detecting ripe fruits (e.g. Osorio and Vorobyev,1996; Sumner and Mollon,2000a,b;Regan et al., 2001) or flush leaves (Dominy and Lucas,2001). The most pertinent stimuli for such modelling are those actually seen by the visual system of the primate in question in the wild. However, these models make (varying) assumptions about how photoreceptor signals are used to make behavioural decisions (e.g.Vorobyev and Osorio, 1998). For any given perceptual task we cannot be sure that model assumptions will hold. To examine whether an actual foraging advantage is conferred by trichromacy, the relative performance of actual subjects must be measured. For example, Caine and Mundy (2000)used artificially coloured food to show a trichromatic advantage for Geoffroy's marmosets (Callithrix geoffroyi) in a foraging task.

Whilst modelling and behavioural experiments imply that trichromacy is advantageous, this has yet to be demonstrated for a colour discrimination task that closely resembles that faced by primates foraging in their natural habitat. This is the goal of the present study. The relative efficiency of di-and trichromacy for tamarins (Saguinus spp.) is evaluated through an experimental protocol utilising captive monkeys and stimuli recreated from the reflectance spectra of actual fruits eaten (and their associated leaves) by wild tamarins in Peru and presented in a dappled naturalistic leaf canopy.

Field observations

Field site and monkeys

Two mixed-species groups of saddleback (Saguinus fuscicollis nigrifrons I. Geoffroy 1850) (N=4 and 8 individuals) and moustached (Saguinus mystax mystax Spix 1823) tamarins (N=5 and 8 individuals) were observed (by A.C.S.) for 164 days (1612 h) from January 2000 until December 2000 at the Estación Biológica Quebrada Blanco II (4°21′ S, 73°09′ W) in northeastern Peru (for details, see Heymann and Hartmann, 1991). The tamarins were observed for approximately 14 days each month.

Data collection and analysis

All observed instances of fruit feeding were recorded. From these data, the number of `tamarin feeding minutes' was calculated (where one `tamarin feeding minute' equals one tamarin feeding for 1 min) and divided by the number of tamarins of the given species to account for differences in group size between groups, and species, and over the course of the study. Furthermore, each month's data were weighted equally to account for slight differences in the number of observation days.

Colour measurement

Colour measurements were taken using a portable S2000 spectrometer, HL2000 halogen light source (both Ocean Optics, Dunedin, FL, USA) and Satellite 4030CDT laptop computer (Toshiba) running SpectraWin 4.1 software (Top Sensor Systems, Eerbeek, The Netherlands). Reflectance spectra from a minimum of three fruits and three associated mature leaves were recorded for each species eaten. Where possible, spectra were recorded from parts of fruits discarded by tamarins as they fed and taken from both the upper and lower surfaces of leaf samples. Spectra were recorded on the day that the samples were collected.

Colour modelling

We estimated the responses of the tamarin's photoreceptors, and hence colour signals to spectral stimuli, as follows. We derived tamarin photoreceptor spectral sensitivities in vivo by fitting a standard exponential model of rhodopsin absorption(Stavenga et al., 1993) to spectral sensitivity maxima measured for common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) cones with sensitivity maxima at 425 nm, 543 nm, 555 nm and 562 nm (Williams et al., 1992),which are close to those for Saguinus(Jacobs et al., 1987) assuming a maximum optical density of 0.4. Spectral absorption by the ocular media was also based on the common marmoset(Tovée et al., 1992). Recent work (Kawamura et al.,2001) lowers the estimated sensitivity maximum of the common marmoset 543 nm receptor to 539 nm; this difference is of negligible significance for the design and interpretation of our study.

Spectral stimuli reaching the eye depend upon the reflectance and illumination spectra. Reflectance was measured as described above, and the illumination spectrum was natural sunlight measured by a spectroradiometer calibrated with a known standard (LS1-cal; Ocean Optics). For an eye viewing the surface of an object, the (relative) quantum catch of the receptor i (Qi) is given by the following expression:
\[\ Q_{i}={{\int}_{{\lambda}_{\mathrm{min}}}^{{\lambda}_{\mathrm{max}}}}R_{\mathrm{i}}({\lambda})S({\lambda})I({\lambda})\mathrm{d}{\lambda},\]
1
where λ denotes wavelength, λmin andλ max denote the lower and upper limits of the visible spectrum, respectively, Ri(λ) is the spectral sensitivity of receptor i, S(λ) is the reflectance spectrum and I(λ) is the illumination spectrum. The receptor response normalised to the illuminant qi is then given by: qi=Qi(t)/Qi(i),where Qi(t) and Qi(i) are estimated quantum catches for a target and the barium sulphate reflectance standard,respectively. Finally, stimulus chromaticities(Fig. 1) were given by Macleod and Boynton (1979)chromaticity coordinates based on outputs of marmoset 425 nm (S), 543 nm (M)and 562 nm (L) cone photoreceptors (see alsoRegan et al., 1998). The Cartesion coordinates are given by S/(L+M) and L/(L+M), which is convenient because S/(L+M) roughly represents the blue–yellow chromatic signal available to a dichromat, while the red–green parameter, L/(L+M), is available only to trichromats. Although the colours used for the experiments did not exactly match those of the plant(Fig. 1), the chromaticity differences between the leaf background and fully ripe fruit were very similar for the real and experimental colours, with the `unripe' and `mid-ripe' model fruit lying at intermediate locations on the red–green axis.
Fig. 1.

Chromaticities of natural Abuta fluminum leaves and fruit and of the model colours used in this experiment, plotted in a standard chromaticity diagram modified for the common marmoset eye (see text;Macleod and Boynton, 1979;Regan et al., 1998). Colour differences on the horizontal axis are visible only to trichromats. Note that distance in this diagram does not directly predict colour discriminability. For example, in general, a given colour distance on the vertical axis will be less discriminable than on the horizontal. L, leaf; U, unripe; M, mid-ripe; R,ripe.

Fig. 1.

Chromaticities of natural Abuta fluminum leaves and fruit and of the model colours used in this experiment, plotted in a standard chromaticity diagram modified for the common marmoset eye (see text;Macleod and Boynton, 1979;Regan et al., 1998). Colour differences on the horizontal axis are visible only to trichromats. Note that distance in this diagram does not directly predict colour discriminability. For example, in general, a given colour distance on the vertical axis will be less discriminable than on the horizontal. L, leaf; U, unripe; M, mid-ripe; R,ripe.

Diet composition and choice of representative fruit species

The tamarins ate fruits from 833 plants from 167 species in 87 genera and 50 families during 164 days of observation. Abuta was chosen as representative of ripe fruit eaten by tamarins for which trichromatic colour vision may give an advantage in the detection and selection. It formed a significant part of the diet of both species in both groups. It was eaten in all months but two; no other genus was eaten in as many months. It was chosen over other important genera (i.e. Parkia, Tapirira, Pourouma, Buchenavia,Unonopsis and Simaba), as these genera typically ripened to a dark purple or black colour for which trichromacy has little benefit, and over Inga, as the bean-like fruit pods of many species of this genus may be deemed cryptic since they remain green even when mature. Six species of Abuta were eaten by the tamarins: A. arborea, A. fluminum, A. imene, A. pahni, A. rufescens and A. solimoensis. Of these, A. fluminum was chosen as representative as it accounted for the greatest number of feeding records. Fig. 2 shows the reflectance spectra of ripe and unripe A. fluminum fruit and leaves (upper surface).

Fig. 2.

Reflectance spectra of ripe and unripe A. fluminum fruit and leaves (upper surface).

Fig. 2.

Reflectance spectra of ripe and unripe A. fluminum fruit and leaves (upper surface).

The fruits and leaves of A. fluminum occupy roughly mid positions on the L/(L+M) axis (the red–green parameter available only to trichromats) of all the species sampled. Of the ripe fruits sampled, those of A. fluminum have a value of 0.5474±0.0052 (N=12 fruits), from a range spanning 0.5032–0.5914 (N=137 species),whereas the leaves of A. fluminum have a value of 0.5009±0.0021 (N=9 leaves), from a range of 0.4957–0.5147 (N=154 species). Their chromaticity is similar to that of other fruits eaten by tamarins and also by other primates(Sumner and Mollon, 2000b;Regan et al., 2001).

Captive experiment

Animals and housing

Eight captive adult saddleback (S. fuscicollis weddelli Deville 1849) and six red-bellied tamarins (S. labiatus labiatus Geoffroy in Humboldt 1812) held at the Belfast Zoological Gardens were observed (by A.C.S.) in the experiment. The numbers of each species are given for each sex and visual phenotype in Table 1. Effort was made to balance sex and visual status across species from the animals available.

The monkeys were housed in standard indoor/outdoor enclosures off-exhibit. Testing took place in the outside enclosures (1.95 m×1.55 m×3.50 m). Each was furnished with a network of approximately eight branches (5 cm to>10 cm diameter), with the three branches closest to the test apparatus placed in the same configuration. The monkeys were accustomed to being held individually in the outside enclosures.

Genotyping

Visual status was determined genetically (by A.K.S.), by amplification and sequencing of the X-linked opsin gene. Tamarin opsin alleles can be defined by four amino acid substitutions at positions 180 in exon 3, 229 and 233 in exon 4 and 285 in exon 5, which are important for spectral tuning(Shyue et al., 1998). DNA was extracted from plucked hair samples from each individual tamarin using a QIAamp DNA mini-kit (Qiagen, Crawley, UK). PCR and sequence analysis of exons 3, 4 and 5 were performed as previously described(Surridge and Mundy, 2002). Genotypes were assigned according to the combined sequence of the four important amino acids in each of the exons mentioned above. These are as follows for each of the three opsin alleles: 543 nm=Ala, Ile, Ser, Ala; 556 nm=Ala, Phe, Ser, Thr; 563 nm=Ser, Phe, Gly, Thr. Trichromatic females were identified by the presence of heterozygous sites in the DNA sequence at these important positions.

Test apparatus

The apparatus consisted of two rigid, wire grid panels. One was covered with laminated paper leaves (leaf background) and the other was unadorned (no background). The leaves, in the oval shape of A. fluminum, ranged from 70 mm×50 mm to 150 mm×115 mm. They were arranged to form a naturalistic canopy, giving dappled lighting from the incident daylight. The randomly varying degrees of illumination from the dappled light ensured that the task could not be solved by brightness cues of the targets alone. Twenty-one fruit bases, made from 1.5 mm card, were fixed at regular intervals as per Fig. 3. Each was covered with a lid, also made from 1.5 mm card that overhung and covered its sides. The lids were covered in one of three colours of paper corresponding to unripe, mid-ripe and ripe A. fluminum fruit. Ripe fruits contained 0.5 g fudge, mid-ripe contained 0.25 g fudge and unripe fruits contained no reward. The pattern of the fruit locations was varied systematically.

Fig. 3.

Diagram of artificial fruit and its coloured lid, and the pattern of the 21 test fruits.

Fig. 3.

Diagram of artificial fruit and its coloured lid, and the pattern of the 21 test fruits.

The leaves were made from a commercially available green paper, the reflectance spectrum of which roughly matched that of real A. fluminum leaves, although overall the colour was somewhat brighter than the real leaves (Table 2,TBL3;Fig. 4). Fruit lid colours were calculated to differ in chromaticity from the model leaves in the same way that natural fruits differ from natural leaves(Fig. 1). This design, with dappled lighting, means that as a test of colour vision the experimental task closely resembles the task faced in natural foraging. We modelled ripe,mid-ripe and unripe A. fluminum fruit(Table 2,TBL3). Colours were made in Adobe Photoshop and printed using an Epson Color 580 inkjet printer.

Table 2.

Quantum catches, relative to a barium sulphate white standard, of tamarin cones for A. fluminum fruit and leaves and recreated stimuli

Fruit and leaves
425 nm543 nm556 nm562 nm
StimulusActualModelActualModelActualModelActualModel
Ripe fruit 0.0227±0.0088 0.0832 0.1666±0.0204 0.2415 0.1908±0.0247 0.2531 0.2017±0.0268 0.2577 
 (12)  (12)  (12)  (12)  
Mid-ripe fruit 0.0181 0.1792 0.1071 0.4628 0.1201 0.4632 0.1257 0.4611 
Unripe fruit 0.0136±0.0008 0.1349 0.0477±0.0018 0.3927 0.0494±0.0025 0.3834 0.0497±0.0028 0.3768 
 (2)  (2)  (2)  (2)  
Leaf (upper side) 0.0087±0.0025 0.1583 0.0483±0.0133 0.3976 0.0489±0.0137 0.3767 0.0485±0.0138 0.3653 
 (9)  (9)  (9)  (9)  
Fruit and leaves
425 nm543 nm556 nm562 nm
StimulusActualModelActualModelActualModelActualModel
Ripe fruit 0.0227±0.0088 0.0832 0.1666±0.0204 0.2415 0.1908±0.0247 0.2531 0.2017±0.0268 0.2577 
 (12)  (12)  (12)  (12)  
Mid-ripe fruit 0.0181 0.1792 0.1071 0.4628 0.1201 0.4632 0.1257 0.4611 
Unripe fruit 0.0136±0.0008 0.1349 0.0477±0.0018 0.3927 0.0494±0.0025 0.3834 0.0497±0.0028 0.3768 
 (2)  (2)  (2)  (2)  
Leaf (upper side) 0.0087±0.0025 0.1583 0.0483±0.0133 0.3976 0.0489±0.0137 0.3767 0.0485±0.0138 0.3653 
 (9)  (9)  (9)  (9)  

Recreated stimuli
S 425nmM 543nmL 562nmS/(L+M)L/(L+M)
StimulusActualModelActualModelActualModelActualModelActualModel
Ripe fruit 0.023 0.083 0.167 0.242 0.2017 0.259 0.0616 0.1667 0.5477 0.5163 
Mid-ripe fruit 0.0181 0.179 0.1071 0.463 0.1257 0.461 0.0777 0.1940 0.5399 0.4991 
Unripe fruit 0.0136 0.135 0.0477 0.393 0.0497 0.377 0.1396 0.1753 0.5103 0.4897 
Leaf 0.0087 0.158 0.0483 0.398 0.0485 0.365 0.0899 0.2075 0.5010 0.4789 
Recreated stimuli
S 425nmM 543nmL 562nmS/(L+M)L/(L+M)
StimulusActualModelActualModelActualModelActualModelActualModel
Ripe fruit 0.023 0.083 0.167 0.242 0.2017 0.259 0.0616 0.1667 0.5477 0.5163 
Mid-ripe fruit 0.0181 0.179 0.1071 0.463 0.1257 0.461 0.0777 0.1940 0.5399 0.4991 
Unripe fruit 0.0136 0.135 0.0477 0.393 0.0497 0.377 0.1396 0.1753 0.5103 0.4897 
Leaf 0.0087 0.158 0.0483 0.398 0.0485 0.365 0.0899 0.2075 0.5010 0.4789 

N is given in parentheses

Fig. 4.

A saddleback tamarin foraging for the artificial fruits when presented on a leaf background.

Fig. 4.

A saddleback tamarin foraging for the artificial fruits when presented on a leaf background.

Protocol

Tamarins were tested individually in their outside enclosures. There were two conditions: condition 1, where 21 fruits, seven of each of three colours,were presented against no background (the plain wire mesh of the guide frame and cage wall), and condition 2, where the same fruits were presented against a leaf background (Fig. 4). Each tamarin received training trials until it had successfully located and taken six fruits. These trials were performed as for condition 2. The experiment was split into two phases: phase 1 was three trials of condition 1,and phase 2 was three trials of condition 2.

Trials were terminated either after the tamarin had taken all 21 fruits or after 15 min, whichever was sooner. During each trial, the time and colour of the fruit the tamarin took was continuously recorded using a hand-held computer running the Observer TM package (Tracksys Ltd., Nottingham, UK). General linear models run through SPSS were used for statistical comparisons.

Results

Trichromats required significantly fewer training trials than their dichromatic counterparts (1.83±1.33 vs 4.60±2.88,respectively: F1,10=9.40, P<0.05) to reach the criterion of six fruits taken. Neither species (saddleback, 2.38±1.60;redbellied, 4.75±3.20: F1,10=1.29, P>0.05) nor sex (male, 3.17±2.64; female, 3.80±2.90: F1,10=4.52, P>0.05) had a significant effect on number of trials to criterion, nor were the interactions of species and vision (F1,10=0.97, P>0.05) and species and sex (F1,10=0.01, P>0.05) significant.

To examine the efficiency with which fruits were selected, the number of ripe fruits within the first six fruits taken was calculated. When the fruits were presented against both the no background and the leaf background,trichromats took significantly more ripe fruits than did dichromats(Table 3). There were no other significant effects.

Table 3.

Mean number of ripe fruits (±S.D.) taken within the first six fruits

Fruits against no backgroundFruits against leaf background
Effect/interactionCategory (N)Mean no. ripe fruitsF1,10PMean no. ripe fruitsF1,10P
Visual status Trichromat (6) 3.28±0.49 7.71 0.02 3.06±0.80 8.08 <0.05 
 Dichromat (10) 2.35±0.59   2.13±0.64   
Species Saddleback (8) 2.75±0.61 0.51 >0.05 2.27±0.60 4.63 >0.05 
 Red-bellied (8) 2.65±0.84   2.69±0.99   
Sex Female (10) 2.80±0.77 0.78 >0.05 2.67±0.82 0.36 >0.05 
 Male (6) 2.53±0.62   2.17±0.78   
Species × visual status   0.16 >0.05  0.16 >0.05 
Species × sex   0.62 >0.05  0.22 >0.05 
Fruits against no backgroundFruits against leaf background
Effect/interactionCategory (N)Mean no. ripe fruitsF1,10PMean no. ripe fruitsF1,10P
Visual status Trichromat (6) 3.28±0.49 7.71 0.02 3.06±0.80 8.08 <0.05 
 Dichromat (10) 2.35±0.59   2.13±0.64   
Species Saddleback (8) 2.75±0.61 0.51 >0.05 2.27±0.60 4.63 >0.05 
 Red-bellied (8) 2.65±0.84   2.69±0.99   
Sex Female (10) 2.80±0.77 0.78 >0.05 2.67±0.82 0.36 >0.05 
 Male (6) 2.53±0.62   2.17±0.78   
Species × visual status   0.16 >0.05  0.16 >0.05 
Species × sex   0.62 >0.05  0.22 >0.05 

Whether the fruits were presented against a leaf background or not had no significant effect on the number of ripe fruits within the first six fruits taken (no background 2.70±0.71; leaf background 2.48±0.82: F1,14=1.41, P>0.05). There was no interaction of visual status and background (F1,14=0.001, P>0.05) nor was there a difference between dichromats and trichromats in the total number of ripe fruits taken by the end of each trial,either when presented against no background (dichromat, 6.30±0.66;trichromat, 6.33±0.73: F1,14=0.009, P>0.05) or a leaf background (dichromat, 5.43±1.29;trichromat, 6.05±0.53: F1,14=1.25, P>0.05).

The main finding is that trichromacy confers an advantage when selecting ripe fruits from those at various stages of maturity; both as a simple task and also when presented as a more naturalistic complex task against a background of distracting leaves. This is the first time that such an advantage has been demonstrated for primates using naturalistic stimuli. In addition, the patchy illumination falling on the fruit and leaves in our experiments resembles that of a natural forest canopy with areas of shadow and sun. These are conditions that might favour colour vision. Despite the benefits of trichromacy in the efficient detection and selection of ripe fruit, the selection of heterozygous trichromats will maintain both trichromacy and dichromacy within the population since, within the X-linked single-locus model, males are always dichromats irrespective of their mother's visual status (Mollon et al.,1984).

The three alleles of the single-locus model give three trichromat phenotypes and three dichromat phenotypes. The spectral tuning of the opsins of each phenotype may render them each more or less advantageous under different photic conditions. Even at a given time of day there are vast differences in illumination within a rain forest. It would repay investigation to examine the actual foraging efficiencies of the different phenotypes using real-world stimuli under a variety of naturalistic lighting conditions. Similarly, it would have been informative to examine differences in the relative performance of each of the three dichromatic and three trichromatic phenotypes, but distribution of the phenotypes of the available animals did not permit this. Indeed, all of the trichromats were 423 nm, 543 nm, 563 nm,and the small sample size did not permit examination of differences between the two dichromat phenotypes in the study, namely 423 nm, 563 nm and 423 nm,543 nm.

Although we have found that trichromacy is advantageous for detection and selection of ripe fruit (at least for the phenotypes present in our study),this does not give a complete picture of the likely costs and benefits of colour vision. Nor does this result demonstrate that trichromacy originally evolved for foraging. For example, trichromacy has been suggested as being more efficient for detecting yellow predators against a green leafy background(Coss and Ramakrishnan, 2000). Examples might include the yellowish jaguar (Panthera onca), ocelot(Leopardus pardalis), margay (L. wiedii) and oncilla (L. tigrina), all of which live in the Neotropics. Dichromacy, however, may be advantageous in breaking camouflage(Morgan et al., 1992). This is relevant not only for detection of predators but also for the detection of insect and other prey items that are taken by many primate species. However, a recent study failed to find any evidence of a dichromat advantage in terms of the number of prey captured by wild and captive tamarins (H. M. Buchanan-Smith, A. C. Smith, A. K. Surridge, M. J. Prescott, D. Osorio and N. I. Mundy, manuscript in preparation).

The detection and discrimination of fruits is a complex task. Fruits must be distinguished from leaves, edible fruits must be discriminated from inedible or toxic fruits, and ripe fruits must be typically picked over unripe fruits. Colouration may aid in all of these tasks; indeed, as this study has shown, primate trichromacy is advantageous in the efficient selection of ripe fruits from an array of unripe, mid-ripe and ripe fruits. However, there are many subtle factors other than colour per se that can influence the choice of fruits by wild primates. As Savage et al.(1987) point out,discrimination may be most acute for those foods that are rarely consumed yet are an essential source of one or more nutrients.

In Peru: we are grateful to Dr E. Montoya (Proyecto Peruano de Primatologia) and Biologo R. Pezo (Universidad Nacional de la Amazonia Peruana) for help with logistical matters; and particular thanks to Ney Shahuano for unflagging field assistance. In the UK: we are grateful to John Stronge and Mark Challis at Belfast Zoological Gardens for continued support of our research, and the zoo staff for maintaining the study animals. We thank Drs S. Vick and J. Kren for comments on an early draft of this manuscript. This study was funded by the BBSRC (98/S11498 to H.M.B.-S.).

Ahnelt, P. K. and Kolb, H. (
2000
). The mammalian photoreceptor mosaic-adaptive design.
Prog. Retin. Eye Res.
19
,
711
-777.
Arrese, C. A., Hart, N. S., Thomas, N., Beazley, L. D. and Shand, J. (
2002
). Trichromacy in Australian Marsupials.
Curr. Biol.
12
,
657
-660.
Caine, N. G. and Mundy, N. I. (
2000
). Demonstration of a foraging advantage for trichromatic marmosets(Callithrix geoffroyi) dependent on food color.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
267
,
439
-444.
Coss, R. G. and Ramakrishnan, U. (
2000
). Perceptual aspects of leopard recognition by wild bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata).
Behavior
137
,
315
-335.
Dominy, N. J. and Lucas, P. W. (
2001
). Ecological importance of trichromatic vision to primates.
Nature
410
,
363
-366.
Guillotin, M., Dubost, G. and Sabatier, D.(
1994
). Food choice and food competition among three major primate species of French Guiana.
J. Zool. Lond.
233
,
551
-579.
Heymann, E. W. and Hartmann, G. (
1991
). Geophagy in mustached tamarins, Saguinus mystax (Platyrrhini:Callitrichidae), at the Rio Blanco, Peruvian Amazonia.
Primates
32
,
533
-537.
Jacobs, G. H. (
1993
). The distribution and nature of color vision among the mammals.
Biol. Rev.
68
,
413
-471.
Jacobs, G. H. (
1984
). Within-species variations in the visual capacity among squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus):color vision.
Vision Res.
24
,
1267
-1277.
Jacobs, G. H., Deegan, J. F., II, Tan, Y. and Li, W.-H.(
2002
). Opsin gene and photopigment polymorhpism in a prosimian primate.
Vision Res.
42
,
11
-18.
Jacobs, G. H., Neitz, J. and Crognale, M.(
1987
). Color-vision polymorphism and its photopigment basis in a callitrichid monkey (Saguinus fuscicollis).
Vision Res.
27
,
2089
-2100.
Jacobs, G. H., Neitz, M., Deegan, J. F. and Neitz, J.(
1996a
). Trichromatic color vision in New World monkeys.
Nature
382
,
156
-158.
Jacobs, G. H., Neitz, M. and Neitz, J. (
1996b
). Mutations in S-cone pigment genes and the absence of colour vision in two species of nocturnal primate.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
263
,
705
-710.
Julliot, C. (
1994
). Frugivory and seed dispersal by red howler monkeys: evolutionary aspect.
Revu d'Ecologie (Terre Vie)
49
,
331
-341.
Kawamura, S., Hirai, M., Takenaka, O., Radlwimmer, F. B. and Yokoyama, S. (
2001
). Genomic and spectral analyses of long to middle wavelength-sensitive visual pigments of common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus).
Gene
269
,
45
-51.
Macleod, D. I. A. and Boynton, R. M. (
1979
). Chromaticity diagram showing cone excitation by stimuli of equal luminance.
J. Opt. Soc. Am.
69
,
1183
-1186.
Mollon, J. D., Bowmaker, J. K. and Jacobs, G. H.(
1984
). Variations of color vision in a New World primate can be explained by polymorphism of retinal photopigments.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
222
,
373
-399.
Morgan, M. J., Adam, A. and Mollon, J. D.(
1992
). Dicromats detect colorcamouflaged objects that are not detected by trichromats.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
248
,
291
-295.
Osorio, D. and Vorobyev, M. (
1996
). Color vision as an adaptation to frugivory in primates.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
263
,
593
-599.
Regan, B. C., Julliot, C., Simmen, B., Vienot, F.,Charles-Dominique, P. and Mollon, J. D. (
1998
). Frugivory and colour vision in Alouatta seniculus, a trichromatic platyrrhine monkey.
Vision Res.
38
,
3321
-3327.
Regan, B. C., Julliot, C., Simmen, B., Vienot, F.,Charles-Dominique, P. and Mollon, J. D. (
2001
). Fruits,foliage and the evolution of color vision.
Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B
356
,
229
-283.
Richard, A. F. (
1985
).
Primates in Nature
. New York: W. H. Freeman & Co.
Savage, A., Dronzek, L. A. and Snowdon, C. T.(
1987
). Color discrimination by the cotton-top tamarin(Saguinus oedipus oedipus) and its relation to fruit coloration.
Folia Primatol.
49
,
57
-69.
Shyue, S. K., Boissinot, S., Schneider, H., Sampaio, I.,Schneider, M. P., Abee, C. R., Williams, L., Hewett-Emmett, D., Sperling, H. G., Cowing, J. A. et al. (
1998
). Molecular genetics of spectral tuning in New World monkey colour vision.
J. Mol. Evol.
46
,
697
-702.
Stavenga, D. G., Smits, R. P. and Hoenders, B. J.(
1993
). Simple exponential functions describing the absorbency bands of visual pigment spectra.
Vision Res.
33
,
1011
-1017.
Sumner, P, and Mollon, J. D. (
2000a
). Catarrhine photopigments are optimized for detecting targets against a foliage background.
J. Exp. Biol.
203
,
1963
-1986.
Sumner, P. and Mollon, J. D. (
2000b
). Chromaticity as a signal of ripeness in fruits taken by primates.
J. Exp. Biol.
203
,
1987
-2000.
Surridge, A. K. and Mundy, N. I. (
2002
). Trans-specific evolution of opsin alleles and the maintenance of trichromatic colour vision in callitrichine primates.
Mol. Ecol.
11
,
2157
-2170.
Tan, Y. and Li, W.-H. (
1999
). Trichromatic vision in prosimians.
Nature
402
,
36
.
Tovée, M. J., Bowmaker, J. K. and Mollon, J. D.(
1992
). The relationship between cone pigments and behavioural sensitivity in a New World monkey (Callithrix jacchus jacchus).
Vision Res.
32
,
867
-878.
Vorobyev, M. and Osorio, D. (
1998
). Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
265
,
351
-358.
Williams, A. J., Hunt, D. M., Bowmaker, J. K. and Mollon, J. D. (
1992
). The polymorphic photopigments of the marmoset:spectral tuning and genetic basis.
EMBO J.
11
,
2039
-2045.