Many tropical butterfly species are well-known for their migratory behaviour. Although these insects can maintain a constant direction throughout the day, the physiological mechanisms of orientation are unknown. It has been argued that tropical migrant butterflies must use a time-compensated sun compass to accomplish their journey, but the crucial experimental manipulations to test this hypothesis have not been conducted. This study reports the results of clock-shift experiments performed with two species of migrating butterflies (Pieridae: Aphrissa statira and Phoebis argante) captured during flight across Lake Gatún, Panamá. The observed constant flight bearing of natural controls suggests that these species are capable of performing time-compensated celestial navigation. Our clock-shift experiments suggest that a sun compass is involved. Individuals submitted to a 4 h advance shift took significantly different mean orientations on release compared with control butterflies. The direction of this difference was consistent with the use of a sun compass. The magnitude was approximately half the predicted value if the vanishing bearing of released butterflies was used as the variable to evaluate the effect of time-shifting and approximately three-quarters of that predicted if the estimated heading was the variable used. Mean vanishing bearings of control and experimental butterflies did not correspond to predicted values. This difference can be attributed largely to the combined effects of wind and handling.

Navigation is usually required to ensure that long-distance migrants reach a particular destination. Two forms of navigation, orientation to a fixed compass bearing and goal-orientation (i.e. true navigation and goal-finding using innate vector programs or navigational maps; Berthold, 1993), require the use of an orientation cue at a great distance (essentially infinite) relative to the position of the animal. For diurnal migrants, such as the butterfly species in the present study, the sun or the geomagnetic pole may provide such a cue. Dependence on one or both of these cues becomes mandatory if migrants must traverse areas where locally visible landmarks are limited and where the topography and wind conditions are variable (Dingle, 1996). In Panamá, Aphrissa statira butterflies flying over a lake compensated, at least in part, for being blown off course by the wind. These results are consistent with either form of navigation outlined above (Srygley et al. 1996). On a larger scale, the particularly complex land-mass of Central America may additionally demand precise control of direction to avoid migrants being lost at sea.

The use of the sun as an orientation cue has been demonstrated in many vertebrate and invertebrate species (Santschi, 1911; von Frisch, 1967; Able, 1980; Wehner, 1984; Waterman, 1989; Wehner et al. 1996). In its simplest form, sun orientation does not incorporate compensation for time of day, so that the animal’s direction of movement changes with the apparent movement of the sun (Baker, 1968a,b, 1969; Wallraff, 1981). The more complex time-compensated sun compass enables a constant direction to be maintained throughout the day. Time-compensation is achieved using an internal circadian clock set to the local time by entrainment to the natural light:dark cycle. Such a mechanism is best demonstrated by clock-shift experiments, in which induced phase shifts in the circadian endogenous clock cause predictable changes in orientation (Emlen, 1975; Able, 1980; Wallraff, 1981; Wehner, 1984; Schmidt-Koenig et al. 1991a). Although many butterfly species are well-known migrants and are often observed to maintain a constant bearing throughout the day (Williams, 1930; Nielsen, 1961; Johnson, 1969; Schmidt-Koenig, 1985; Walker and Riordan, 1981; Walker, 1985; Oliveira, 1990), until recently the source of their compass information remained unknown (Able, 1980; Baker, 1984; Wehner, 1984; Schmidt-Koenig, 1985; Walker and Littell, 1994; Brower, 1996; Dingle, 1996). Able (1980) and Baker (1984) argued that long-distance migrant butterflies (e.g. the monarch Danaus plexippus; see Brower, 1995) must use a time-compensated sun compass to accomplish their journey. Observations that particularly suggested the use of a sun compass were made by Baker (1968a,b, 1969) for short-distance migrant butterflies in Europe, by Kanz (1977) for D. plexippus in experimental cages, and by Oliveira (1990) for migration of pierids (including the species reported in the present study) in the eastern Brazilian Amazon. These studies present examples in which butterflies showed only partial time-compensation (or none at all) and their orientation tended to follow the sun’s azimuth (the compass bearing of the nearest point on the horizon to the sun). Recent work by Perez et al. (1997) has provided experimental clock-shift evidence for a sun compass in monarch butterflies.

In central Panamá, butterflies of the species Aphrissa statira (Cramer), A. boisduvalii (Felder), Phoebis argante (Fabricius), P. trite (L.), P. sennae (L.), P. philea (L.), Marpesia chiron (Fabricius) and M. petreus (Cramer) are routinely observed in migratory flight from mid May to late July (for Aphrissa spp., see Fig. 1). Migrating butterflies appear just after the onset of the rainy season, and south-southwest is the predominant migratory direction (Fig. 2). A map of the flyway across the isthmus of Panamá for Aphrissa spp. is shown in Srygley et al. (1996). The daily peak of migratory activity occurs between 10:00 and 12:00 h, although activity may last from 09:00 to 15:00 h or later on sunny, hot days (Fig. 3). The present study focused on two species in this migratory system, A. statira and P. argante (Pieridae: Coliadinae). We first determined whether naturally migrating butterflies maintain a constant direction throughout the day. Using experimental manipulation of the butterflies’ endogenous clock, we then investigated whether they use the sun as an orientation cue and whether they adjust their flight orientation to compensate for the sun’s apparent movement.

Fig. 1.

Observed maximum number per minute of migrating Aphrissa spp. butterflies flying south-westerly across a 300 m transect on Lake Gatún, Panamá, during May–July of 1991–1996. A solid line connecting the points indicates successive daily counts. We pooled data for A. statira and A. boisduvalii because it is difficult to distinguish these two species during free flight.

Fig. 1.

Observed maximum number per minute of migrating Aphrissa spp. butterflies flying south-westerly across a 300 m transect on Lake Gatún, Panamá, during May–July of 1991–1996. A solid line connecting the points indicates successive daily counts. We pooled data for A. statira and A. boisduvalii because it is difficult to distinguish these two species during free flight.

Fig. 2.

Migratory direction of free-flying Aphrissa spp. and Phoebis argante butterflies across Lake Gatún in 1992–1996. Length of radii corresponds to the number of observations within each 10 ° arc. An arc outside each circle shows the mean direction with 95 % confidence intervals. The scale is different in each diagram, because of differing sample sizes. The outer arc represents 70, 65, 60, 65 and 20 individuals for Aphrissa spp. for 1992–1996, respectively, and 12, 7, 20, 20 and individuals for P. argante. Here, and in subsequent figures, bearings are given with reference to magnetic north. Local magnetic declination between 1992 and 1996 was negligible (range approximately 51′ to 1°28′; see Peddie, 1993) relative to measurement imprecision.

Fig. 2.

Migratory direction of free-flying Aphrissa spp. and Phoebis argante butterflies across Lake Gatún in 1992–1996. Length of radii corresponds to the number of observations within each 10 ° arc. An arc outside each circle shows the mean direction with 95 % confidence intervals. The scale is different in each diagram, because of differing sample sizes. The outer arc represents 70, 65, 60, 65 and 20 individuals for Aphrissa spp. for 1992–1996, respectively, and 12, 7, 20, 20 and individuals for P. argante. Here, and in subsequent figures, bearings are given with reference to magnetic north. Local magnetic declination between 1992 and 1996 was negligible (range approximately 51′ to 1°28′; see Peddie, 1993) relative to measurement imprecision.

Fig. 3.

Number of migrating Aphrissa spp. butterflies flying south-westerly across a 300 m transect on Lake Gatún at different times on the day of highest migration rate of each year between 1991 and 1996 (see Fig. 1). For 1994, data for the day of second highest migration rate are plotted because for the day of highest rate we did not have samples at different times of day. One (1) was added to individual counts to allow logarithmic representation of counts that yielded no individuals. Migratory activity normally occurred from 09:00 to 15:00 h with a peak between 10:00 and 12:00 h, but varied across days according to the prevailing weather. Activity often ceased in the early afternoon owing to a rapid deterioration in the weather that normally occurs at this time of day in this area (see Windsor, 1990).

Fig. 3.

Number of migrating Aphrissa spp. butterflies flying south-westerly across a 300 m transect on Lake Gatún at different times on the day of highest migration rate of each year between 1991 and 1996 (see Fig. 1). For 1994, data for the day of second highest migration rate are plotted because for the day of highest rate we did not have samples at different times of day. One (1) was added to individual counts to allow logarithmic representation of counts that yielded no individuals. Migratory activity normally occurred from 09:00 to 15:00 h with a peak between 10:00 and 12:00 h, but varied across days according to the prevailing weather. Activity often ceased in the early afternoon owing to a rapid deterioration in the weather that normally occurs at this time of day in this area (see Windsor, 1990).

Field and laboratory work were conducted at Barro Colorado Island, Panamá (9°10′ N, 79°51′ W), in May–July of 1991–1996. To determine whether time of day had an effect on the flight direction of naturally migrating butterflies, we intercepted the flight path and measured the track direction of 1739 Aphrissa spp. and 296 P. argante flying across Lake Gatún with the aid of a Suunto KB-14 sighting compass. A 30 horsepower motorboat was used to follow each individual. We regressed flight direction on time of day (h). To meet the assumption of linear regression analysis, values more than ±90 ° from the mean (210 ° for Aphrissa spp. and 196 ° for P. argante) were excluded (7 % of the total for Aphrissa spp. and 5 % for P. argante). To control for the effect of cross-winds, we excluded flight directions measured when wind speed exceeded 2 m s−1 (52 % of the total for Aphrissa spp. and 49 % for P. argante for those individuals within ±90 ° from the respective means). This approach is justified by the observation that these species may compensate fully for cross-wind drift when wind speed does not exceed 2 m s−1 [for Aphrissa spp., y=215−0.92x, where y is heading (in degrees) and x is drift (in m); 95 % confidence intervals −1.61, −0.23; N=63; P<0.009; see methods in Srygley et al. 1996]. Wind direction and speed were measured every 2–5 min using a weathervane and an anemometer (TSI, model 1650).

In the present study, track refers to the path the butterflies take when migrating naturally (before handling), and track direction refers to the direction of that path relative to magnetic north. Flight direction is used for both track direction before handling and vanishing bearing following release (see below). All butterflies used in the clock-shift experiments were captured during flight across the lake. Before capture was attempted, the butterfly’s track direction was measured. Each captured butterfly was placed in a numbered glassine envelope within which it was kept between the time of capture and the time of release. The butterflies captured on each day were sorted by species and into pairs of individuals matched as closely as possible for original flight direction, body mass, wing wear and sex. Individuals from each pair were then assigned randomly to control or experimental treatments.

Experimental individuals were maintained in an environmental chamber (Percival 1-35LL) set to a light:dark photoperiod 4 h ahead of the true solar time (4 h advance shift). Control individuals were kept in a second identical chamber in which the light:dark cycle was in synchrony with the natural cycle. Temperature in the chambers was set to 28 °C during the light phase and 25 °C during the dark phase. These values approximately match those measured in a forest clearing on Barro Colorado Island at the same time of year (Windsor, 1990). To compensate for natural twilight, we set the light phase to begin 10–15 min prior to sunrise (objective and subjective time for controls and experimentals, respectively) and to end 10–15 min after sunset. Control and experimental butterflies were hand-fed 20 % honey solution once daily in their objective and subjective afternoon, respectively. Approximately 15 % of the individuals died during the treatment, 62 % of which were in the experimental group. This latter percentage is not significantly different from 50 % (binomial test with normal approximation, Z=1.60, 0.10<P<0.20).

After 2–4 days under these conditions, butterflies were released individually from the boat moored near the centre of Buena Vista bay on Lake Gatún (distance from shoreline approximately 1200 m). Butterflies were not exposed to skylight until release. Control and experimental individuals were released alternately at 2–3 min intervals. For release, each butterfly was placed on the dorsal surface of the releaser’s left hand positioned above his head before release. Butterflies voluntarily adopted variable body orientations prior to take-off. After taking off, the butterfly was followed visually, and its vanishing bearing (the compass direction in which the butterfly could no longer be seen with the naked eye) was recorded from the point of release. The individuals disappeared from sight approximately 300–500 m from the point of release. Less than 2 % of released individuals landed on the water within approximately 50 m of the site of release. These individuals were excluded from the analysis. All releases were performed by E.G.O. between 10:00 and 11:15 h, and only when the position of the sun was clearly identifiable. Clock-shift releases were performed between 28 May and 20 July over the years 1992–1994.

To estimate the wind conditions each individual experienced following release, we again measured wind direction and wind speed every 2–5 min using a weathervane and an anemometer (TSI, model 1650). Wind direction always refers to the direction of the wind source, except (i) in the vector analysis used to estimate headings and (ii) in Figs 5B,E, 6B,E, 9C, where wind direction refers to wind destination.

To measure the effect of handling on the orientation of butterflies, we conducted an experiment in which migrating butterflies were captured, held in glassine envelopes for 5 min, and released from a point equidistant from both shorelines along the same track each individual was originally flying. Captures and releases were performed between 10:00 and 15:00 h. Wind speed and direction were recorded between the capture and release of each individual.

Data analysis

Primary analysis of orientation data used circular statistics methods as described by Batschelet (1981) with the aid of the software Oriana (Kovach, 1994) and Daten (by A. Ganzhorn). The Rayleigh test was used to test for uniform distribution. Watson’s and Watson–Williams’ F-tests were used for two- and multi-sample comparisons, respectively. These analyses were performed for (i) butterfly track direction before capture, (ii) expected and observed vanishing bearing after treatment, (iii) wind direction at capture and release times, and (iv) estimated butterfly heading (i.e. body compass orientation, see below) following release.

For controls, the expected vanishing bearing on release was the flight direction (track) measured prior to capture. For experimental individuals (4 h time shift), on the basis of the sun’s azimuth curve for Barro Colorado Island in the period 10 May to 10 August (see Fig. 4A), the expected vanishing bearing on release was the individual’s original track direction plus 120 °. In 1992, however, the butterfly track direction before capture was not recorded. For all individuals captured in 1992, we used the mean flight direction φ obtained from samples of naturally migrating butterflies in that period (Aphrissa spp., φ=225±5 °, R=0.758, P<0.0001, N=510; P. argante, φ=190±10 °, R=0.751, P<0.0001, N=89; mean ±95 % CIs; Fig. 2) as the basis for calculating expected bearings.

Fig. 4.

The sun’s azimuth at Barro Colorado Island, Panamá, during the period 10 May to 10 August 1993 (open circles; solid line obtained by interpolation), and track direction of naturally migrating Aphrissa spp. (A) and Phoebis argante (B) butterflies over Lake Gatún versus time of day (GMT minus 5 h) (filled squares). Mean flight direction and 95 % confidence intervals for 30 min periods are plotted (see also Table 1). With full compensation for the sun’s movement (and wind speed not exceeding 2 m s−1), the predicted flight direction is the mean flight direction (horizontal broken line). The expected flight direction with compensation based on the mean motion of the sun between 08:00 and 16:00 h (curvilinear dashed line) was estimated as the difference between the sun’s true position and its average position. In both species, the confidence intervals for mean flight direction from each 30 min of data overlap the horizontal line but are very different from the curvilinear one.

Fig. 4.

The sun’s azimuth at Barro Colorado Island, Panamá, during the period 10 May to 10 August 1993 (open circles; solid line obtained by interpolation), and track direction of naturally migrating Aphrissa spp. (A) and Phoebis argante (B) butterflies over Lake Gatún versus time of day (GMT minus 5 h) (filled squares). Mean flight direction and 95 % confidence intervals for 30 min periods are plotted (see also Table 1). With full compensation for the sun’s movement (and wind speed not exceeding 2 m s−1), the predicted flight direction is the mean flight direction (horizontal broken line). The expected flight direction with compensation based on the mean motion of the sun between 08:00 and 16:00 h (curvilinear dashed line) was estimated as the difference between the sun’s true position and its average position. In both species, the confidence intervals for mean flight direction from each 30 min of data overlap the horizontal line but are very different from the curvilinear one.

We used vector analysis to estimate the heading of individual butterflies following release (for methods, see Srygley et al. 1996). Individual wind direction and wind speed were the best estimates based on measurements made at 2–5 min intervals; individual vanishing bearings were entered as their track direction; and 4.5 m s−1 was set as the butterfly air speed for all individuals (A. statira and P. argante, controls and experimentals). To account for the observation that released butterflies fly more slowly than those crossing the lake naturally (Dudley and Srygley, 1994), the set air speed was approximately 10 % less than the mean air speed measured during natural flight (see Srygley et al. 1996).

To analyze the effect of wind direction on the observed vanishing bearings of released butterflies, we regressed the magnitude of the differences between the expected and observed vanishing bearings on the magnitude of the differences between the wind direction and expected vanishing bearings. If wind direction had an effect on the vanishing bearing of released butterflies, the difference between the expected and observed vanishing bearing would increase as the difference between the wind direction and expected vanishing bearing decreased (i.e. as wind direction became more adverse). For A. statira, we tested for effects of treatment and wind direction on the difference between the expected and observed vanishing bearings using a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). For this test, wind direction was divided into three categories (adverse, cross- or favourable) on the basis of the magnitude of the difference between the wind direction and the expected vanishing bearing. We also used ANOVA to test whether the wind speed experienced by control and experimental butterflies differed among the three wind direction categories. Finally, to verify whether wind speed per se had an impact on the flight orientation of released butterflies, we regressed the magnitude of the difference between the expected and observed vanishing bearings on wind speed.

Time of day and flight direction of naturally migrating butterflies

When wind speed did not exceed 2 m s−1, there was no significant effect of time of day t (in h) on the flight direction D (in degrees) of naturally migrating Aphrissa spp. (N=784; D=196+1.64t; CIs, −0.43, 3.47; P=0.099; Fig. 4A). P. argante showed a small but significant counter-clockwise change of direction (N=135; D=258−5.65t; CIs, −10.55, −0.75; P=0.024; Fig. 4B) (Table 1). Since wind speed typically increased with time of day, a large fraction of bearings recorded in the afternoon were excluded from this analysis. When data gathered at all wind speeds were included, both species showed significant changes (Aphrissa spp.: N=1622; D=191+1.67t; CIs, 0.37, 2.97; P=0.012; P. argante: N=279; D=231−3.07t; CIs, −5.96, −0.19; P=0.037) (Table 1). However, these changes were still very small. Even when the effects of wind were not controlled for, very little change in flight direction occurred over the course of a day. The estimated rate of directional change in the total data set (all wind speeds) was no higher than the rate in the data set gathered at low wind speeds. This result corroborates our previous finding showing that naturally migrating A. statira and P. argante compensate to a large extent for cross-wind drift (Srygley et al. 1996).

Table 1.

Flight direction of naturally migrating Aphrissa spp. and Phoebis argante butterflies on Lake Gatún, Panamá (May–July 1992–1996)

Flight direction of naturally migrating Aphrissa spp. and Phoebis argante butterflies on Lake Gatún, Panamá (May–July 1992–1996)
Flight direction of naturally migrating Aphrissa spp. and Phoebis argante butterflies on Lake Gatún, Panamá (May–July 1992–1996)

Capture and release times

Captures took place during the whole period of flight activity with mean capture time between 11:30 and 12:00 h in both species. For both species, the two treatments did not differ in mean capture time (time of day that they were captured) (A. statira, t= 0.67; d.f.= 159, P=0.253; P. argante, t=−0.39; d.f.=29, P=0.350).

The mean release times for A. statira in 1992–1994 were 10:36 h ±2 min (mean ± S.E.M.; range 09:59–11:13 h) for controls and 10:37 h ±2 min (range 10:01–11:15 h) for experimentals, with no significant difference between the two treatments (t=−0.443, d.f.=187, P=0.659). On average, releases (controls and experimentals combined) were performed 10–14 min earlier in 1994 than in 1992–1993 (F2,186=13.695, P<0.0001; 1992 versus 1993, P= 0.828; 1992 versus 1994, P=0.0002; 1993 versus 1994, P<0.0001). This time difference corresponded to a change in the sun’s azimuth of less than 4 °. The mean release times for P. argante (1992–1994) were 10:50 h ±4 min (range 10:16–11:10 h) for controls and 10:50 h ±4 min (range 10:16–11:12 h) for experimentals, again with no significant difference between the two groups (t=−0.008, d.f.=34, P=0.994). On average, releases (controls and experimentals combined) were performed 22–26 min earlier in 1992 than in 1993–1994 (F2,33=5.888, P=0.0065; 1992 versus 1993, P=0.0031; 1992 versus 1994, P=0.0035; 1993 versus 1994, P=0.519). This time difference corresponded to a change in the sun’s azimuth of approximately 5 °. On the basis of the mean release time, the difference between the two treatments was predicted to be approximately 2 ° less for P. argante than for A. statira.

The effect of clock-shifting

The sun’s azimuth curve for the corresponding latitude and time of the year deviates greatly from that plotted for the sun’s mean rate of movement (15 ° h−1) but remains fairly constant across the season (Fig. 4A). Because releases took place between 10:00 and 11:15 h, one would expect the mean orientation of experimental (4 h advance shift) butterflies to be approximately 120 ° clockwise relative to that of controls (experimentals should expect the sun to be near 295 ° rather than near 55 °). The mean vanishing bearing of released controls was expected not to differ from the mean flight orientation recorded prior to capture. This ideal ‘expected’ scenario assumes (i) that the butterflies showed perfect compensation for the sun’s apparent movement (i.e. that the butterflies use a fully time-compensated sun compass), (ii) that the treatment produced the expected degree of shift in the endogenous chronometer of butterflies in the experimental group, (iii) that the wind direction and wind speed experienced by each individual were the same at the times of capture and release (since these butterflies are to a certain extent subject to wind drift; Srygley et al. 1996), (iv) that the treatment had no effect on the butterfly’s ability to compensate for wind drift (i.e. that flight performance was the same before and after treatment), and (v) that there was no effect of capture, handling or captivity on the butterfly’s navigation system (i.e. that preferred migratory bearing was not affected).

The pooled 1992–1994 data for A. statira show that, on release, 4 h advance-shifted butterflies oriented significantly differently from their respective controls (F1,189=43.486, P<0.0001; Fig. 5D). The two groups did not differ in mean flight direction before capture (Fig. 5A). In all three years, the mean vanishing bearing of experimentals was clockwise relative to that of controls. This difference was significant in 1992 (P<0.002) and in 1993 (P<0.0001) but not in 1994 (P=0.256) (Table 2, which also shows across-year comparisons within each group). The 1992–1994 mean vanishing bearings showed a difference of 70 ° between the two groups (Fig. 5D). Estimated butterfly heading, in contrast, showed a difference of 91 ° between the two treatments (P<0.0001; Fig. 5F). This latter value corresponds to 76 % of the predicted 120 ° difference between the two treatments. The observed mean vanishing bearings of released controls and experimentals were both shifted counter-clockwise with respect to the predicted direction. The magnitude of this shift was 74 ° for controls and 125 ° for experimentals (Fig. 5C,D). Similar results were obtained for P. argante. Upon release, 4 h advance-shifted butterflies oriented significantly differently from controls (1992–1994: F1,34=5.239, P= 0.028; Fig. 6D; Table 2). The mean vanishing bearing of experimentals was 45 ° clockwise relative to that of controls. There was also a non-significant difference of 13 ° between the two groups before capture (F1,29=1.923, P=0.176; Fig. 6A). Estimated headings of released butterflies show a difference of 83 ° between the two treatments (Fig. 6F), corresponding to 69 % of the predicted difference (120 °). The difference in mean vanishing bearing between the two groups was significant in 1992 (F1,3=18.629, P= 0.023) and 1994 (F1,21=5.459, P= 0.029). In both years, experimentals oriented clockwise with regard to controls (vanishing bearings in 1993 were uniformly distributed in both groups). As with A. statira, the mean vanishing bearings of released controls and experimentals were both counter-clockwise with respect to the expected means. This difference was 81 ° for controls and 145 ° for experimentals (Fig. 6C,D). The estimated headings precisely illustrate differences in the flight behaviour exhibited by released control and experimental butterflies. Following release, control butterflies typically flew with the prevailing northwest winds, whereas experimentals appeared to adjust their headings so that they compensated for this cross-wind drift. The behaviour of experimentals resulted in a curvilinear track from the point of release to their vanishing point. Therefore, the difference between the two groups based on the estimated headings, rather than that based on the vanishing bearings, seems to be a more accurate representation of the magnitude of the clock-shifting effect (see also Perez et al. 1997).

Table 2.

Mean flight direction of control and experimental Aphrissa statira and Phoebis argante butterflies before capture and when released on Lake Gatún after treatment in the 4 h advance shift experiment

Mean flight direction of control and experimental Aphrissa statira and Phoebis argante butterflies before capture and when released on Lake Gatún after treatment in the 4 h advance shift experiment
Mean flight direction of control and experimental Aphrissa statira and Phoebis argante butterflies before capture and when released on Lake Gatún after treatment in the 4 h advance shift experiment
Fig. 5.

Orientation of control and experimental Aphrissa statira before capture (A) and when released following treatment in the 4 h advance shift experiment (D). Also shown is the wind destination at the moment of capture (B) or release (E), the expected vanishing bearing of released butterflies based on the 4 h clock shift (C), and the estimated butterfly headings at release (F) calculated from their vanishing bearing, air speed, wind speed and wind direction. Data are for 1992–1994. Small triangles represent individual bearings. An arc outside each circle shows the mean direction with 95 % confidence intervals. φ, mean direction; N, number of observations; r, length of mean vector; P, significance (Rayleigh test). Among experimentals, there was one case with no single solution for heading, so it was excluded (F).

Fig. 5.

Orientation of control and experimental Aphrissa statira before capture (A) and when released following treatment in the 4 h advance shift experiment (D). Also shown is the wind destination at the moment of capture (B) or release (E), the expected vanishing bearing of released butterflies based on the 4 h clock shift (C), and the estimated butterfly headings at release (F) calculated from their vanishing bearing, air speed, wind speed and wind direction. Data are for 1992–1994. Small triangles represent individual bearings. An arc outside each circle shows the mean direction with 95 % confidence intervals. φ, mean direction; N, number of observations; r, length of mean vector; P, significance (Rayleigh test). Among experimentals, there was one case with no single solution for heading, so it was excluded (F).

Fig. 6.

Orientation of control and experimental Phoebis argante before capture (A) and when released following treatment in the 4 h advance shift experiment (D). Also shown is the wind destination at the moment of capture (B) or release (E), the expected vanishing bearing of released butterflies based on the 4 h clock shift (C), and the estimated butterfly headings at release (F) calculated from their vanishing bearing, air speed, wind speed and wind direction. Data are for 1992–1994. Symbols as in Fig. 5. The mean vector for wind direction before capture of controls (B) was obtained by doubling the angles (Batschelet, 1981, p. 25). The mean vector is not shown because in this case the software (Kovach, 1994) automatically displays the data as axial (i.e. each angle is plotted twice in symmetrically opposite directions).

Fig. 6.

Orientation of control and experimental Phoebis argante before capture (A) and when released following treatment in the 4 h advance shift experiment (D). Also shown is the wind destination at the moment of capture (B) or release (E), the expected vanishing bearing of released butterflies based on the 4 h clock shift (C), and the estimated butterfly headings at release (F) calculated from their vanishing bearing, air speed, wind speed and wind direction. Data are for 1992–1994. Symbols as in Fig. 5. The mean vector for wind direction before capture of controls (B) was obtained by doubling the angles (Batschelet, 1981, p. 25). The mean vector is not shown because in this case the software (Kovach, 1994) automatically displays the data as axial (i.e. each angle is plotted twice in symmetrically opposite directions).

The effects of wind direction and wind speed

Winds from the northwest (and to a lesser extent from the southeast) prevailed during field work in all three years (Figs 5B,E, 6B,E). Winds were therefore approximately perpendicular to the preferred migratory direction at this time of the year. For A. statira, the combined (controls and experimentals) 1992–1994 mean wind direction (source) during releases was 299±11 ° (mean ±95 % CIs; N=189, P<0.0001) and there was no effect of year (F2,186=3.976, P>0.10). The combined 1992–1994 mean wind speed during releases was 2.3±0.1 m s−1 (mean ± S.E.M.; range 0.1–5.8 m s−1, N=189), but there was significant variation across the years (F2,186=12.341, P<0.0001), with 1994 winds being relatively stronger (1992 versus 1993, P= 0.992; 1992 versus 1994, P= 0.0007; 1993 versus 1994, P<0.0001). This factor may explain the more similar control and experimental mean vectors of released A. statira in 1994 (Table 2) (i.e. stronger winds from the northwest would tend to bring the two mean vectors closer).

For P. argante, the combined 1992–1994 mean wind direction (source) on release was 316±14 ° (N=36, P<0.0001), and the mean wind speed was 3.4±0.2 m s−1 (mean ± S.E.M.; range 0.1–6.0 m s−1). Both species encountered the same wind direction on release (control, F1,112=1.721, P=0.192; experimental, F1,109=0.551, P=0.460), but P. argante experienced higher wind speeds than did A. statira (control, t=−3.76, d.f.=112, P=0.0001; experimental, t=−3.07, d.f.=109, P= 0.001). This factor may explain the smaller difference between mean release vectors for the two treatments in P. argante compared with A. statira (see Table 2). In both species, controls and experimentals experienced the same wind direction (1992–1994: A. statira, F2,187=1.324, P=0.251; P. argante, F1,34=0.026, P=0.873) and wind speed (1992–1994: A. statira, t=− 0.11, d.f.= 187,P= 0.457;P. argante, t=0.48, d.f.=34, P=0.317) on release. For both species, wind direction during release was not significantly different from wind direction during capture (1993–1994: A. statira, control, F1,164=0.022, P=0.882; experimental, F1,154=0.007, P=0.935; P. argante, control, F1,28=3.162, P= 0.086; experimental, F1,30=1.819, P=0.188). In contrast, wind speed during releases was lower than wind speed during captures for A. statira (1993–1994: control, t=3.42, d.f.=164, P<0.0001; experimental, t=2.89, d.f.=154, P=0.002) but higher for P. argante (control, t=−2.45, d.f.= 28, P= 0.010; experimental, t=−1.97, d.f.= 30, P= 0.029). This factor may also have contributed to the smaller difference between the mean release vectors of the treatments in P. argante.

Wind direction had a strong effect on the vanishing bearing of released butterflies. As the wind direction became more favourable (with respect to the expected vanishing bearing following release), the difference between the expected and the observed mean vanishing bearing decreased (Fig. 7). Linear regression for wind direction minus expected vanishing bearing versus expected minus observed vanishing bearing showed a significant effect of wind direction in both treatments and species (A. statira, control, y=−0.50x+128.80, r2=0.17, N=97, P<0.0001; experimental, y=−0.48x+141.08, r2=0.29, N=92, P<0.0001; P. argante, control, y=−0.83x+179.37, r2=0.43, N=17, P<0.0001; experimental, y=−0.70x+148.50, r2=0.24, N=19, P=0.034). For A. statira, two-way ANOVA for the difference between expected and observed vanishing bearing showed a marginally significant effect of treatment (F1,183=3.807, P= 0.053) and a significant effect of wind direction (F2,183=21.513, P<0.0001), but no interaction between these two factors (F2,183=0.28, P= 0.812). In both species, butterflies in the experimental group were mostly released under adverse wind with respect to the predicted bearing following release, while butterflies in the control group were more frequently exposed to crosswinds (Figs 5–7).

Fig. 7.

Mean difference (+ S.E.M.) between absolute values of expected and observed vanishing bearings for Aphrissa statira (A) and Phoebis argante (B) in three 60 ° wind direction categories depending on whether it was favourable, cross or adverse to the expected vanishing bearing of released controls (filled columns) and experimentals (open columns) in the clock-shift experiment. N is given above each column.

Fig. 7.

Mean difference (+ S.E.M.) between absolute values of expected and observed vanishing bearings for Aphrissa statira (A) and Phoebis argante (B) in three 60 ° wind direction categories depending on whether it was favourable, cross or adverse to the expected vanishing bearing of released controls (filled columns) and experimentals (open columns) in the clock-shift experiment. N is given above each column.

For A. statira, ANOVA for wind speed in the three wind direction categories showed no significant effect of treatment (F1,183=3.315, P= 0.070) or wind direction (F2,183=2.127, P=0.122) but a significant interaction between the two factors (F2,183=8.320, P= 0.0003). In addition to being adverse in direction, the winds encountered by most butterflies in the experimental group were also relatively strong (Fig. 8A). Consistent with these observations, the regression of wind speed on expected minus observed vanishing bearing showed a significant effect of wind speed on the vanishing bearing of A. statira experimentals (y=12.05x+81.02, r2=0.08, P=0.005) but not of controls (y=−1.12x+86.48, r2=0.00, P=0.782). There was no significant relationship between the two variables for P. argante (controls, y=−7.80x+111.16, r2=0.05, N=17, P= 0.415; experimentals, y=5.05x+107.04, r2=0.03, N=19, P=0.519). Control and experimental P. argante experienced essentially the same wind speed (Fig. 8B).

Fig. 8.

Mean wind speed (+ S.E.M.) experienced by control (filled columns) and experimental (open columns) Aphrissa statira (A) and Phoebis argante (B) grouped into the wind direction categories described in Fig. 7.

Fig. 8.

Mean wind speed (+ S.E.M.) experienced by control (filled columns) and experimental (open columns) Aphrissa statira (A) and Phoebis argante (B) grouped into the wind direction categories described in Fig. 7.

The effect of handling

Captured A. statira and P. argante that experienced 5 min of captivity prior to release along their original flight trajectory had a vanishing bearing significantly different from the flight direction measured before capture (F1,72=32.866, P<0.0001; Fig. 9). This difference can be attributed largely to a tendency of released butterflies to fly with the prevailing winds. Pairwise t-tests for A. statira showed that the difference between the wind source direction and the butterfly’s track direction following release was significantly larger than before capture (t=−2.783, d.f.=32, P=0.005). Winds from the west-northwest (293±29 °, P= 0.001) with a speed of 2.4±0.2 m s−1 (range 0.1–3.9 m s−1) prevailed during such releases. The mean vanishing bearing of A. statira subjected to this treatment was not significantly different from that of control butterflies used in the 4 h advance-shift experiment (F1,128=1.323, P=0.252).

Fig. 9.

Orientation of Aphrissa statira and Phoebis argante before capture (A) (data for both species pooled) and on release 5 min following capture (B). Wind destination is shown in C. Mean vanishing bearings (and 95 % CIs) of released butterflies for each species are as follows: A. statira, φ=108±45 °, N=33, P=0.048; P. argante, φ=65±34 °, N=4, P=0.019. Other details are as in Fig. 5.

Fig. 9.

Orientation of Aphrissa statira and Phoebis argante before capture (A) (data for both species pooled) and on release 5 min following capture (B). Wind destination is shown in C. Mean vanishing bearings (and 95 % CIs) of released butterflies for each species are as follows: A. statira, φ=108±45 °, N=33, P=0.048; P. argante, φ=65±34 °, N=4, P=0.019. Other details are as in Fig. 5.

Sex ratio, wing length and body mass

The sex ratio among test individuals was moderately biased towards males (A. statira, control, 1.2:1; experimental, 1:1; P. argante, control, 1.4:1; experimental, 2.2:1). There was, however, no significant difference between the control and experimental groups in wing length (A. statira, t=−0.964, d.f.=156, P=0.337; P. argante, t=−0.700, d.f.=27, P=0.490) or in body mass (A. statira, t=−0.170, d.f.= 156, P=0.866; P. argante, t=−1.837, d.f.=27, P=0.077).

Taken together, the flight directions of naturally migrating butterflies and those of our experimentally clock-shifted butterflies support the hypothesis that Aphrissa statira and Phoebis argante use the sun as a compass when migrating. For both species, free-flying butterflies maintained a fairly constant direction throughout the day. Relative to controls, those A. statira and P. argante with their biological clock experimentally shifted changed their heading in a direction consistent with the use of the sun as a compass (i.e. experimentals oriented clockwise relative to controls). However, the fact that the mean flight direction of the released controls was not the same as that measured prior to capture suggests that these butterflies may use the sun as a compass during escape behaviour. Factors that may have contributed to this change in direction are evaluated below. If this is the case, then by extension to migratory behaviour it is likely that these butterflies also use the sun as a compass when migrating. However, in studies of navigation, such as those with pigeons Columba livia, the mean direction of control groups rather than the home direction identifies the reference (Sollwert) for evaluating the effects of clock-shifts (see Wiltschko et al. 1994) to incorporate the effects of handling. Similarly, the direction of released controls, rather than the migratory direction, is presumed to be the reference in the present study with butterflies.

Results for both naturally migrating and experimental butterflies support the hypothesis that the butterflies compensate fully for changes in the sun’s position over the course of the day. The flight direction of naturally migrating Aphrissa spp. did not differ significantly from the mean bearing over the course of a day, while P. argante showed only a slight directional change. For both species, the magnitude of the experimental shift in mean estimated heading (91±35 ° for A. statira and 83±78 ° for P. argante) was not significantly different from the expected change of 120 ° if the butterflies were fully compensating for a shift in the sun’s position from 10:00 to 14:00 h. However, it was also not significantly different from the expected change of 60 ° if the butterflies compensate by time-averaging the sun’s change in position over 4 h.

Because the rate of change in the sun’s azimuth is not constant over the course of the day (Fig. 4A), the use of the sun as a reliable orientation cue requires a variable time-compensation mechanism (e.g. the use of astronomical tables). In this regard, tropical latitudes provide an extreme environment. At temperate latitudes, compensating by time-averaging the sun’s apparent movement over the course of the day and by fully compensating generally give more similar results than at tropical latitudes. Hence, butterflies might opt for time-averaging in temperate latitudes. For Danaus plexippus at a temperate latitude (Kansas, USA), Perez et al. (1997) found that estimated headings of 6 h delayed butterflies were shifted in the appropriate direction. The magnitude of the shift (75 ° relative to controls and 85 ° relative to the natural flight direction) agreed with that expected from the sun’s average rate of azimuthal movement (in which case the predicted shift would be 90 °). However, the predicted shift if the butterflies were released in mid-afternoon and were compensating fully was approximately 120 °. Hence, the effect of the time-shift was consistent with the time-averaging mechanism and fell short of the full-compensation mechanism. The effect of experimentally clock-shifting organisms may be less than predicted for a variety of reasons, including duration of treatment, age, direction of shift, incomplete compensation for the sun’s movement and conflict with compass information provided by other cues (Aschoff, 1978; Wiltschko et al. 1994; Chappell, 1997). In the present study, a reasonable supposition is that treatment duration was less than that required to achieve the desired degree of shift. We opted to keep the treatment duration between 2 and 4 days considering that mortality increased with increasing captivity time. Moreover, if these butterflies in nature undergo continuous migration for only a few days, an extended period of captivity could potentially inhibit behaviour associated with migration. Migration in insects is typically restricted to the premature imaginal phase (Johnson, 1969; but see Spieth et al. 1998).

The butterfly compass may also allow only partial compensation for changes in the sun’s azimuth. Time of day clearly affected the flight direction of these species during migration across Carajás, Brazil (Oliveira, 1990), and that of short-distance migrant pierids in Europe (Baker, 1968a,b, 1969). A sun compass with imperfect time compensation has been observed in crustacean sandhoppers Talitrus saltator (Wallraff, 1981), ants Cataglyphis bicolor (Wehner, 1984), honeybees Apis mellifera (Gould, 1980) and pigeons Columba livia (Schmidt-Koenig et al. 1991a; Wiltschko et al. 1994; Chappell, 1997). Desert ants, for example, underestimate high rates of change in the sun’s azimuth while overestimating low ones (Wehner, 1984).

In the present study, the effects of the prevailing winds explain in great part both (i) the fact that the mean flight direction of released controls was not the same as that measured prior to capture and (ii) the fact that the magnitude of the difference between the two treatments was smaller (but not significantly so) than predicted. The results of the analyses considering the effect of wind on the flight orientation of released A. statira and P. argante imply that the prevailing northwest winds caused both control and experimental mean vectors to shift counter-clockwise. The experimental group was particularly vulnerable to the effects of the wind because these butterflies faced winds that were generally opposing the expected vanishing bearing. Although naturally migrating butterflies adjusted their headings to compensate for wind drift, at least in part (Srygley et al. 1996), captive butterflies may be less motivated or physiologically less capable of compensating for wind drift. For example, in a sample of 13 species, released butterflies had significantly lower body temperatures than those naturally crossing Lake Gatún (Dudley and Srygley, 1994). A. statira and P. argante captured during migration showed body temperatures 7.5±0.7 °C (mean ± S.E.M.; N=10) and 8.5±0.4 °C (N=8), respectively, above ambient temperature (see methods in Srygley and Chai, 1990), a condition not likely to occur in the test individuals prior to release.

As with released control A. statira and P. argante butterflies in the clock-shift experiment, a nearly downwind flight was the most obvious effect of capture and handling in the 5 min captivity experiment. Kanz (1977) reported the same effect in D. plexippus. This behaviour allows the most rapid escape from a site where danger is imminent. It is intriguing, however, that, despite this escape behaviour, experimental butterflies (flying under the same wind conditions as controls) altered their flight orientation and adopted a bearing that was shifted in the direction predicted by the sun compass hypothesis. Apparently, this presumed ‘escape orientation’ is under the control of an endogenous clock. This effect was also observed in sandhoppers T. saltator (Papi and Pardi, 1963).

If the sun compass is to be used all year round, long-distance migrants may be required to compensate not only for the time of day but also for the seasonal variation in the sun’s arc, especially in tropical latitudes. This is difficult to achieve, but some organisms do approach a true solar calendar (e.g. the internal ephemeris function of homing pigeons appears to lag behind real time by several weeks; Schmidt-Koenig et al. 1991b). The butterflies studied here may circumvent this problem by confining their migratory activity to a period when the sun’s course is relatively stable (Figs 1, 4A). In Panamá, major transitions in the sun’s course occur in early September and mid April (i.e. from September to April, the midday sun is due south).

Migrating butterflies have also been observed to maintain a constant direction when solar information was unlikely to be available, either because the sun was directly overhead (Larsen, 1991) or because the sky was completely overcast (Schmidt-Koenig, 1985; Gibo, 1986; Walker and Littell, 1994). This suggests the use of a geomagnetic compass. Although such a mechanism has never been demonstrated in migrant butterflies (Wehner, 1984; Brower, 1996; Dingle, 1996), it has been documented in the migratory moths Noctua pronuba and Agrotis exclamationis (Baker and Mather, 1982; Baker, 1987). It is now well established that animals can use a variety of cues in a flexible manner in order to orient and navigate under changeable ambient conditions (Dingle, 1996; Wehner et al. 1996).

Butterflies observed migrating across the isthmus of Panamá are probably long-distance travellers (Srygley et al. 1996). Butterflies were observed flying in a straight line over the Caribbean Sea beyond visible landmarks off the northern Colombian coast, and the direction of flight was the same as that observed in central Panamá (R. B. Srygley and E. G. Oliveira, unpublished data). To orient reliably, the use of a time-compensated sun compass may be crucial for these neotropical migrants. In addition to being essential to our understanding of the ecology and evolution of migration, research on the orientation mechanisms used by migratory butterflies may contribute to the preservation of these spectacular natural phenomena. Their ability to keep within a flyway in variable topographical and wind conditions requires reliable cues for orienting and navigating (Srygley et al. 1996). The sun provides such a cue, and the butterflies have adapted to compensate for its motion so as to maintain a constant bearing through the day. As habitat loss narrows corridors that are suitable to sustain long-distance migrations, knowledge of the ability of butterflies to orient and navigate is pertinent to mapping migration flyways and ultimately to conserving migrations (Brower and Malcom, 1991).

We thank N. Zangen, R. Aizprua and N. Daguerre for assistance in the field and the staff on Barro Colorado Island for help with field and laboratory equipment. We also thank R. Ranvaud, P. Chai, T. F. Kawasaki, M. C. Singer, L. E. Gilbert, K. Hansen and anonymous reviewers for valuable comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript and D. M. Windsor, M. A. Rankin and T. J. Walker for useful suggestions on the project. We are also grateful to A. Ganzhorn for the Pascal program to treat circular data and to V. Medeiros (INPE, Brazil) for the Fortran program to calculate solar azimuth and elevation. Special thanks from E.G.O. go to W. W. Benson and R. Ranvaud for sharing enthusiasm and knowledge. This study was supported by fellowships and grants from CNPq-Brazil, Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute, Department of Zoology and the Institute of Latin American Studies at the University of Texas at Austin to E.G.O. and a grant from the National Geographic Society to the three co-authors. We thank INRENARE for permission to collect in the Republic of Panamá.

Able
,
K. P.
(
1980
).
Mechanisms of orientation, navigation and homing
.
In Animal Migration, Orientation and Navigation
(ed.
S. A.
Gauthreaux
Jr
), pp.
281
387
.
New York
:
Academic Press
.
Aschoff
,
J.
(
1978
).
Problems of re-entrainment of circadian rhythms: asymmetry effect, dissociation and partition
.
In Environmental Endocrinology
(ed.
I.
Assenmacher
and
D. S.
Farner
), pp.
185
195
.
Berlin
:
Springer-Verlag
.
Baker
,
R. R.
(
1968a
).
Sun orientation during migration in some British butterflies
.
Proc. R. ent. Soc. Lond., Ser. A
143
,
89
95
.
Baker
,
R. R.
(
1968b
).
A possible method of evolution of the migratory habit in butterflies
.
Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B
253
,
309
341
.
Baker
,
R. R.
(
1969
).
Evolution of the migratory habit in butterflies
.
J. Anim. Ecol
.
38
,
703
746
.
Baker
,
R. R.
(
1984
).
The dilemma: when and how to go or stay
.
In The Biology of Butterflies
(ed.
R. I.
Vane-Wright
and
P. R.
Ackery
), pp.
279
296
.
New York
:
Academic Press
.
Baker
,
R. R.
(
1987
).
Integrated use of moon and magnetic compasses by the heart-and-dart moth, Agrotis exclamationis
.
Anim. Behav
.
35
,
94
101
.
Baker
,
R. R.
and
Mather
,
J. G.
(
1982
).
Magnetic compass sense in the large yellow underwing moth, Noctua pronuba L
.
Anim. Behav
.
30
,
543
548
.
Batschelet
,
E.
(
1981
).
Circular Statistics in Biology
.
New York
:
Academic Press
.
Berthold
,
P.
(
1993
).
Bird Migration: A General Survey
.
Oxford University Press
.
Brower
,
L. P.
(
1995
).
Understanding and misunderstanding the migration of the monarch butterfly (Nymphalidae) in North America
.
J. Lepidopt. Soc
.
49
,
304
385
.
Brower
,
L. P.
(
1996
).
Monarch butterfly orientation: missing pieces of a magnificent puzzle
.
J. exp. Biol
.
199
,
93
103
.
Brower
,
L. P.
and
Malcom
,
S. B.
(
1991
).
Animal migrations: endangered phenomena
.
Am. Zool
.
31
,
265
276
.
Chappell
,
J.
(
1997
).
An analysis of clock-shift experiments: is scatter increased and deflection reduced in clock-shifted homing pigeons?
J. exp. Biol
.
200
,
2269
2277
.
Dingle
,
H.
(
1996
).
Migration: The Biology of Life on the Move
.
New York
:
Oxford University Press
.
Dudley
,
R.
and
Srygley
,
R. B.
(
1994
).
Flight physiology of neotropical butterflies: allometry of airspeeds during natural free flight
.
J. exp. Biol
.
191
,
125
139
.
Emlen
,
S. T.
(
1975
).
Migration: orientation and navigation
.
In Avian Biology V
(ed.
D. S.
Farner
and
J. R.
King
), pp.
129
219
.
New York
:
Academic Press
.
Gibo
,
D.
(
1986
).
Flight strategies of migrating monarch butterflies (Danaus plexippus L.) in southern Ontario
.
In Insect Flight, Dispersal and Migration
(ed.
W.
Danthanarayana
), pp.
172
184
.
Berlin
:
Springer-Verlag
.
Gould
,
J. L.
(
1980
).
Sun compensation by bees
.
Science
207
,
545
547
.
Johnson
,
C. G.
(
1969
).
Migration and Dispersal of Insects by Flight
.
London
:
Methuen
.
Kanz
,
J. E.
(
1977
).
The orientation of migrant and non-migrant monarch butterflies Danaus plexippus L
.
Psyche
84
,
120
141
.
Kovach
,
W. L.
(
1994
).
Oriana for Windows, Version 1.0
.
Wales, UK
:
Kovach Computer Services
.
Larsen
,
T. B.
(
1991
).
The Butterflies of Kenya and their Natural History
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.
Nielsen
,
E. T.
(
1961
).
On the habits of the migratory butterfly Ascia monuste L
.
Biol. Medd. Dan. Vid. Selsk
.
23
,
1
81
.
Oliveira
,
E. G.
(
1990
).
Orientação de vôo de lepidópteros migratórios na região de Carajás, Pará
.
MS thesis, Instituto de Biologia, Universidade Estadual de Campinas
,
São Paulo, Brazil
.
230pp
.
Papi
,
F.
and
Pardi
,
L.
(
1963
).
On the lunar orientation of sandhoppers
.
Biol. Bull. mar. biol. Lab., Woods Hole
124
,
97
105
.
Peddie
,
N. W.
(
1993
).
The Magnetic Field of the Earth, 1990 Declination Chart
.
Reston, VA
:
US Department of Interior, US Geological Survey
.
Perez
,
S. M.
,
Taylor
,
O. R.
and
Jander
,
R.
(
1997
).
A sun compass in monarch butterflies
.
Nature
387
,
29
.
Santschi
,
F.
(
1911
).
Observations et remarques critiques sur le mécanisme de l’orientation chez les fourmis
.
Rev. suisse Zool
.
19
,
305
338
.
Schmidt-koenig
,
K.
(
1985
).
Migration strategies of monarch butterflies. In Migration: Mechanisms and Adaptive Significance
(ed.
M. A.
Rankin
).
Contrib. mar. Sci. (Suppl
.)
27
,
786
798
.
Schmidt-koenig
,
K.
,
Ganzhorn
,
J. U.
and
Ranvaud
,
R.
(
1991a
).
The sun compass
.
In Orientation in Birds
(ed.
P.
Berthold
), pp.
1
15
.
Berlin
:
Birkhauser Verlag
.
Schmidt-koenig
,
K.
,
Ranvaud
,
R.
,
Ganzhorn
,
J. U.
and
Gasparotto
,
O. C.
(
1991b
).
Retardation of homing pigeons’ ephemerides?
Naturwissenschaften
78
,
330
333
.
Spieth
,
H. R.
,
Ralf-gerd
,
C.
and
Dorka
,
M.
(
1998
).
Flight directions in the migratory butterfly Pieris brassicae: results from semi-natural experiments
.
Ethology
104
,
339
352
.
Srygley
,
R. B.
and
Chai
,
P.
(
1990
).
Predation and the elevation of thoracic temperature in brightly colored neotropical butterflies
.
Am. Nat
.
135
,
766
787
.
Srygley
,
R. B.
,
Oliveira
,
E. G.
and
Dudley
,
R.
(
1996
).
Wind drift compensation, flyways and conservation of diurnal, migrant Neotropical Lepidoptera
.
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
263
,
1351
1357
.
Von frisch
,
K.
(
1967
).
The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees
.
Cambridge, MA
:
Harvard University Press
.
Walker
,
T. J.
(
1985
).
Butterfly migration in the boundary layer
.
In Migration: Mechanisms and Adaptive Significance
(ed.
M. A.
Rankin
).
Contrib. mar. Sci
. (Suppl.)
27
,
704
723
.
Walker
,
T. J.
and
Littell
,
R. C.
(
1994
).
Orientation of fall-migrating butterflies in north peninsular Florida and in source areas
.
Ethology
98
,
60
84
.
Walker
,
T. J.
and
Riordan
,
A. J.
(
1981
).
Butterfly migration: are synoptic-scale wind systems important?
Ecol. Ent
.
6
,
433
440
.
Wallraff
,
H. G.
(
1981
).
Clock-controlled orientation in space
.
In Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology 4
(ed.
J.
Aschoff
), pp.
299
309
.
New York
:
Plenum Press
.
Waterman
,
T. H.
(
1989
).
Animal Navigation
.
New York
:
Scientific American Library
.
Wehner
,
R.
(
1984
).
Astronavigation in insects
.
A. Rev. Ent
.
29
,
277
298
.
Wehner
,
R.
,
Lehrer
,
M.
and
Harvey
,
W. R.
(
1996
).
(eds) Navigation: Migration and Homing
.
J. exp. Biol
.
199
,
1
261
.
Williams
,
C. B.
(
1930
).
The Migration of Butterflies
.
Edinburgh
:
Oliver & Boyd
.
Wiltschko
,
R.
,
Kumpfmüller
,
R.
,
Muth
,
R.
and
Wiltschko
,
W.
(
1994
).
Pigeon homing: the effect of a clock-shift is often smaller than predicted
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol
.
35
,
63
73
.
Windsor
,
D. M.
(
1990
).
Climate and Moisture Variability in a Tropical Forest: Long-Term Records From Barro Colorado Island, Panamá. Smithsonian Contributions to the Earth Sciences
29
.
Washington
:
Smithsonian Institution Press
.