The coordination of ventilatory and locomotor rhythms has been documented in many birds and mammals. It has been suggested that the physiological significance of such coordination is a reduction in the cost of ventilation which confers an energetic advantage to the animal. We tested this hypothesis by measuring the external work required to ventilate birds mechanically during simulated flight. Patterns of wing motion and breathing were produced in which the relationship between wing motion and breathing was in phase and out of phase with the relationship seen during normal flight. Differences between the energetic costs of in-phase versus out-of-phase synchronization were particularly large (26 %) in instances where locomotion and respiration frequency were synchronized at one breath per wingbeat. The saving (9 %) obtained from in-phase versus out-of-phase coordination at the 3:1 coordination ratio seen normally in free-flying Canada geese was smaller but still supported the hypothesis that there is a significant net saving obtained from reducing the mechanical interference between locomotion and ventilation by locomotor–respiratory coupling.

During galloping, quadrupeds from the gerbil to the rhinoceros take one breath per stride (Bramble and Carrier, 1983; Young et al. 1992), the breath always occurring at the same part of the locomotor cycle. Similarly, in some birds during flight, inspiration is synchronized with the upstroke of the wings (Tomlinson, 1957; Hart and Roy, 1966; Butler et al. 1977; Berger et al. 1970). Such coordination could reduce the work of breathing in two ways: (i) by reducing the mechanical interference between locomotion and respiration (both require movements of the thorax and abdomen); (ii) by transferring the work of producing ventilation, at least in part, from the normal respiratory muscles to the movements produced by locomotion. For example, by acting as a bellows, the action of bending and extending the trunk during galloping in the horse may completely relieve the work of the diaphragm (Young et al. 1992). Furthermore, inertial oscillations of the viscera in hopping wallabies (Baudinette et al. 1987) and trotting dogs (Bramble and Jenkins, 1993) may act as a piston mechanism (Alexander, 1993) assisting in producing respiratory airflow. It should be noted, however, that such transfer of work will only reduce the net work of breathing if work is transferred to muscles that are more efficient.

The hypothesis that the work of breathing is reduced by reducing the mechanical interference between locomotion and respiration appears reasonable, especially for cases where the locomotor and respiratory rhythms show 1:1 coordination. Coordination ratios other than 1:1, however, are frequently observed in bipeds and quadrupeds during overground locomotion (Bramble and Carrier, 1983) and in birds during flight (Berger et al. 1970; Butler and Woakes, 1980; Funk et al. 1993). For example, many birds exhibit 3:1 coordination between wingbeats and respiration (three wingbeats to each breath) (Butler and Woakes, 1980; Funk et al. 1993). In this case, there are two downstrokes and one upstroke associated with inspiration and two upstrokes and one downstroke associated with expiration. The mechanical and energetic consequences of this coupling are not immediately obvious.

Despite its appeal, the hypothesis that locomotor–respiratory coupling reduces the work of breathing has not been tested. In no instance has an energy saving associated with locomotor–respiratory coordination been measured, largely because of the difficulty of uncoupling locomotion and respiration in freely behaving animals. Recently, we developed a technique for artificially flapping the wings of decerebrate, paralyzed Canada geese (Branta canadensis) which produced locomotor–respiratory coordination similar to that seen in conscious, flying animals (Funk et al. 1992a). In the present study, we employed this technique together with artificial ventilation of animals at wingbeat to ventilation ratios of 1:1 and 3:1 with the relationship between respiration and wingbeat both in phase and out of phase with that seen in free flight. This should mimic the best- and worst-case conditions, respectively, if the above hypothesis is correct. By measuring the effects of altering the phase relationship on the mechanical work required to ventilate the lungs, we were able to provide a crude test of this hypothesis.

Experiments were performed on three Canada geese (Branta canadensis Alison, 3.4±0.2 kg body mass; mean ± S.E.M.). The surgical procedures used were identical to those described previously (Funk et al. 1992a). Briefly, surgery was performed under halothane/nitrous oxide anesthesia. The trachea was cannulated to permit artificial ventilation and to monitor ventilation via pneumotachography. Body temperature was monitored via a thermistor placed down the esophagus and maintained at 41 °C by the use of a heat lamp and a copper, water-perfused heat exchanger inserted through the anus into the intestine.

The birds were supported in a sling, their heads were placed in a stereotaxic head holder, and the animals were decerebrated. The body was then suspended and stabilized from the dorsal surface by two bone clamps, one attached to the pelvic girdle and the other to the dorsal vertebral spines just caudal to the brachial plexus. This was required to prevent compression of the thorax onto the sling during the downstroke of the wing flapper. Each wing was fastened to a bar that held it in the approximate position seen during free flight (Funk et al. 1992a). Each wing bar was attached to a support rod that overlay an eccentric adjustable cam. The anchored end of the support rod acted as a pivot. A 186 W direct-current motor rotated the cam and generated the passive wing movements. Wing flapping frequency (fW) was adjusted via a rheostat attached to the motor. Wingbeat amplitude was adjusted to approximate the amplitude observed during free flight at 50–60 km h−1 (Funk et al. 1992a) by adjusting the diameter of the eccentric cam.

In the present study, we also electrically stimulated the pectoralis major muscles, since they have the largest impact on the mechanics of the thorax. They form the largest part of the wing musculature (unlike any other tetrapod muscle, they may constitute up to 35 % of body mass) and are the main muscles driving the power stroke (the downstroke) of the wing cycle. Although precise activation and coordination of many muscle groups is required to produce wing motion during free flight, the energetic consequences of activating additional wing musculature were not examined.

The electrical stimulus was applied in a manner that resembled the activity pattern of the pectoralis electromyogram (EMG) activity in free-flying birds (Dial et al. 1988). The stimulus parameters used were 10–15 V, 0.3–0.6 ms and 50 Hz, and the stimulus was applied between 0.90 and 0.40 of the wing cycle period, where 0.0 represents the top of the upstroke, 0.45 represents the bottom of the downstroke and 1.00 represents completion of the cycle. Thus, the stimulus was applied from just before the top of the upstroke until just before the completion of the downstroke, closely resembling the activity pattern of pectoralis EMG activity in free-flying pigeons Columba livia (Dial et al. 1988). The electrodes were sewn into the muscle and these stimulus parameters were supramaximal, suggesting that the entire muscle was recruited. Following surgery, anesthesia was discontinued and animals were paralyzed using gallamine triethiodide and artificially ventilated using a positive pressure pump (Harvard Apparatus dual-phase respirator, model 665).

We used this technique while mechanically ventilating the lungs of the geese at rest and during artificial wing flapping at 1:1 and 3:1 ratios of wing flapping frequency (fW) to ventilation frequency (fV). Ventilation frequencies and volumes were initially set to equal the respiratory frequencies and volumes recorded in intact animals at rest and were appropriate for maintaining blood gases at constant levels (Funk et al. 1989). For 3:1 coordination, we selected a fW (0.60–0.70 Hz) that was three times greater than resting respiratory frequency (fV=0.20–0.23 Hz). We then increased fV to match fW to produce 1:1 coordination. This was performed where: (i) the phase relationship between respiration and wingbeat frequency observed in free-flying birds (Tomlinson, 1957; Hart and Roy, 1966; Butler et al. 1977; Berger et al. 1970; Butler and Woakes, 1980; Funk et al. 1993) was reproduced (in-phase activity; for 3:1 entrainment this was two downstrokes and one upstroke associated with each inspiration and two upstrokes and one downstroke associated with each expiration; for 1:1 entrainment, the upstroke was produced during inspiration and the downstroke during expiration); (ii) the phase relationship between wingbeat and respiration frequency was 180 ° out of phase with that observed during free flight.

Under all conditions, we measured the mechanical work of ventilation (Otis, 1964). Pressure–volume loops, constructed from tracheal pressure (monitored using a strain-gauge pressure transducer attached to the tracheal cannula) and tidal volume (monitored via pneumotachography) data were used to calculate the total, resistive and elastic work required to produce each breath using the method described by Otis (1964). Differences in calculated work levels were compared for the different conditions using an analysis of variance (ANOVA), and Bonferroni simultaneous confidence intervals for multiple comparisons (RS/PROBE, BBN Laboratories) were used to test for differences between means (P<0.05 was assumed to be significant).

With the wing flapping device turned off, the work required to ventilate the lungs of these birds at 12–14 breaths min−1 (equivalent to the ventilatory rate used at a 3:1 ratio) was 333×10−6±83×10−6 J breath−1, and this increased to 448×10−6±120×10−6 J breath−1 when the ventilation frequency was increased to 36–42 min−1 (equivalent to the ventilatory rate at a 1:1 ratio) (Table 1). This was due to a 1.23-fold increase (not significant) in the work required to overcome elastic forces and a 1.77-fold increase in the work required to overcome resistive forces. These values are not surprising given that tidal volume remained constant while only breathing frequency was increased.

Table 1.

Cost of ventilation at rest and during simulated flight

Cost of ventilation at rest and during simulated flight
Cost of ventilation at rest and during simulated flight

Fig. 1A shows examples of the pressure, flow and volume traces obtained during 1:1 coordination (synchronization) of wingbeat and respiration, both when the upstroke was coincident with inspiration, as occurs in birds which exhibit this ratio in nature (‘in phase’, Table 1) (Tomlinson, 1957; Hart and Roy, 1966; Butler et al. 1977; Berger et al. 1970) and when the phase relationship was shifted by 180 ° so that inspiration occurred during the downstroke (‘out of phase’, Table 1). Fig. 1B,C shows the pressure–volume loops associated with the traces illustrated in Fig. 1A. Note the much larger pressure excursions associated with artificial ventilation at a constant tidal volume when inspiration occurs during the downstroke (out of phase, Fig. 1C) than when it occurs during the upstroke (in phase, Fig. 1B). The total mechanical work required to ventilate the lungs decreased significantly (9 %) from control levels (no flapping, Table 1) with the onset of in-phase wing flapping. When the phase relationship was shifted by 180 ° so that inspiration occurred during the downstroke (‘out of phase’, Table 1), the cost of ventilating the lungs increased by 17 % relative to control values. Thus, the reduction in the cost of ventilation during ‘in-phase’ flapping relative to ‘out-of-phase’ flapping was 26 % (Table 1). The difference between the cost of ventilation during ‘in-phase’ versus ‘out-of-phase’ flapping was primarily due to a 54 % decrease in the work required to overcome flow resistive forces (resistive work, Table 1) between these two conditions.

Fig. 1.

(A) Volume, airflow and intrapulmonary pressure traces from an individual Canada goose during artificial ventilation. Ventilation was coupled 1:1 with artificial wing flapping such that inspiration occurred during the upstroke (in phase) or during the downstroke (out of phase). The upper horizontal bar below the traces indicates when the wings were rising (upstroke, black band) and falling (downstroke, white band). The black band in the lower horizontal bar indicates when electrical stimulation of the pectoralis muscles occurred. (B,C) Pressure–volume relationships obtained from the traces shown in A. Measurements of the work required to overcome elastic and resistive forces to produce each breath were obtained from these relationships as described in the text.

Fig. 1.

(A) Volume, airflow and intrapulmonary pressure traces from an individual Canada goose during artificial ventilation. Ventilation was coupled 1:1 with artificial wing flapping such that inspiration occurred during the upstroke (in phase) or during the downstroke (out of phase). The upper horizontal bar below the traces indicates when the wings were rising (upstroke, black band) and falling (downstroke, white band). The black band in the lower horizontal bar indicates when electrical stimulation of the pectoralis muscles occurred. (B,C) Pressure–volume relationships obtained from the traces shown in A. Measurements of the work required to overcome elastic and resistive forces to produce each breath were obtained from these relationships as described in the text.

Wingbeat and respiration frequency are primarily coupled at a 3:1 ratio during free flight in Canada geese, with inspiration beginning at the top of the upstroke and ending at the bottom of the downstroke of the following cycle (i.e. two downstrokes and one upstroke for each inspiratory event) (Funk et al. 1993). Fig. 2A shows examples of the pressure, flow and volume traces obtained during 3:1 coordination of wingbeat and respiration, both when synchronization occurred in phase with the relationship seen in nature (Tomlinson, 1957; Hart and Roy, 1966; Butler et al. 1977; Berger et al. 1970) and when the phase relationship was shifted by 180 ° so that two upstrokes and one downstroke occurred during inspiration (‘out of phase’). Fig. 2B,C shows the pressure–volume loops associated with the traces illustrated in Fig. 2A. Again, note the much larger pressure excursions associated with artificial ventilation at a constant tidal volume when the inspiration is produced out of phase than when it is produced in phase. Under these simulated ‘in-phase’ conditions, the total mechanical work required to ventilate the lungs was not significantly greater than that required to ventilate the lungs under control conditions (Table 1). When the phase relationship between wingbeat and respiratory movements was shifted to 180 ° out of phase with the relationship seen during free flight, the mechanical cost of ventilating the lungs increased significantly (17 %) relative to that required to ventilate the lungs under control conditions. Under these conditions, both flow resistive work and the work required to overcome elastic recoil forces (Table 1) increased, although these increases were not significant. The net savings of ‘in-phase’ versus ‘out-of-phase’ coordination were much less for 3:1 than for 1:1 coordination (9 % versus 26 %). While the total work associated with in-phase flapping for 3:1 coordination was not significantly greater than that associated with ‘no flapping’ conditions, the total work associated with out-of-phase coordination was significantly elevated above the latter value.

Fig. 2.

(A) Volume, airflow and intrapulmonary pressure traces from an individual Canada goose during artificial ventilation. Ventilation was coupled 3:1 with artificial wing flapping such that two downstrokes and one upstroke occurred during inspiration (in phase) or two upstrokes and one downstroke occurred during inspiration (out of phase). The upper horizontal bar below the traces indicates when the wings were rising (upstroke, black band) and falling (downstroke, white band). The black band in the lower horizontal bar indicates when electrical stimulation of the pectoralis muscles occurred. (B,C) Pressure–volume relationships obtained from the traces shown in A. Measurements of the work required to overcome elastic and resistive forces to produce each breath were obtained from these relationships as described in the text.

Fig. 2.

(A) Volume, airflow and intrapulmonary pressure traces from an individual Canada goose during artificial ventilation. Ventilation was coupled 3:1 with artificial wing flapping such that two downstrokes and one upstroke occurred during inspiration (in phase) or two upstrokes and one downstroke occurred during inspiration (out of phase). The upper horizontal bar below the traces indicates when the wings were rising (upstroke, black band) and falling (downstroke, white band). The black band in the lower horizontal bar indicates when electrical stimulation of the pectoralis muscles occurred. (B,C) Pressure–volume relationships obtained from the traces shown in A. Measurements of the work required to overcome elastic and resistive forces to produce each breath were obtained from these relationships as described in the text.

The measurements obtained in the present study provide only an indication of the mechanical work required to artificially ventilate the lungs of the birds under our experimental conditions, as opposed to the biological work required for the bird to ventilate its lungs under natural conditions. We believe that these values will underestimate both the total costs of ventilation and the energetic benefits of natural locomotor–respiratory coordination. Resting ventilation was produced in birds with reduced postural tone and hence a relaxed thorax and abdomen, and the artificial activation of the flight muscles during wing flapping was confined to the pectoralis muscles alone. During free flight, postural tone would be high, and precise activation and coordination of many muscle groups is required to produce wing motion. Activation of other muscles is likely to increase chest stiffness and increase elastic work, which should further increase the difference between in-phase and out-of-phase flapping. Furthermore, the wingbeat and ventilatory frequencies used in the present study (fW=0.6–0.7 Hz; fV=0.2–0.7 Hz) were much lower than those recorded in free flight (fW=3.9–4.5 Hz; fV=1.3–1.5 Hz; Funk et al. 1993) owing to the technical difficulties of flapping the wings at such high frequencies. Our use of low values for fW and fV would probably reduce the differences in work between in-phase and out-of-phase coordination since the mechanical costs of ventilation increase with increasing fV (Table 1).

Despite these likely underestimates, there is a significant saving between in-phase and out-of-phase coordination of wingbeat and ventilation frequency that supports the hypothesis that synchronization of locomotion and respiration serves to reduce the cost of breathing in flying birds. This saving was larger for 1:1 coordination than for 3:1 coordination.

Although 1:1 coordination of wingbeat and respiration frequency in Canada geese has only been observed under laboratory conditions where ‘flight’ was induced by electrical stimulation of specific brainstem locomotor command centers (Funk et al. 1992b; Valenzuela et al. 1990), 1:1 coordination does occur during free flight in pigeons and crows (Tomlinson, 1957; Hart and Roy, 1966; Butler et al. 1977; Berger et al. 1970). Our data suggest that the energetic savings from locomotor–respiratory coupling may be greatest in these species.

Wingbeat and respiration frequencies are coupled primarily at a 3:1 ratio during free flight in Canada geese, with inspiration beginning at the top of the upstroke and ending at the bottom of the downstroke of the following cycle (i.e. there are two downstrokes and one upstroke for each inspiratory event) (Funk et al. 1993). Although 3:1 coordination of fW and fV is the common pattern during free flight in Canada geese, this relationship is not fixed. Brief transitions from 3:1 to 2:1 coordination are occasionally observed (Funk et al. 1993). 3:1 coordination is also the preferred pattern in Barnacle geese (Branta leucopsis), although patterns of 2:1 and 4:1 coordination have been observed (Butler and Woakes, 1980). Under the simulated ‘in-phase’ conditions of the present study, the total mechanical work required to ventilate the lungs in a 3:1 ratio was not significantly greater than that required to ventilate the lungs under control conditions (Table 1). When the phase relationship between wingbeat and respiratory movements was shifted by 180 ° to be out-of-phase relative to the relationship seen during free flight (‘out of phase’ then representing two upstrokes and one downstroke for each inspiratory event), the mechanical cost of ventilating the lungs increased significantly (17 %) relative to that required to ventilate the lungs under control conditions. Under these conditions, both flow resistive work and the work required to overcome elastic recoil forces (elastic work, Table 1) showed a tendency to increase, although this was not significant. The net saving from ‘in-phase’ versus ‘out-of-phase’ coordination, was much less for 3:1 than for 1:1 coordination (9 % versus 26 %). In both instances, the increase in total work associated with out-of-phase breathing was due primarily to muscle activation since differences in work associated with passive flapping with no electrical stimulation were only 1 % and 7 % for 3:1 and 1:1 coordination, respectively (results not shown).

Thus, the results from the 3:1 coordination experiments were also consistent with the hypothesis that coordination reduces ventilatory costs. Since synchronizing inspiration to the upstroke of the wings was energetically favorable for 1:1 coordination, we anticipated that the pattern of coupling which maximized the coincidence of upstroke and inspiration during 3:1 coupling (two upstrokes and one downstroke) would also be energetically favorable. This was not the case. The ‘in-phase’ pattern of two downstrokes and one upstroke used by geese during free flight (Butler and Woakes, 1980; Funk et al. 1993) was less expensive and suggests that the benefits of such coordination are derived more from a reduction in mechanical interference than from a transfer of the work of breathing from the respiratory muscles to locomotor activity. D. F. Boggs, F. A. Jenkins and K. P. Dial (in preparation) have shown that inspiratory flow in black-billed magpies (Pica pica) is impeded by the downstroke but assisted by the upstroke (and vice versa for expiratory flow), which more strongly supports this conclusion.

In summary, these results suggest that locomotor– respiratory coordination significantly reduces the mechanical costs associated with breathing, especially under conditions in which the coupling ratio is 1:1. Reductions in biological work are predicted to be larger than the reductions in mechanical work reported here.

We wish to thank J. Nicol, S. Bharma and D. Sheldon for their assistance with data analysis. We would also like to thank Dona Boggs for insightful discussions. This study was supported by the NSERC of Canada.

Alexander
,
R. McN
. (
1993
).
Breathing while trotting
.
Science
262
,
196
197
.
Baudinette
,
R. V.
,
Gannon
,
B. J.
,
Runciman
,
W. B.
,
Wells
,
S.
and
Love
,
J. B.
(
1987
).
Do cardiorespiratory frequencies show entrainment with hopping in the tammar wallaby?
J. exp. Biol
.
129
,
251
263
.
Berger
,
M.
,
Roy
,
O. Z.
and
Hart
,
J. S.
(
1970
).
The co-ordination between respiration and wing beats in birds
.
Z. vergl. Physiol.
66
,
190
200
.
Bramble
,
D. M.
and
Carrier
,
D. R.
(
1983
).
Running and breathing in mammals
.
Science
219
,
251
256
.
Bramble
,
D. M.
and
Jenkins
,
F. A.
, Jr
(
1993
).
Mammalian locomotor–respiratory integration: implications for diaphragmatic and pulmonary design
.
Science
262
,
235
240
.
Butler
,
P. J.
,
West
,
N. H.
and
Jones
,
D. R.
(
1977
).
Respiratory and cardiovascular responses of the pigeon to sustained level flight in a wind tunnel
.
J. exp. Biol.
71
,
7
26
.
Butler
,
P. J.
and
Woakes
,
A. J.
(
1980
).
Heart rate, respiratory frequency and wing beat frequency of free flying barnacle geese Branta leucopsis
.
J. exp. Biol.
85
,
213
226
.
Dial
,
K. P.
,
Kaplan
,
S. R.
and
Goslow
,
G. E.
, Jr
(
1988
).
Afunctional analysis of the primary upstroke and downstroke muscles in the domestic pigeon (Columba livia) during flight
.
J. exp. Biol.
134
,
1
16
.
Funk
,
G. D.
,
Milsom
,
W. K.
,
Sholomenko
,
G. N.
and
Steeves
,
J. D.
(
1989
).
Role of the telencephalon in the synchronization of locomotor and respiratory frequencies during walking in Canada geese
.
J. exp. Biol.
145
,
283
301
.
Funk
,
G. D.
,
Milsom
,
W. K.
and
Steeves
,
J. D.
(
1992a
).
Coordination of wingbeat and respiration in the Canada goose. I. Passive wing flapping
.
J. appl. Physiol.
73
,
1014
1024
.
Funk
,
G. D.
,
Sholomenko
,
G. N.
,
Valenzuela
,
I. J.
,
Steeves
,
J. D.
and
Milsom
,
W. K.
(
1993
).
Coordination of wingbeat frequency and respiration in Canada geese during free flight
.
J. exp. Biol.
175
,
317
323
.
Funk
,
G. D.
,
Steeves
,
J. D.
and
Milsom
,
W. K.
(
1992b
).
Coordination of wingbeat and respiration in the Canada goose. II. ‘Fictive flight’
.
J. appl. Physiol.
73
,
1025
1033
.
Hart
,
J. S.
and
Roy
,
O. Z.
(
1966
).
Respiratory and cardiac responses to flight in pigeons
.
Physiol. Zool.
39
,
291
306
.
Otis
,
A. B.
(
1964
).
The work of breathing
. In
Handbook of Physiology, section 3, vol. 1, Respiration
(ed.
W. O.
Fenn
and
H.
Rahn
), pp.
463
476
. Washington, DC: American Physiological Society.
Tomlinson
,
J. T.
(
1957
).
Pigeon wing beats synchronized with breathing
.
Condor
59
,
401
.
Valenzuela
,
J. I.
,
Hasan
,
S. J.
and
Steeves
,
J. D.
(
1990
).
Stimulation of the brainstem reticular formation evokes locomotor activity in embryonic chicken (in ovo)
.
Devl Brain Res
.
56
,
13
18
.
Young
,
I. S.
,
Warren
,
R. D.
and
Altringham
,
J. D.
(
1992
).
Some properties of the mammalian locomotory and respiratory systems in relation to body mass
.
J. exp. Biol.
164
,
283
294
.