The bobolink (Dolichonyx oryzivorus) uses the earth’s magnetic field as one source of directional information for its migratory orientation. However, the location and structure of the magnetoreceptors that transduce the magnetic information to the nervous system are unknown. Because treatment with a strong magnetic pulse results in a change in the direction of orientation, one of the receptors is thought to involve a magnetizable material such as magnetite. The effects of the magnetizing treatment can be abolished (i.e. the bird returns to its original orientation) by blocking the ophthalmic branch of the trigeminal nerve, but the ability of the bird to select and maintain a direction is not affected. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that a magnetizable material such as magnetite is part of the magnetoreceptors that are associated with the ophthalmic nerve.

The ability of animals to detect and utilize directional information from the ambient magnetic field has received increasing documentation. Much of the work in this field has been focused on the navigational abilities of homing and migratory birds (Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1988; R. Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1995). Of the various magnetoreceptor mechanisms that have been proposed for terrestrial animals, two (magnetite and photopigments) have received the greatest amount of attention (Beason and Semm, 1994). Experimental evidence supports the use of a wavelength-sensitive transducer (Wiltschko et al. 1993; W. Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1995) and a magnetizable material (Wiltschko et al. 1994; W. Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1995; Beason et al. 1995; R. C. Beason, R. Wiltschko and W. Wiltschko, in preparation) by birds for detecting the magnetic field. Two, apparently independent, magnetoreceptors have also been postulated for an amphibian, the eastern red-spotted newt (Notophthalmus viridescens), with at least one receptor being wavelength-sensitive (Phillips, 1986; Phillips and Borland, 1994).

It has been proposed that the two avian magnetoreceptor systems may serve different functions, with the wavelength-sensitive receptors serving the magnetic compass and the magnetite-based receptors serving the map system (Semm and Beason, 1990). Two species of migratory birds (the Australian silvereye Zosterops lateralis and the European robin Erithacus rubecula) are disoriented when they are provided with only red illumination, but orient correctly under the other wavelengths tested (Wiltschko et al. 1993; W. Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1995). These responses are consistent with the explanation that the avian compass is wavelength-dependent and presumably involves photopigments. The results from experiments on magnetizing migratory birds (Wiltschko et al. 1994; W. Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1995; Beason et al. 1995) are less clear and could be interpreted to mean that either magnetic compass or magnetic ‘map’ receptors (or both) are based on a magnetic material such as magnetite. However, the data from magnetized homing pigeons clearly indicate that, although the treatment affected the birds’ vanishing directions, it was not a magnetic compass that was affected (Wiltschko and Beason, 1991; R. C. Beason, R. Wiltschko and W. Wiltschko, in preparation). Neither can we rule out the possibility that these mechanisms are two components of a single complex system (W. Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1995). It is thought that the wavelength-sensitive receptors are the retinal photoreceptors and the magnetite receptors are associated with the ophthalmic branch of the trigeminal nerve (Beason and Semm, 1991), but there is no direct evidence to support either conclusion.

The bobolink Dolichonyx oryzivorus, a New World transequatorial migratory bird, has been shown to use the geomagnetic field for directional information during its migratory orientation (Beason, 1987, 1989, 1992). Although visual cues (star patterns in a planetarium) play a role in the orientation of this species, the magnetic field provides the primary compass information when there is a conflict with other sources of information (Beason, 1987, 1989). The magnetic compass functions in the same fashion as reported for other species, i.e. it is an inclination compass that distinguishes between ‘equatorward’ and ‘poleward’ directions rather than north and south (Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1972). The objectives of these experiments were to test the hypotheses that the magnetite-based magnetoreceptor is associated with the ophthalmic nerve and that the information carried by this nerve is used for orientation. As in previous studies to investigate the transducer substance, we used a behavioural assay to determine the effects of the manipulation on the physiological system.

For these experiments, a total of 14 bobolinks Dolichonyx oryzivorus (L.) were tested during the autumn migratory season in funnel-test cages (Emlen and Emlen, 1966). The birds were captured as adults using mist-nets. All birds were housed indoors without a view of the natural sky throughout the experiment. When the bobolinks exhibited migratory readiness, they were tested between 19:30 h and 21:30 h under the natural magnetic field within a planetarium (see Beason, 1989, for details). The cages were covered with translucent tops that precluded the birds from obtaining any visual cues outside the cage. The experimental protocol was the same as that described previously (Beason et al. 1995), but involved three conditions: control, magnetized and nerve-blocked. Because the objective of the experiment was to determine the effect of the treatments on the orientation of individual birds, each bird served as its own control. Each bird’s modal nightly direction of activity was measured to the nearest 10 °. The modal direction (the direction with the greatest amount of activity) was used as a measure of central tendency because it is easier to evaluate and does not differ significantly from the mean direction (Cherry and Able, 1986). During the control period, the directional preference of each bird was determined by testing it on four successive nights and calculating the mean of the nightly modal directions. All nightly responses to the experimental manipulations were calculated as deviations from that bird’s control mean direction to reduce the effects of inter-individual variation in preferred headings (Beason, 1989). The effects of the treatments were assessed in an analysis of the second-order means. The significance of orientation for each pooled data set was tested with the Rayleigh test and means were compared with the control direction using confidence intervals (Zar, 1984). The means of treatments were compared using Watson’s U2-test (Zar, 1984).

Following the control period, each bird was magnetized north-anterior and tested on the following three nights. The birds were magnetized such that if the bill were iron, the tip would attract the south end of a compass (see Beason et al. 1995, for details of the magnetizing apparatus). In the final set of experiments, the ophthalmic nerve of the magnetized birds was blocked by the nightly application of a drop of Lidocaine (diluted 1:10 in saline) directly onto the nerve immediately before testing for three consecutive nights. Access to the nerve was gained surgically through a small incision anterior to the orbit made 2 days prior to the control portion of the experiment (three birds) or by insertion of a hypodermic needle through the skin covering the anterior part of the orbit (11 birds). For the surgical procedure, the birds were anaesthetized with a mixture of ketamine hydrochloride (Ketaset, 0.05 mg g−1) and xylazine (Rompun, 0.01 mg g−1). The nerve was blocked each night immediately before the birds were placed into the test cages at 19:30 h. The effectiveness of the nerve blocking was tested by tugging on the feathers of the forehead. If the bird flinched, additional anaesthesia was applied. Tugging the feathers was used as an indicator of successful anaesthesia because the ophthalmic nerve also receives sensory information from the mechanoreceptors associated with the feathers of the forehead. The birds were removed from the test cages at 21:30 h, before the effects of the anaesthesia dissipated. Because there were no differences in headings for any of the experiments between the birds whose ophthalmic nerve was exposed surgically and those in which the injection was made through the skin, the data for the two groups were pooled.

The mean direction of the birds during the control portion of the experiment was seasonally appropriate (Fig. 1), towards the southeast (α=138 °, r=0.854, P<0.001, where α is the mean direction and r is the mean vector length). After being magnetized north-anterior, the birds exhibited a mean deviation of 77 ° counterclockwise (α=283 °, r=0.629, N=11, P<0.01), which was significantly different (P<0.01) from the control direction (Fig. 2). When the ophthalmic branch of the trigeminal nerve of the magnetized bird was blocked by an injection of Lidocaine immediately prior to testing each evening, the mean deviation (α=13 °, r=0.746, N=10, P<0.01) of the birds was not significantly different (P>0.05) from the control direction, but differed significantly from their heading when the nerve was not blocked (U2=0.2945, P<0.01). The reason for the differences in sample sizes in Fig. 2A,B is that some birds gave a generalized response to the Lidocaine application and became inactive, apparently sleeping, during the last experiment.

Fig. 1.

Orientation of bobolinks during the autumn migration period. The mean direction for all birds was 138 ° (r=0.854, P<0.001, where a. is the mean direction and r is the mean vector length). Mean vectors of individual bobolinks during the control portion of the experiment are drawn proportional to the radius of the circle and relative to magnetic north (at the top of the circle). Each dot on the periphery represents the respective mean heading of each bird, N=14.

Fig. 1.

Orientation of bobolinks during the autumn migration period. The mean direction for all birds was 138 ° (r=0.854, P<0.001, where a. is the mean direction and r is the mean vector length). Mean vectors of individual bobolinks during the control portion of the experiment are drawn proportional to the radius of the circle and relative to magnetic north (at the top of the circle). Each dot on the periphery represents the respective mean heading of each bird, N=14.

Fig. 2.

Responses of individual bobolinks to treatment with a magnetic pulse and with Lidocaine relative to their individual control directions (indicated by the tick at the top of the circle). (A) Orientation of birds magnetized north-anterior (pooled mean: α=283 °, r=0.629, N=11, P<0.01). (B) Orientation of the same magnetized birds when their ophthalmic nerve was blocked with Lidocaine immediately prior to testing (pooled mean: α=13 °, r=0.746, N=10, P<0.01). The mean vector for each bird is drawn proportional to the radius of the circle. Each dot on the periphery represents the mean heading of one bird.

Fig. 2.

Responses of individual bobolinks to treatment with a magnetic pulse and with Lidocaine relative to their individual control directions (indicated by the tick at the top of the circle). (A) Orientation of birds magnetized north-anterior (pooled mean: α=283 °, r=0.629, N=11, P<0.01). (B) Orientation of the same magnetized birds when their ophthalmic nerve was blocked with Lidocaine immediately prior to testing (pooled mean: α=13 °, r=0.746, N=10, P<0.01). The mean vector for each bird is drawn proportional to the radius of the circle. Each dot on the periphery represents the mean heading of one bird.

It has been shown previously that the primary orientation cue used by bobolinks during the migratory season is the geomagnetic field (Beason, 1987, 1989, 1992). The mean direction selected by the birds in this experiment is the same as in previous experiments in which orientation cues were manipulated (Beason, 1989, 1992) and is seasonally appropriate. The deviation produced by the magnetizing pulse is not significantly different (U2=0.077, P>0.05) from that reported previously for birds magnetized in the same manner (Beason et al. 1995) and indicates that a magnetizable material (probably magnetite) is employed by the birds for magnetoreception during migratory orientation. This receptor appears to be associated with the ophthalmic nerve because the effects of the magnetization could be abolished by blocking the nerve. Consequently, the electrophysiological responses of ophthalmic nerve cells to magnetic stimulation (Beason and Semm, 1987; Semm and Beason, 1990) are biologically relevant and probably reflect the sensory capability of magnetic receptors associated with the magnetite deposits.

Wiltschko et al. (1994) reported that the effect of magnetization had diminished by the second day following treatment in silvereyes and was absent by the fourth day. In previous experiments with bobolinks, the effect of the magnetization persisted through the 4–5 days of post-treatment testing (Beason et al. 1995). Because the present experiments only lasted 6 days after treatment, the change in heading on day 4 is almost certainly caused by the nerve block and not by an accommodation to the magnetization.

These results could indicate that more than one type of receptor is used to obtain magnetic compass information, with the bird giving priority to the magnetite-based receptor of the ophthalmic nerve. When information from the ophthalmic nerve is unavailable (because of the blocked nerve in this case), the bird would have to rely exclusively on alternative compass receptors. If the sole magnetic compass receptor were affected by the nerve blocking, then the birds would not have been able to select and maintain a constant direction in the absence of visual cues. Because the Lidocaine-treated birds were not disoriented and showed the same heading as they did before being magnetized, they must have had a functioning compass that was not based on a magnetizable material. The only compasses that have been reported in migratory birds are visual and magnetic (Beason and Semm, 1991). When the visual cues (star patterns in a planetarium) and the natural or artificial magnetic field were oriented in opposite directions, bobolinks used the magnetic compass as their primary source of directional information (Beason, 1987, 1989). In the present experiments, it is unlikely that visual cues were being used for orientation because the cages were covered with translucent plastic tops that diffused the light and prevented the birds from discerning any objects outside their cages. The dim illumination (1.0 lx) was produced by bulbs that ringed the inside of the planetarium dome and reflected off the dome, resulting in uniform illumination. The test cages were distributed in a circle about the centre of the dome and positioned so that they were level with the widest point of the dome to prevent light/dark patterns or gradients in light intensity.

If the birds were not using visual cues for orientation when the ophthalmic nerve was blocked, then one explanation is that they were using another magnetic receptor. The retinal photoreceptors could potentially fulfill that role. The use of retinal photoreceptors for magnetic field detection would be consistent with the results of other experiments on vertebrates in which the direction selected for orientation was modified by the wavelength of the illumination (Phillips and Borland, 1992, 1994; Wiltschko et al. 1993; W. Wiltschko and Wiltschko, 1995).

Alternatively, if the bobolink uses a ‘map and compass’ in the same way as humans (Kramer, 1953), these results could be interpreted to mean that magnetite-based receptors associated with the ophthalmic nerve provided location (‘map’) information and another receptor using a nonferromagnetic transducer provided compass information. The sensitivity of the ophthalmic nerve cells meets the requirements of a receptor used for a magnetic map (Semm and

Beason, 1990). If the bird did not know where it was (its map was not functioning because the input was blocked by the Lidocaine), one strategy would be to adopt a heading in a seasonally appropriate direction (i.e. southeast in the autumn). Because all the birds in our experiment were captured as adults, they had made at least one migratory round trip previously and could have been ‘goal-oriented’ towards their non-breeding grounds. The responses of the magnetized bobolinks are what would be predicted for birds compensating for longitudinal displacement to the west (Beason et al. 1995). However, in order to compensate, they would require a functioning ‘map’. The magnetic field has the theoretical potential to provide bicoordinate map information (comparing horizontal field intensity with vertical or total field intensity in the northeastern United States results in a grid intersecting at 20 °), but whether it actually fulfills that function is unresolved (Gould, 1982).

The real proof of these conclusions can only come from navigation experiments in which the ophthalmic nerve is blocked and if map effects then disappear. In order to establish that it is a map factor which is affected, the bird’s location and its goal must be known. The most logical experiment is to magnetize homing pigeons (Wiltschko and Beason, 1991; R. C. Beason, R. Wiltschko and W. Wiltschko, in preparation) and to test the effect of blocking the ophthalmic nerve on their homing abilities.

Financial assistance was provided by the National Science Foundation (to R.C.B.) and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (to P.S.). We thank Christine Hertler and Michelle Ferreri for their assistance with the experiments and Stephen Wiener for providing the Lidocaine. Charles Walcott, Wolfgang Wiltschko, Sue Bennett and two anonymous reviewers provided helpful comments on earlier versions of the manuscript.

Beason
,
R. C.
(
1987
).
Interaction of visual and non-visual cues during migratory orientation by the bobolink
.
J. Orn.
128
,
317
324
.
Beason
,
R. C.
(
1989
).
Use of an inclination compass during migratory orientation by the bobolink (Dolichonyx oryzivorus)
.
Ethology
81
,
291
299
.
Beason
,
R. C.
(
1992
).
You can get there from here: Responses to simulated magnetic equator crossing by the bobolink (Dolichonyx oryzivorus)
.
Ethology
91
,
75
80
.
Beason
,
R. C.
and
Brennan
,
W. J.
(
1986
).
Natural and induced magnetization in the bobolink, Dolichonyx oryzivorus (Aves: Icteridae)
.
J. exp. Biol.
125
,
49
56
.
Beason
,
R. C.
,
Dussourd
,
N.
and
Deutschlander
,
M. E.
(
1995
).
Behavioural evidence for the use of magnetic material in magnetoreception by a migratory bird
.
J. exp. Biol.
198
,
141
146
.
Beason
,
R. C.
and
Semm
,
P.
(
1987
).
Magnetic responses of the trigeminal nerve system of the bobolink, Dolichonyx oryzivorus (Aves: Icteridae)
.
Neurosci. Lett.
80
,
229
234
.
Beason
,
R. C.
and
Semm
,
P.
(
1991
).
Neuroethological aspects of avian orientation
. In
Orientation in Birds
(ed.
P.
Berthold
), pp.
106
127
. Basel: Birkhäuser Verlag.
Beason
,
R. C.
and
Semm
,
P.
(
1994
).
Receptors for and detection of magnetic fields in birds
. In
Biological Effects of Electric and Magnetic Fields
, vol.
1
(ed.
D. O.
Carpenter
and
S.
Ayrapetyan
), pp.
241
260
.
New York
:
Academic Press
.
Cherry
,
J. D.
and
Able
,
K. P.
(
1986
).
An alternative method for the analysis of Emlen funnel data
.
Auk
103
,
225
227
.
Emlen
,
S. T.
and
Emlen
,
J. T.
(
1966
).
A technique for recording migratory orientation of captive birds
.
Auk
83
,
361
367
.
Gould
,
J. L.
(
1982
).
The map sense of pigeons
.
Nature
296
,
205
211
.
Kramer
,
G.
(
1953
).
Die Sonnenorientierung der Vogel
.
Verh. dt. zool. Ges.
1953
,
72
84
.
Phillips
,
J. B.
(
1986
).
Two magnetoreception pathways in a migratory salamander
.
Science
233
,
765
767
.
Phillips
,
J. B.
and
Borland
,
S. C.
(
1992
).
Behavioural evidence for use of a light-dependent magnetoreception mechanism by a vertebrate
.
Nature
359
,
142
144
.
Phillips
,
J. B.
and
Borland
,
S. C.
(
1994
).
Use of a specialized magnetoreception system for homing by the eastern red-spotted newt (Notophthalmus viridescens)
.
J. exp. Biol.
188
,
275
291
.
Semm
,
P.
and
Beason
,
R. C.
(
1990
).
Responses to small magnetic variations by the trigeminal system of the bobolink
.
Brain Res. Bull.
25
,
735
740
.
Wiltschko
,
R.
and
Wiltschko
,
W.
(
1995
).
Magnetic Orientation in Animals
.
Berlin
:
Springer-Verlag
.
Wiltschko
,
W.
and
Beason
,
R. C.
(
1991
).
Magneteffekte bei der Heimorientierung von Brieftauben
.
Verh. dt. zool. Ges.
83
,
435
436
.
Wiltschko
,
W.
,
Munro
,
U.
,
Beason
,
R. C.
,
Ford
,
H.
and
Wiltschko
,
R.
(
1994
).
A magnetic pulse leads to a temporary deflection in the orientation of migratory birds
.
Experientia
50
,
697
700
.
Wiltschko
,
W.
,
Munro
,
U.
,
Ford
,
H.
and
Wiltschko
,
R.
(
1993
).
Red light disrupts magnetic orientation of migratory birds
.
Nature
364
,
525
527
.
Wiltschko
,
W.
and
Wiltschko
,
R.
(
1972
).
Magnetic compass of European robins
.
Science
176
,
62
66
.
Wiltschko
,
W.
and
Wiltschko
,
R.
(
1988
).
Magnetic orientation in birds
.
Curr. Orn.
5
,
67
121
.
Wiltschko
,
W.
and
Wiltschko
,
R.
(
1995
).
Migratory orientation of European robins is affected by the wavelength of light as well as by a magnetic pulse
.
J. comp. Physiol. A
177
,
363
369
.
Zar
,
J. H.
(
1984
).
Biostatistical Analysis, 2nd edn
.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ
:
Prentice Hall
.