Conopressin G, a molluscan vasopressin-like peptide, when superfused over the abdominal ganglion suppressed gill withdrawal reflex behavior patterns. The effects of conopressin G on Aplysia californica central sensory neurons and on the sensory neuron to motor neuron synapse were inconsistent with its behavioral effects. That is, superfusion of the peptide facilitated synaptic transmission at this synapse and reversed low-frequency homosynaptic depression. Further, conopressin G potentiated frequency-dependent spike broadening, reduced spike threshold and reduced accommodation. A voltage-dependent outward K+ current was suppressed by the peptide. This current was also suppressed by Co2+ and Ba2+ and was relatively resistant to tetraethylammonium and 4-aminopyridine. The effects produced by conopressin G on the sensory neurons were not observed when Ca2+ was removed from the saline, when a low-Ca2+, high-Mg2+ saline was used or when other procedures that impair synaptic transmission were used. These results suggest that the effects of conopressin G were mediated by a polysynaptic pathway acting on the sensory neurons.

A novel molluscan vasopressin analogue, conopressin G, extracted from the venom offish-hunting snails of the genus Conus (Cruz et al. 1987), has been shown to suppress the siphon-evoked gill withdrawal reflex (GWR) and to increase the rate of spontaneous gill movements (SGMs) in semi-intact Aplysia californica preparations (Martínez-Padrón et al. 1992). Furthermore, conopressin G or a closely related peptide might be endogenous to Aplysia californica (McMaster et al. 1989a,b). The experiments described here explore the possibility that conopressin G suppresses the GWR at the level of the sensory neuron to motor neuron synapse.

The siphon sensory neuron to motor neuron synapse located in the abdominal ganglia of Aplysia californica has traditionally been considered to be an important neural correlate of the GWR and its plasticity. Behavioral sensitization has been shown to be caused, at least in part, by presynaptic facilitation of this synapse (Castellucci and Kandel, 1976; Klein and Kandel, 1978). For the most part, changes in the efficacy of this synapse have been attributed to changes in the duration of the action potential of the sensory neuron and in the presynaptic calcium current, which alter the number of transmitter quanta released by the sensory cell during synaptic transmission (Castellucci and Kandel, 1976).

Considerable information has been gathered on the ability of various neuroactive agents to influence the electrical properties and synaptic efficacy of siphon sensory cells. Both serotonin and small cardioactive peptide (SCPb) broaden the action potential of the sensory neuron, facilitate synaptic transmission (Abrams et al. 1984; Brunelli et al. 1976; Klein and Kandel, 1978) and enhance the siphon-stimulated GWR (Abrams et al. 1984). Conversely, arginine vasotocin (AVT) and FMRFamide cause narrowing of the action potential, synaptic depression (Goldberg et al. 1987; Mackey et al. 1987) and suppression of the GWR (Thornhill et al. 1981).

A common target for the action of endogenous modulatory neuroactive agents seems to be a specific, relatively voltage-independent potassium current in the sensory neurons termed the S current because of its sensitivity to serotonin (Klein et al. 1982). For example, serotonin both depolarized and caused an increase in the input resistance of Aplysia californica sensory neurons by closing the S channel (Pollock et al. 1985). Similar increases in the input resistance of these cells and closure of the S channel are caused by SCPb (Abrams et al. 1984). In constrast, both FMRFamide (Belardetti et al. 1987; Brezina et al. 1987) and acetylcholine (Ichinose et al. 1989) open the S channel. Modulation of the S channel is thought to be an important process in synaptic plasticity. Since the S current contributes to the repolarization of the neuron during an action potential, its closure by serotonin or SCPb will increase the duration of the action potential in sensory neurons so that more calcium enters the presynaptic terminal, allowing more transmitter quanta to be released (Klein and Kandel, 1980; Pollock et al. 1985). Opening of the S channel by FMRFamide results in narrowing of the action potential with the opposite consequences (Mackey et al. 1987). From these studies it seemed reasonable to hypothesize that conopressin G suppressed the GWR in semi-intact preparations (Martínez-Padrón et al. 1992) by narrowing the sensory neuron action potential in a similar manner.

However, the experiments described here show that conopressin G depresses a voltage-dependent potassium current in the sensory neurons and increases the efficacy of the sensory neuron to motor neuron synapse. These effects resemble those produced by serotonin on these cells. Such effects at the level of the single neuron are contrary to what was expected from the behavioral effects of conopressin G and are inconsistent with the prominent role given to these elements as neuronal correlates of the GWR.

Aplysia californica ranging in mass from 100 to 250 g were obtained from Alacrity Marine Biological Services (Redondo Beach, California). Experiments were performed on the isolated abdominal ganglion or on a preparation consisting of the abdominal ganglion attached to the siphon. In the latter, the abdominal ganglion was isolated from the bath by means of a surrounding plastic ring sealed with Vaseline. This made it possible to apply chemicals specifically to either part of the preparation. To improve space-clamp conditions, in some experiments clusters of siphon sensory neurons were isolated surgically and secured by suction to the tip of a glass pipette to facilitate penetration.

Animals were anesthetized (by injection of one-third of their body weight of isotonic MgCI2), after which the abdominal ganglion was removed and pinned down, ventral side up, on a Sylgard (Dow-Corning) coated culture dish. Desheathing of the left ventral side, to gain access to the sensory neurons, was performed after bathing the ganglion in hypertonic artificial sea water (2 mol I−1 sucrose in ASW 1:1) for 15 min. In some cases, the ganglia had previously been bathed for 1 h in ASW containing 2% protease type IX (Sigma P-6141) to facilitate desheathing. No differences in the effects of conopressin G were observed as a result of the use of protease.

Siphon sensory neurons were identified by their morphology, their position in the ganglion and their firing properties as described in the literature (Byrne et al. 1974; Koester and Kandel, 1977). A group of small siphon motor neurons (LFS cells, Frost et al. 1988) located in close proximity to the sensory cells at the entrance of the siphon nerve, as well as L7 and LBS motor neurons, were used as follower cells.

Single-barrelled micropipettes were pulled from thin-walled borosilicate glass (1.5mm outer diameter) to a final resistance of 5–15M Ω when filled with 0.8 mol I−1 KCI solution. A Wheatstone bridge circuit (Getting M-5 or Axoclamp 2-A) was used for membrane potential recordings and/or current injection into the cells. In order to achieve proper voltage control of the postsynaptic membrane potential, large cells such as L7 and LBS were impaled with two electrodes, one for passing current and the other for voltage recording. Moderate-intensity (1g, 50–250 ms) tactile stimulation was delivered to the siphon by means of a mechanical tapper, as described by Peretz and Lukowiak (1975). Neuronal activity was displayed on digital (Nicolet 2090-III) and analog (Tektronix) storage oscilloscopes and on a four-channel Gould recorder.

The effect of conopressin G on the duration of the sensory neuron action potential was assessed during frequency-dependent spike broadening (FDSB, Edstrom and Lukowiak, 1985). Trains of action potentials were produced by injecting short (5 ms) depolarizing current pulses (3–7 nA) into the cell at a frequency of 6 Hz for 5 s. The short duration of the pulses ensured that the action potential was triggered after the current break, preventing possible distortions in shape due to interaction between electrode and membrane currents. The trains were acquired with pCLAMP software running on a COMPAQ Deskpro computer and the action potentials were aligned using the maximum rate of change of voltage for comparison.

Neuronal excitability was quantified as the number of spikes triggered by a 500ms depolarizing pulse (0.75–1.5 nA) injected into the cell. During both FDSB and excitability experiments, the sensory cells were stimulated once every 5 min. At least three control measurements were taken before application of any drug to ensure that the response of the cell was reproducible. In experiments examining the effects of conopressin G on homosynaptic depression, the sensory neurons were stimulated once every 50 s. Conopressin G was added to the bath following the tenth stimulus.

Single action potentials were evoked in the sensory cell by injecting short (5 ms) depolarizing current pulses (3–5nA). This produced a complex mono-and polysynaptic excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP) in the follower cells. To avoid low-frequency homosynaptic depression, the sensory cell was stimulated once every 15–20 min. To improve the resolution of individual components within the EPSP, the excitability of polysynaptic elements of the circuit was reduced by increasing the concentration of extracellular Mg2+ to 80 mmol I−1. On several occasions during conopressin G application, the amplitude of the first component of the EPSP could not be resolved, being masked by the appearance of an early second component. In such cases all EPSP amplitudes were taken at the time when the second component started.

Cells were voltage-clamped using an Axoclamp-2A amplifier (Axon Instruments) in the discontinuous single-electrode voltage-clamp configuration. The electrodes were shielded to within 100 mm of the tip with silver conductive paint connected to the driven shield and insulated with a coating of polystyrene Q-Dope to reduce the capacitance of the electrodes and to allow the use of high switching rates (15–20kHz).

The cells were voltage-clamped at − 40 mV and membrane currents were activated by 400 ms pulses to hyperpolarized and depolarized subthreshold potentials (usually up to -20mV). Pulses were elicited at 5s intervals, and no cumulative inactivation was observed with this protocol. Voltage command generation and data acquisition were managed by pCLAMP software (clampex, 5.5). Up to three control trials were taken prior to the initiation of the experiment to ensure that the recording was stable. Conopressin G was applied once to verify the responsiveness of the cell prior to any attempt to block its effect. Passive membrane leakage was calculated by measuring current flow in response to a 5 mV voltage command in the hyperpolarizing direction. The leakage current was assumed to be a linear function of membrane potential and it was subtracted, after scaling, from the current traces in order to isolate the time-and voltage-dependent components of the total membrane current.

To perform tail current analysis, the membrane currents were activated by depolarizing voltage steps to − 20 mV for 270 ms followed by a second pulse to a variety of membrane potentials in the range of the expected reversal potential (see Fig. 7A). Strong hyperpolarizing command pulses, however, produced an activation of other current(s), which contaminated the recordings, and the analysis had to be restricted to membrane potentials more positive than −75 mV. Each family of tail currents thus obtained was fitted by a series of single-exponential equations with the aid of a computer program (CLAMPFIT, pCLAMP), and the amplitudes of the tail currents were calculated by extrapolating the exponential values to the time of the beginning of the second pulse. By plotting the tail current amplitude versus holding membrane potential the data points can be fitted by a straight line whose intersection with the zero point on the y-axis gives an estimate of the reversal potential. This series of experiments was performed on isolated abdominal sensory clusters, because inadequate space-clamp of the cell tended to distort measurements of the reversal potential (see Jack et al. 1975).

The chamber containing the abdominal ganglia was superfused at a constant rate of 1–3 ml min−1 with one of the various solutions listed in Table 1. Intermediate concentrations of tetraethylammonium (TEA+) and 4-aminopyridine (4-AP) were obtained by mixing their respective solutions with ASW in the proper proportions. Likewise, intermediate concentrations of extracellular potassium were obtained by mixing specific proportions of 0 mmol l−1 K+ ASW with 100 mmol E1 K+ ASW. In some experiments the recording electrode contained 2 mol l-1 CsCl solution. CsCl was usually allowed to leak passively or was injected into the cell by applying depolarizing direct current.

Table 1.

All solutions were adjusted to pH 7.78 at room temperature and contained 26 mmol l-1MgSO4and 5 mmol l-1Hepes buffer, and the following concentrations (mmoll-1) of other salts

All solutions were adjusted to pH 7.78 at room temperature and contained 26 mmol l-1MgSO4and 5 mmol l-1Hepes buffer, and the following concentrations (mmoll-1) of other salts
All solutions were adjusted to pH 7.78 at room temperature and contained 26 mmol l-1MgSO4and 5 mmol l-1Hepes buffer, and the following concentrations (mmoll-1) of other salts

Drugs were applied to the preparation after being dissolved in the superfusate or were added directly into the chamber to the desired final concentration. Conopressin G was used at the same concentration (1 μmoll−1) as that required to produce behavioral changes of the GWR and the respiratory rate (Martínez-Padrón et al. 1992). Other vasopressin analogues used in these studies (usually at 10μmoll−1) included conopressin S, arginine vasopressin, lysine vasopressin, arginine vasotocin, lysine vasotocin, oxytocin and mesotocin. These were purchased from Bachem Inc. (Torrance, CA). Serotonin and SCPb were purchased from Sigma Ltd (St Louis, MO). Conopressin G and S were a gift from Dr W. Gray (Utah).

Conopressin G enhances frequency-dependent spike broadening

The normal broadening of a sensory neuron action potential during a repetitive train of activity is shown in Fig. 1A, where alternate action potentials in a train of 30 are shown superimposed. The duration of the action potential increases during the train, with the growth of a shoulder on the repolarization phase. The last action potential in the train is much longer than the first. In the presence of conopressin G, the shoulder grows wider more rapidly (Fig. IB). This effect is reversible within 10 min (not shown) and is not mimicked by any other vasopressin analogue.

Fig. 1.

Representative data showing the effect of conopressin G on frequencydependent spike broadening in siphon sensory neurons. (A) A train of 30 action potentials was produced by injecting short depolarizing pulses (3–5 nA, 5 ms) at a frequency of 6 Hz for 5 s in normal ASW. Alternate action potentials are displayed after alignment on the maximum rate of voltage change. (B) Bath application of conopressin G (1μmoll−1) results in a delayed repolarization of the membrane with respect to the control. (C) Computer subtraction of the train of action potentials in ASW from that produced during superfusion of conopressin G (Con.G). The progressive increase in the plateau caused by the peptide appears as a series of positive curves showing a maximum between 4 and 7 ms. (D) Plot of the maximum amplitude increase for each action potential of the train. The filled squares represent the change in voltage produced by conopressin G with respect to the control at the time of the peak effect (from C). For comparison, the triangles display the voltage change between the control (C2) and a previous control (Cl, not shown) taken 5 min earlier.

Fig. 1.

Representative data showing the effect of conopressin G on frequencydependent spike broadening in siphon sensory neurons. (A) A train of 30 action potentials was produced by injecting short depolarizing pulses (3–5 nA, 5 ms) at a frequency of 6 Hz for 5 s in normal ASW. Alternate action potentials are displayed after alignment on the maximum rate of voltage change. (B) Bath application of conopressin G (1μmoll−1) results in a delayed repolarization of the membrane with respect to the control. (C) Computer subtraction of the train of action potentials in ASW from that produced during superfusion of conopressin G (Con.G). The progressive increase in the plateau caused by the peptide appears as a series of positive curves showing a maximum between 4 and 7 ms. (D) Plot of the maximum amplitude increase for each action potential of the train. The filled squares represent the change in voltage produced by conopressin G with respect to the control at the time of the peak effect (from C). For comparison, the triangles display the voltage change between the control (C2) and a previous control (Cl, not shown) taken 5 min earlier.

Differences in FDSB in two trains can be quantified by subtracting each action potential in one train from its counterpart in another, to obtain a family of curves showing the difference in voltage throughout each action potential (Fig. 1C). The phenomenon can be summarized by taking the peak amplitude of each curve and using it as an index of the degree to which broadening was increased between trains. This is defined as the maximum amplitude increase (MAI, Edstrom and Lukowiak, 1985) and is shown in Fig. ID, where the enhanced FDSB in 1 μmol l−1 conopressin G is depicted by filled squares. The filled triangles show the MAI for two control trains (Cl and C2), demonstrating the reproducibility of the phenomenon under these conditions. The difference between the two traces is statistically significant (Wilcoxon signed-rank test, P<0.01). Statistically similar results to those shown in Fig. 1 were obtained in all other siphon sensory cells analyzed (N=10) in this study. The maximum amplitude increase for the last action potential of the trains ranged between 9 and 40 mV. The effect of conopressin G was usually not apparent when the first action potentials of each train were compared or when the peptide was tested on single action potentials elicited at 5 min intervals.

This observed enhancement of FDSB by conopressin G was not due to its effect on the membrane potential, since it was not inhibited by injecting hyperpolarizing current to hold the resting potential constant and it could not be induced by a comparable depolarization with depolarizing current in the absence of conopressin G.

Sensory neuron excitability

The ability of conopressin G to increase the excitability of sensory neurons, defined here as the ability of depolarizing current injected at the soma to induce action potentials, is shown in Fig. 2. Under normal conditions a 500 ms pulse of current triggers no more than 2–3 action potentials at the beginning of the pulse (Fig. 2A). In the presence of conopressin G, the threshold current for the first action potential is reduced, and action potentials can occur throughout the pulse (Fig. 2B) and, in some cases, outlast the pulse (not shown) so that the number of action potentials for a given current is increased. This effect is reversible within 10min and is not mimicked by the other vasopressin analogues tested.

Fig. 2.

An example of the effect of conopressin G on siphon sensory neuron excitability. (A) Long depolarizing current injection (500 ms) produces a characteristic accommodating firing pattern in Aplysia californica sensory cells (left-hand traces). Note that, in ASW, increasing the current from 3 to 5 nA produces only one additional action potential and the cell accommodates strongly. 1 μmol I−1 conopressin G applied to the bath greatly decreases accommodation, allowing the cell to fire during the entire duration of the pulse (right-hand traces). (B) Plot of the number of spikes triggered by the cell represented in A versus current injected in normal ASW (squares) and after bath application of conopressin G (triangles). The firing threshold for both situations is indicated by the arrows, and it was lowered by conopressin G, in this example from 3 to 1 nA. Note that the sensory cell did not fire more than two action potentials in ASW when injected current was 5 or 7 nA.

Fig. 2.

An example of the effect of conopressin G on siphon sensory neuron excitability. (A) Long depolarizing current injection (500 ms) produces a characteristic accommodating firing pattern in Aplysia californica sensory cells (left-hand traces). Note that, in ASW, increasing the current from 3 to 5 nA produces only one additional action potential and the cell accommodates strongly. 1 μmol I−1 conopressin G applied to the bath greatly decreases accommodation, allowing the cell to fire during the entire duration of the pulse (right-hand traces). (B) Plot of the number of spikes triggered by the cell represented in A versus current injected in normal ASW (squares) and after bath application of conopressin G (triangles). The firing threshold for both situations is indicated by the arrows, and it was lowered by conopressin G, in this example from 3 to 1 nA. Note that the sensory cell did not fire more than two action potentials in ASW when injected current was 5 or 7 nA.

The average number of spikes in 17 cells in response to a constant pulse of current was 1.88±0.26 (mean±s.E.M.) under control conditions, 7.76±0.75 in the presence of conopressin G and 2.00±0.24 after washout; the difference between the control and the conopressin G treatment was statistically significant (t=−9.71, d.f. = 15, P<0.01). In five other experiments similar to that depicted in Fig. 2, several levels of current injection were applied to the cells. In all cases the peptide not only increased the number of spikes at all current levels, but also caused a reduction in the firing threshold.

Abdominal sensory neurons are naturally activated by mechanical stimulation of the siphon and mantle skin rather than by synaptic input to the soma or dendrites (Byrne et al. 1974). It is possible that these changes in excitability seen in the soma may affect the volley of action potentials coming from the siphon. To test this hypothesis the siphon-attached preparation was used (see Materials and methods). Mechanical stimulation of the siphon (1g, 50–250 ms) produced a train of action potentials in the sensory cell soma rising suddenly from the baseline and reflecting propagation of spikes into the soma from the axon. These trains retained a relatively constant pattern when the siphon was stimulated at 5 min intervals. In these preparations the application of conopressin G to the abdominal ganglion increased somatic excitability as expected, but did not have any noticeable effect on the siphon-evoked sensory discharge. The average number of action potentials following siphon stimulation in six experiments was 4.66±1.63 (mean±s.E.M.) before and 4.33±1.75 after conopressin G (t=1.58, d.f.=4, P>0.05).

Like conopressin G, serotonin and SCPb have also been shown to produce an increase in somatic excitability (Klein et al. 1986) and action potential duration (Abrams et al. 1984; Klein and Kandel, 1978; Pollock et al. 1985). These agents appear to act directly on the sensory neurons, since they are active on isolated cells and/or in the presence of high Ca2+ and high Mg2+ concentrations (Abrams et al. 1984; Baxter and Byrne, 1990). Whether conopressin G acted directly on siphon sensory neurons or through interneurons was tested by applying the peptide after blocking synaptic transmission with low-Ca2+, high-Mg2+ ASW. Under these conditions, conopressin G did not increase sensory neuron excitability (N=5), whereas serotonin (Fig. 3) and SCPb continued to increase neuronal excitability to the same extent as they did in normal saline. In addition, although conopressin G very often produced a slight depolarization of the membrane, this effect was slow, more than 1 min was needed to see an effect, compared with the effects of serotonin and SCPb, which appeared within a few seconds.

Fig. 3.

Blocking synaptic transmission abolishes the effect of conopressin G on sensory neuron accommodation. Application of 1 μmoll−1 conopressin G in low-Ca2+, high-Mg2+ solution, a treatment that prevents synaptic transmission, did not result in increased excitability (upper traces), although the same cell was responsive in ASW (not shown). The same treatment did not prevent the effect of 50μmol l−1 serotonin, which acts directly on the sensory cell (lower traces).

Fig. 3.

Blocking synaptic transmission abolishes the effect of conopressin G on sensory neuron accommodation. Application of 1 μmoll−1 conopressin G in low-Ca2+, high-Mg2+ solution, a treatment that prevents synaptic transmission, did not result in increased excitability (upper traces), although the same cell was responsive in ASW (not shown). The same treatment did not prevent the effect of 50μmol l−1 serotonin, which acts directly on the sensory cell (lower traces).

Siphon sensory neuron to motor neuron synaptic transmission

The application of 1μmoll−1 conopressin G produced a reversible increase in the amplitude of the initial component of the motor neuron’s EPSP, which presumably corresponds to the monosynaptic pathway (Fig. 4). In these experiments (N=9), conopressin G significantly increased the peak amplitude of the initial component of the EPSP to 229% (t=3.7, d.f. =7, P<0.02). This increment in amplitude sometimes occurred without a change in the rate of rise, resulting in an increased time-to-peak of the synaptic potentials. In other cases, conopressin G produced an EPSP with a faster rising rate (see Fig. 5B).

Fig. 4.

(A) Data from a single experiment showing the synaptic facilitation induced by l μmoll−1 conopressin G applied to the bath. EPSPs were evoked in an LFS motor neuron by firing the sensory neuron every 15 min. At this frequency the amplitude of the EPSP was stable. The peptide was applied to the bath 5 min before the experimental trial. As can be seen, conopressin G increased the amplitude of the monosynaptic EPSP. The amplitude of the EPSP returned to control levels following washout of the peptide. (B) Normalized data from nine similar experiments. Conopressin G caused a greater than 100% increase in the average amplitude of the EPSP. Values are mean+s.D.

Fig. 4.

(A) Data from a single experiment showing the synaptic facilitation induced by l μmoll−1 conopressin G applied to the bath. EPSPs were evoked in an LFS motor neuron by firing the sensory neuron every 15 min. At this frequency the amplitude of the EPSP was stable. The peptide was applied to the bath 5 min before the experimental trial. As can be seen, conopressin G increased the amplitude of the monosynaptic EPSP. The amplitude of the EPSP returned to control levels following washout of the peptide. (B) Normalized data from nine similar experiments. Conopressin G caused a greater than 100% increase in the average amplitude of the EPSP. Values are mean+s.D.

Fig. 5.

Conopressin G counteracts homosynaptic depression. In A and C a sensory neuron was stimulated by current injection to produce one action potential every 50 s, which resulted in homosynaptic depression of the EPSP recorded in a follower LFS motor neuron. A few selected traces are shown in A. (B) Traces 1, 10 and 13 from A aligned to compare their amplitudes and rising rate. Note that homosynaptic depression resulted in smaller amplitudes and slower rising rates of the EPSP and that 1μmoll−1 conopressin G was able to restore the rising rate and slightly increase the amplitude of the EPSP with respect to control levels. (C) The EPSP amplitude for all trials in this experiment are plotted. The addition of conopressin G (arrow) produces a marked increase in the EPSP amplitude after a delay of 100s.

Fig. 5.

Conopressin G counteracts homosynaptic depression. In A and C a sensory neuron was stimulated by current injection to produce one action potential every 50 s, which resulted in homosynaptic depression of the EPSP recorded in a follower LFS motor neuron. A few selected traces are shown in A. (B) Traces 1, 10 and 13 from A aligned to compare their amplitudes and rising rate. Note that homosynaptic depression resulted in smaller amplitudes and slower rising rates of the EPSP and that 1μmoll−1 conopressin G was able to restore the rising rate and slightly increase the amplitude of the EPSP with respect to control levels. (C) The EPSP amplitude for all trials in this experiment are plotted. The addition of conopressin G (arrow) produces a marked increase in the EPSP amplitude after a delay of 100s.

Conopressin G counteracted homosynaptic depression produced by stimulating the sensory neuron every 50s (Fig. 5). Conopressin G (1 μmol l−1) was introduced into the bath in a single application immediately after trial 10. By trial 13 the initial component of the EPSP in the postsynaptic cell had recovered to control levels; it remained elevated for several more trials. The EPSPs from trials 1,10 and 13 have been aligned in Fig. 5B to compare their rates of rise. Between trials 1 and 10 there was a progressive decrement in the rate of rise that accompanied the reduction in amplitude. Conopressin G restored the rate of rise to the initial control level and produced an additional increase in amplitude. Reversal of homosynaptic depression by conopressin G only became apparent 2 min after the application of the peptide. This is in contrast to the more rapid effects of serotonin and SCPb observed within a few seconds in 10 other experiments (not shown).

To test whether the action of conopressin G was directly mediated, the effect of the peptide was tested after the ganglion had been bathed in high-Ca2+, high-Mg2+ solution. This procedure abolished the facilitatory effect of conopressin G on the initial component of the EPSP (N=5) and also abolished the ability of this peptide to reverse homosynaptic depression (not shown).

Conopressin G suppresses an outward potassium current

Typically Aplysia californica sensory neurons have a resting membrane potential close to − 40 mV and show no synaptic input or spontaneous activity. When they are voltage-clamped close to their resting membrane potential, step command pulses to subthreshold depolarized potentials elicit a relatively slow, noninactivating outward current followed by a large tail current upon return to the holding potential.

Bath application of 1 μmol l−1 conopressin G had a variety of effects on the total membrane current (Fig. 6). First, there was a shift of the holding current in the inward direction, paralleled by a slight depolarization of the resting potential under current-clamp conditions (not shown). Second, there was a reduction in the magnitude of the leakage current. Third, conopressin G produced a significant reduction in the total outward membrane current activated by pulses in the depolarizing direction (Fig. 6A). These effects were completely reversible. Usually, the more depolarized voltage commands (to − 20 mV) caused the cells to fire uncontrolled action potentials when conopressin G was present in the bath (not shown). The threshold concentration for the action of conopressin G was about 1 nmol l−1. This concentration caused a small decrease in the outward current in two out of four cells tested, whereas 50% suppression of the outward current occurred in the range 0.1–1μmol l−1.

Fig. 6.

Effect of conopressin G on sensory neuron outward membrane current.(A) Membrane current produced by 400 ms square command voltages (3 mV steps) from a holding potential of −40mV in the negative and positive directions; before, during and after bath application of 1μmoll−1 conopressin G. The horizontal line represents the zero current level. Conopressin G produced a reversible reduction of the total outward membrane current and an inward shift of the holding current.(B) Quasi steady-state current-voltage plot of the cell represented in A, at the end of the depolarizing pulse (no leakage subtraction applied). Control, filled squares; conopressin G, filled triangles. (C) Quasi steady-state current-voltage plot of the conopressin-G-sensitive current obtained by subtracting the total current in conopressin G from that in ASW (top). The voltage-dependent component of the conopressin G effect was isolated by subtracting its effect on the linear leakage current (bottom).

Fig. 6.

Effect of conopressin G on sensory neuron outward membrane current.(A) Membrane current produced by 400 ms square command voltages (3 mV steps) from a holding potential of −40mV in the negative and positive directions; before, during and after bath application of 1μmoll−1 conopressin G. The horizontal line represents the zero current level. Conopressin G produced a reversible reduction of the total outward membrane current and an inward shift of the holding current.(B) Quasi steady-state current-voltage plot of the cell represented in A, at the end of the depolarizing pulse (no leakage subtraction applied). Control, filled squares; conopressin G, filled triangles. (C) Quasi steady-state current-voltage plot of the conopressin-G-sensitive current obtained by subtracting the total current in conopressin G from that in ASW (top). The voltage-dependent component of the conopressin G effect was isolated by subtracting its effect on the linear leakage current (bottom).

The current-voltage relationship of the total membrane current in ASW and conopressin G is shown in Fig. 6B (same cell as in A), where the current observed at the end of the step is plotted against the voltage step. Conopressin G decreased membrane current at all potentials. The current-voltage relationship for the conopressin-G-sensitive current was obtained by subtracting the plots in the presence of conopressin G from those in the presence of ASW (Fig. 6C) and it usually consisted of two components; an outward current that is approximately linear at potentials below about − 50 mV and an outward rectifying component above − 45 mV. The voltage-sensitive component of the conopressin G effect was isolated by subtracting a straight line fitted to the lower portion of the current-voltage relationship from the total net current (Fig. 6C).

A tail current analysis was performed (Fig. 7), as described in the Materials and methods section, after the extracellular concentrations of a number of ions had been varied in order to determine their ability to affect the reversal potential of the outward rectifying current. Almost complete substitution of extracellular sodium with N-methyl glucamine (three cells, not shown) or partial substitution of extracellular chloride with methane sulfonate (three cells, not shown) had little or no effect on the extrapolated reversal potential. In contrast, increasing the concentration of extracellular potassium (three cells) produced a progressive reduction in the tail current amplitudes for a given membrane potential and a shift in the reversal potential in the positive direction. As shown in Fig. 7B, a slope of about 45–50mV in the reversal potential occurred per 10-fold change in [K+]o; this is close to that predicted by the Nernst equation for a K+-selective conductance, strongly suggesting that the slow, non-inactivating outward current produced by depolarizing command steps to subthreshold membrane potentials is carried mainly by potassium.

Fig. 7.

Tail current analysis of the outward membrane current produced by subthreshold depolarizing voltage commands. (A) From a holding potential of − 40 mV, a 270 ms prepulse to − 20 mV was applied to activate the current; this was followed by hyperpolarizing pulses between − 40 and − 80 mV. Tail currents obtained in this manner were fitted by exponential equations and the tail amplitude was extrapolated to the beginning of the pulse (see Materials and methods for details). (B) The extrapolated reversal potential plotted against the logarithm of extracellular potassium concentration (in mmol I−1). The slopes of three individual experiments shown in the graph were 45.6, 45.8 and 51.3mV per 10-fold increased in [K+]o. This strongly suggests that under these conditions the outward membrane current is largely carried by potassium.

Fig. 7.

Tail current analysis of the outward membrane current produced by subthreshold depolarizing voltage commands. (A) From a holding potential of − 40 mV, a 270 ms prepulse to − 20 mV was applied to activate the current; this was followed by hyperpolarizing pulses between − 40 and − 80 mV. Tail currents obtained in this manner were fitted by exponential equations and the tail amplitude was extrapolated to the beginning of the pulse (see Materials and methods for details). (B) The extrapolated reversal potential plotted against the logarithm of extracellular potassium concentration (in mmol I−1). The slopes of three individual experiments shown in the graph were 45.6, 45.8 and 51.3mV per 10-fold increased in [K+]o. This strongly suggests that under these conditions the outward membrane current is largely carried by potassium.

Reduction of the outward potassium current by conopressin G could be due either to a suppression of the outward potassium current or to an increase in an antagonistic inward current. To investigate this, conopressin G was tested after the outward potassium current had been blocked by internal perfusion of cesium (2 mol l−1 CsCl in the electrode, Fig. 8). In the first part of the experiment, diffusion of cesium into the cell was reduced by injecting repeated hyperpolarizing current pulses to retard the leakage of Cs+ from the electrode into the cell long enough to ascertain that the cell was responsive to the peptide. Although some leakage of cesium may have taken place, the top tracings in Fig. 8 show that conopressin G was still effective and able to reduce the outward current. The current could be partially restored after washing (not shown). Next, cesium was allowed to diffuse or was actively injected into the cell until the reduced magnitude of the outward current reached a constant level (10–25% after approximately 30min). Under these conditions, the effect of conopressin G was greatly reduced or practically absent, as shown in the bottom tracings in Fig. 8 (representative of five cells).

Fig. 8.

Representative recordings showing that ionophoretic injection of CsCl blocks the conopressin-G-sensitive K+ current. Cells were impaled with electrodes containing 2 mol l−1 CsCl. From a holding potential of − 40 mV, membrane currents were activated by command pulses up to − 20 mV in 5 mV steps. In the first part of the experiment, the cell was kept hyperpolarized between trials to prevent leakage of Cs+ from the electrode in order to obtain a control for the effect of conopressin G (upper traces). Next, Cs+ was injected into the cell by applying 2–3 nA depolarizing direct current. Conopressin G had little or no effect on the remaining outward current after most of the membrane current had been blocked by Cs+ (lower traces).

Fig. 8.

Representative recordings showing that ionophoretic injection of CsCl blocks the conopressin-G-sensitive K+ current. Cells were impaled with electrodes containing 2 mol l−1 CsCl. From a holding potential of − 40 mV, membrane currents were activated by command pulses up to − 20 mV in 5 mV steps. In the first part of the experiment, the cell was kept hyperpolarized between trials to prevent leakage of Cs+ from the electrode in order to obtain a control for the effect of conopressin G (upper traces). Next, Cs+ was injected into the cell by applying 2–3 nA depolarizing direct current. Conopressin G had little or no effect on the remaining outward current after most of the membrane current had been blocked by Cs+ (lower traces).

Characterization of the conopressin-G-sensitive potassium current

Known potassium currents in molluscs can be characterized by their voltage sensitivity and kinetics as well as by their sensitivity to TEA+ and 4-AP. Both TEA+ and 4-AP reduced the outward potassium current in response to voltage steps from −40 to −20mV (not shown). The conopressin-G-sensitive current is highly resistant to TEA+ and moderately resistant to 4-AP. The dose-response curve (Fig. 9) shows an apparent dissociation constant (Kd) of about 235 mmol l−1 for TEA+ (filled triangles, four cells) and 11 mmol I−1 for 4-AP (filled squares, three cells).

Fig. 9.

Block of the outward K+ current by 4-AP (three cells) and TEA+ (four cells). Membrane currents were activated by voltage commands from −40 to −20 mV in ASW, followed by exposure to increasing concentrations of both blockers, and are given as a percentage of the control current. Continuous lines were obtained using the expression: percentage current=100/(l+[blocker]/Cd) for the binding of a single molecule. The apparent dissociation constant values (Kd) was 11 mmol I−1 for 4-AP and 235mmoll−1 for TEA+. Values are mean+s.D.

Fig. 9.

Block of the outward K+ current by 4-AP (three cells) and TEA+ (four cells). Membrane currents were activated by voltage commands from −40 to −20 mV in ASW, followed by exposure to increasing concentrations of both blockers, and are given as a percentage of the control current. Continuous lines were obtained using the expression: percentage current=100/(l+[blocker]/Cd) for the binding of a single molecule. The apparent dissociation constant values (Kd) was 11 mmol I−1 for 4-AP and 235mmoll−1 for TEA+. Values are mean+s.D.

The similarities between the effects of conopressin G and serotonin on the electrical properties of the sensory neurons suggested that they could be acting through common mechanisms. This possibility was explored by applying them alone and together. The effects on the outward potassium current of serotonin and conopressin G were not additive (Fig. 10). In six cells in which the effects of both agents were tested, the outward potassium current produced by depolarization steps to −20 mV was reduced by 53% by 1 μmol l−1 conopressin G and by 46% by 50μmol I−1 serotonin. The combined action of conopressin G and serotonin, however, caused only 48% suppression of the current. If conopressin G and serotonin were acting via a serotoninergic interneuron then conopressin superfusion should have no effect when serotonin was used. Morever, two serotonin antagonists, ritanserine and spiperone, tested in this study (N=3) failed to block the action of serotonin and were not tested against conopressin G.

Fig. 10.

Co-perfusion of serotonin and conopressin G produces non-additive suppression of the K+ current. Representative data from a single experiment showing the current suppression caused by conopressin G (1μmoll−1) and serotonin (50μmol l−1) when applied separately or in combination. Notice that the current reduction resulting from co-application of both substances was not larger than that produced by either substance alone. Voltage pulses from −40 mV in 5 mV steps.

Fig. 10.

Co-perfusion of serotonin and conopressin G produces non-additive suppression of the K+ current. Representative data from a single experiment showing the current suppression caused by conopressin G (1μmoll−1) and serotonin (50μmol l−1) when applied separately or in combination. Notice that the current reduction resulting from co-application of both substances was not larger than that produced by either substance alone. Voltage pulses from −40 mV in 5 mV steps.

The dependence of the outward potassium current on Ca2+ was tested by replacing Ca2+ in the extracellular solution with Co2+ or Ba2+. Exposure of the sensory neurons to Ca2+-substituted ASW caused a slight membrane depolarization, reduction of firing threshold (not shown) and increased excitability during long depolarizing current injections (N=3, Fig. 11A). The outward potassium current was also inhibited by replacing Ca2+ with Ba2+ (three cells, not shown) or Co2+ (four cells, Fig. 11B). The effect of Co2+ on the quasi-steady current-voltage relationship is shown in Fig. 11C. As can be seen, there was a reduction in the outward potassium current.

Fig. 11.

Substitution of Ca2+ with Co2+ mimics the effect of conopressin G on Aplysia californica sensory neurons. Replacement of Ca2+ with Co2+ (10 mmol 1−1) caused (A) a membrane depolarization and a large decrement in spike frequency adaptation and (B) a substantial suppression of the outward membrane current in response to depolarizing voltage steps from a holding potential of −40 mV. (C) Quasi steady-state current-voltage plot of the cell represented in B.

Fig. 11.

Substitution of Ca2+ with Co2+ mimics the effect of conopressin G on Aplysia californica sensory neurons. Replacement of Ca2+ with Co2+ (10 mmol 1−1) caused (A) a membrane depolarization and a large decrement in spike frequency adaptation and (B) a substantial suppression of the outward membrane current in response to depolarizing voltage steps from a holding potential of −40 mV. (C) Quasi steady-state current-voltage plot of the cell represented in B.

Conopressin G was the only peptide of the vasopressin family tested that was able to increase the excitability and prolong the duration of the action potentials in Aplysia californica siphon sensory neurons. These included conopressin S, which is derived from a different species, Conus striatus. The effects of other vasopressin analogues on synaptic transmission were not systematically investigated. Arginine vasotocin, however, has been reported to narrow the sensory neuron action potential slightly during FDSB and to depress synaptic transmission (Goldberg et al. 1987). In our experiments, arginine vasotocin was as likely to produce a slight potentiation as a slight reduction of FDSB, although the number of experiments was insufficient to determine whether there is a trend in either direction. Its effects, in any case, were considerably smaller than those of conopressin G, even though 10 times higher concentrations were used.

The effects of conopressin G on siphon sensory neurons more closely resemble those of serotonin and SCPb than those of arginine vasotocin. First, like serotonin and SCPb (Klein et al. 1986), conopressin G increased the somatic excitability of sensory neurons by decreasing the threshold current needed to elicit action potentials in the soma and by reducing accommodation. This strong effect did not alter the number of action potentials appearing in the sensory cell soma in response to mechanical stimulation of its receptive field in the siphon, again like serotonin (Klein et al. 1986). These studies employed short, moderately intense mechanical stimulation of the siphon and it remains to be determined whether the observed increase in the soma excitability is effective during persistent or noxious (e.g. an intense stimulus such as a pinch) stimulation.

Second, conopressin G, like serotonin, enhanced frequency-dependent broadening of the action potential but, unlike serotonin, broadening of single action potentials by conopressin G was not usually apparent in recordings from the cell body when single action potentials were elicited at 5 min intervals. It is not known whether the peptide increases the duration of the action potentials at the terminal and, if so, whether this effect is related to its ability to facilitate synaptic transmission.

Third, conopressin G, like serotonin, facilitated synaptic transmission between sensory neurons and motor neurons. Further studies, including quantal analysis, are necessary to determine whether the enhancement of the monosynaptic EPSP is presynaptic. However, potentiation of the EPSP would be expected on the basis of the ability of the peptide to reduce the potassium current and, consequently, to prolong the action potential. During synaptic facilitation due to action potential broadening, which is an important contributor to serotonin-induced synaptic facilitation, there is no change in the rate of rise of the EPSP (Hochner et al. 1986a). Synaptic facilitation without an increased rate of rise of the EPSP was usually observed in this study (Fig. 4A), but synaptic facilitation induced by conopressin G was sometimes accompanied by an increase in the rate of rise of the EPSP. This, and the fact that conopressin G was able to facilitate the EPSP after homosynaptic depression (Fig. 5), a condition under which broadening of the action potential has little effect (Hochner et al. 1986b), suggests that, like serotonin (Dale and Kandel, 1990), conopressin G may affect additional processes contributing to facilitation of this synapse.

Fourth, the effects of serotonin and conopressin G on membrane currents in Aplysia californica sensory cells are very similar with respect to their magnitude, kinetics and sensitivity to pharmacological agents. Under voltage-clamp conditions, nanomolar concentrations of conopressin G, like serotonin, suppress a voltage-dependent outward membrane current that is activated by depolarizing voltage steps to -20 mV and which tail current analysis indicates is carried primarily by potassium and shows little or no inactivation over time. This potassium current is not the early potassium current (IA) because steady-state inactivation of IA is almost complete at −40 mV (Connor and Stevens, 1971; Neher, 1971), the holding potential used in these experiments. This potassium current is not likely to be the delayed potassium current (IKV) either. This current is activated by depolarizing steps above −30 mV, with maximum currents occurring between 0 and 10 mV (Aldrich et al. 1979a; Thompson, 1977). The delayed potassium current takes about 100ms to reach its peak, after which it deactivates to a non-zero steady-state value (Aldrich et al. 1919a,b). This is different from the conopressin-G- and serotonin-sensitive currents, which show no apparent inactivation over time. Also, Baxter and Byrne (1989) have shown that depolarizing pulses to −20 mV do not significantly activate IKV in Aplysia californica sensory neurons. The relative insensitivity of the conopressin-G-sensitive potassium current to blockade by either TEA+ or 4-AP also supports the conclusion that it is not IKv. Externally applied TEA+ blocks IKV with a Kd of about 6–8 mmol l−1 (Baxter and Byrne, 1989; Hermann and Gorman, 1981a) and externally applied 4-AP blocks IKv at a concentration of around 2 mmol I−1 (Hermann and Gorman, 1981a). These values are much less than the Kd for blockade by TEA+ (235 mmol l−1) or 4-AP (llmmoll−1) reported here and are close to the concentrations of these drugs necessary to block Is, the serotoninsensitive potassium current (Kd for TEA+ is about 100 mmol I−1, Baxter and Byrne, 1989; Brezina et al. 1987; Shuster and Siegelbaum, 1987; Kd for 4-AP is about 7mmoll−1, Brezina et al. 1987; Brezina, 1988).

It is difficult to interpret the observation that the conopressin-G-sensitive outward potassium current was greatly suppressed when extracellular calcium was replaced by divalent cations (Ba2+ and Co2+) that prevent calcium entry into the cell. First, it is not possible to assess the extent to which calcium replacement interferes with the conopressin-G-induced effects since calcium replacement impairs synaptic transmission. Conopressin G had no effect when synaptic transmission was impaired. Nevertheless, the similarity of the actions of these divalent cations suggests that they suppress the same potassium current as conopressin G. Like exposure to conopressin G, exposure of the cell to Ca2+-substituted ASW caused slight membrane depolarization and a large increase in sensory neuron excitability (Fig. 11). An alternative explanation, that barium and cobalt may induce activation of an inward current, seems unlikely. Although calcium channels are more permeable to Ba2+ than to Ca2+ (Hagiwara et al. 1974), the observation that similar effects were obtained with cobalt, which does not permeate the channel, argues against this possibility. Thus, it appears that the conopressin-G-sensitive current is a calcium-sensitive potassium current.

Since the effects of conopressin G and serotonin on membrane currents are almost indistinguishable and since serotonin can occlude the effect of conopressin G on membrane current; it is necessary to consider the possibility that they both suppress the same current. Consequently, it is necessary to compare the observed effects of conopressin G with published descriptions of Is.

Although the voltage- and time-dependence of the conopressin-G-sensitive potassium current do not conform to the theoretically expected behavior of Is, based on single-channel analysis of the S current, they do compare favorably with actual descriptions of macroscopic whole-cell Is in the literature. Patch-clamp studies show that the serotonin-sensitive S channel remains open most of the time over the entire physiological range of membrane potentials (Siegelbaum, 1987; Camardo and Siegelbaum, 1983) and is only weakly affected by membrane potential (Brezina et al. 1987; Siegelbaum et al. 1982). This suggests that, during a depolarizing voltage step, current will flow instantaneously through the majority of the S channels, which are open at the holding potential, and only a small proportion of the remaining closed channels will subsequently open at the new voltage. This predicted time-independent, outward-rectifying leakage is in stark contrast to the slow activation and marked voltage-dependence of the serotoninsensitive current observed below -20 mV in Aplysia californica pleural and abdominal sensory neurons, which are called Is and attributed to the S channel in the literature (Baxter and Byrne, 1989; Klein and Kandel, 1980; Klein et al. 1982), but it is very similar to the properties of the current we describe here.

The literature concerning the sensitivity of serotonin-sensitive potassium currents to calcium is less than clear. Suppression of IC by serotonin in molluscan neurons has been reported (Paupardin-Tritsch et al. 1981), and even in Aplysia californica pleural sensory neurons, where serotonin modulates a steady-state Ca2+-activated potassium current (Walsh and Byrne, 1989). This form of Ic is sensitive to 5 mmol I−1 extracellular TEA+, whereas the conopressin-G-sensitive current in abdominal sensory cells is highly resistant to TEA+. Given the similar electrical properties of pleural and siphon sensory neurons, it may seem unlikely that this discrepancy could be due to differences in cell types but the possibility cannot be dismissed since it has been reported that serotonin reduces the outward current produced by Ca2+ injections into pleural (Walsh and Byrne, 1989) but not abdominal sensory neurons (Klein et al. 1982).

Initial reports on the responses of siphon sensory neurons to serotonin emphasized that replacement of Ca2+ with Ba2+ did not affect serotonin suppression of the membrane current activated by 25 ms depolarizing command pulses from −50 mV (Klein et al. 1982; Pollock et al. 1985), a protocol unsuitable for detecting a slowly activating current. These same authors mention in passing that calcium channel blockers, such as cobalt and cadmium, attenuate the serotonin response but they do not specify the conditions under which this significant observation was made. The slowly activating serotonin- and conopres-sin-G-sensitive current we describe here would have contributed to the serotonin effect observed during the long step pulses used by Klein et al. (1982) and the ramps used by Pollock et al. (1985). The reduction of this current following calcium substitution would have been significant and might account for their incompletely described results.

Taken together, the results presented here indicate that conopressin G and serotonin suppress a slowly activating, non-inactivating outward rectifying potassium current that is present at the resting membrane potential. This current might be a species of Ic, which, unlike other Ca2+-dependent potassium currents, is insensitive to TEA+. Alternatively, suppression of the current by a Ba2+- and Co2+-substituted solution might be due to a requirement for Ca2+ on the extracellular side or to a more effective screening of membrane charges by Co2+ and Ba2+, resulting in a voltage shift of channel activation towards more positive potentials. Further experimentation is needed to investigate this. Given these assumptions, the voltage-independent, linear component of the effect of conopressin G may represent suppression of the true Is.

Several lines of evidence suggest that the action of conopressin G on siphon sensory neurons is not directly mediated, but that it requires one or more intermediate neurons. First, the effects of conopressin G require more than 1 min to develop. This is in contrast to the rapid onset of the effects of serotonin and SCPb, which are known to act directly on the sensory cells (Abrams et al. 1984; Baxter and Byrne, 1990). In addition, blocking synaptic transmission in the ganglion with a low-Ca2+, high-Mg2+ solution prevents the effect of conopressin G on accommodation, whereas it does not prevent the effect of serotonin and SCPb. In a similar manner, synaptic facilitation produced by conopressin G was blocked by bathing the abdominal ganglion in high-Ca2+, high-Mg2+ solutions, a procedure that would reduce the excitability of putative interneurons whose activation by conopressin G might ultimately lead to the effect of the peptide on synaptic transmission. If the effects of conopressin G are indeed mediated through an intermediate cell as suggested, there is the possibility that this cell releases serotonin or another facilitatory substance such as SCPb. Unfortunately, two serotonin inhibitors, ritanserine and spiperone, did not block the effects of serotonin itself and, therefore, it was not possible to investigate this further.

Regardless of its exact identity, because of its slow activation kinetics the voltage-dependent component may not contribute to the broadening of the action potential, which occurs within 10 ms. The activation kinetics of this current, however, may be much faster at more depolarized potentials. Suppression of this current would be expected to contribute to the increased excitability and, because it is active at rest, to the membrane depolarization caused by conopressin G in current-clamp conditions.

The effects of conopressin G and serotonin are different in one very important respect: despite its facilitatory effect on the sensory neuron to motor neuron synapse, conopressin G suppresses the gill withdrawal reflex (Martínez-Padrón et al. 1992). In semi-intact preparations, the superfusion of conopressin G over the abdominal ganglion suppresses gill withdrawal reflex behavior. That is, not only does the observed synaptic facilitation fail to enhance the reflex but also the reflex is suppressed in spite of it. This clearly indicates that facilitation of the sensory neuron to motor neuron synapse is not sufficient to mediate behavioral sensitization. Since the central sensory neuron to motor neuron synapse is assumed to be a neural correlate of the gill withdrawal reflex, we tested the hypothesis that behavioral suppression may be accompanied by synaptic depression at this synapse. This proved not to be the case, since conopressin G caused potentiation of FDSB, facilitation of the sensory neuron to motor neuron synapse, and dramatically increased the excitability of the sensory neurons. Therefore, it appears that, in this case, the behavioral and synaptic changes caused by conopressin G are not correlated. A similar lack of correlation between events at these two levels of organization has been reported during associative learning (Colebrook and Lukowiak, 1988).

The function of FDSB is not known, but its effects on the action potential are likely to be substantial during persistent or noxious stimulation when the cell fires prolonged discharges. Obvious broadening of the action potential is observed in the presence of conopressin G when neurons fire long, self-regenerating trains that outlast the stimulation. The potentiation of FDSB by conopressin G may have an even more dramatic effect on the synaptic output of the sensory cells than the one described here for single action potentials. It is relevant to note that a nociceptive function has been attributed to pleural and, presumably, to siphon sensory neurons (Walters et al. 1983) and site-specific sensitization of the tail withdrawal reflex using noxious electrical stimulation of the skin causes activity-dependent changes in pleural sensory neurons similar to those described here for siphon mechanoreceptors (Walters, 1987). These changes include lowering of the spike threshold, enhanced synaptic transmission and, in some cases, the appearance of regenerative bursts during short-duration stimulation. This last characteristic was occasionally observed in the presence of conopressin G.

Interestingly, there is evidence that the noxious or aversive stimulation traditionally used to induce sensitization of the Aplysia californica GWR also causes behavioral inhibition (Mackey et al. 1987; Marcus et al. 1988). Both neurally mediated processes are triggered simultaneously by tail shock but the rapidly decaying inhibition is initially strong enough to mask sensitization so that inhibition is only apparent briefly before sensitization predominates (Mackey et al. 1987). In addition, Krontiris-Litowitz et al. (1989) found that the GWR can be suppressed by hemolymph from animals subjected to intense electrical or mechanical stimulation and by unidentified factors released from traumatized body wall. Therefore, noxious stimulation seems to generate both neural and humoral signals that suppress defensive reflexes.

In this context, it may be possible to suggest a role for conopressin G in aversive stimulus-induced behavioral inhibition and subsequent sensitization. Conopressin G might cause suppression at some sites in the network (Martínez-Padrón et al. 1992) and facilitation at others (perhaps by causing release of serotonin), with the suppressive process being stronger, but shorter-lived. These effects include a suppression of the ability of the gill motor neurons to elicit gill contractions and a decrease in the excitability of the motor neuron itself. Termination of such suppression would allow sensitization to be expressed. Detailed experimentation on the parametric characteristics of the behavioral effects of conopressin G will be necessary to address this possibility. Contrary to this idea, however, is the observation that tail shock produces narrowing of the sensory neuron action potential and heterosynaptic depression (Mackey et al. 1987). However, this latter effect could be produced by noxious stimulation through a conopressin-independent mechanism that helps to ensure the initial pre-eminence of behavioral inhibition. Therefore, it would be interesting to see whether FMRFamide, which has been linked to synaptic depression during behavioral inhibition (Abrams et al. 1984; Mackey et al. 1987), can counteract the effects of conopressin G.

M.M.-P. is the recipient of a grant from the Provincial Government of the Canary Islands. We would like to thank Dr Wayne Giles, Dr Bryan MacVicar and Dr Steve Barnes for their helpful suggestions on earlier versions of the manuscript. This work was supported in part by a grant from the Medical Research Council of Canada.

Abrams
,
T. W.
,
Castellucci
,
V. F.
,
Camardo
,
J. S.
,
Kandel
,
E. R.
and
Lloyd
,
P. E.
(
1984
).
Two endogenous neuropeptides modulate the gill and siphon withdrawal reflex in Aplysia by presynaptic facilitation involving cAMP-dependent closure of a serotonin-sensitive potassium channel
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
81
,
7956
7960
.
Aldrich
,
R. W.
,
Getting
,
P. A.
and
Thompson
,
S. H.
(
1979a
).
Inactivation of delayed outward current in molluscan neurone somata
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
291
,
507
530
.
Aldrich
,
R. W.
,
Getting
,
P. A.
and
Thompson
,
S. H.
(
1979b
).
Mechanism of frequencydependent broadening of molluscan neurone soma spikes
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
291
,
531
544
.
Baxter
,
D. A.
and
Byrne
,
J. H.
(
1989
).
Serotonergic modulation of two potassium currents in the pleural sensory neurons of Aplysia
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
62
,
665
679
.
Baxter
,
D. A.
and
Byrne
,
J. H.
(
1990
).
Differential effects of cAMP and serotonin on membrane current, action-potential duration, and excitability in somata of pleural sensory neurons of Aplysia
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
64
,
978
990
.
Belardetr
,
F.
,
Kandel
,
E. R.
and
Siegelbaum
,
S. A.
(
1987
).
Neuronal inhibition by the peptide FRMFamide involves opening of S K+ channels
.
Nature
325
,
153
156
.
Brezina
,
V.
(
1988
).
Guanosine 5’-triphosphate analogue activates potassium current modulated by neurotransmitters in Aplysia neurones
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
407
,
15
40
.
Brezina
,
V.
,
Eckert
,
R.
and
Erxleben
,
C.
(
1987
).
Modulation of potassium conductances by an endogenous neuropeptide in neurones of Aplysia californica
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
382
,
267
290
.
Brunelli
,
M.
,
Castellucci
,
V.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1976
).
Synaptic facilitation and behavioral sensitization in Aplysia’. possible role of serotonin and cyclic AMP
.
Science
194
,
1178
1181
.
Byrne
,
J.
,
Castellucci
,
V.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1974
).
Receptive fields and response properties of mechanoreceptor neurons innervating siphon skin and mantle shelf in Aplysia
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
37
,
1041
1064
.
Camardo
,
J. S.
and
Siegelbaum
,
S. A.
(
1983
).
Single-channel analysis in Aplysia neurons: A specific K+ channel is modulated by serotonin and cyclic AMP
.
In Single Channel Recording
(ed.
B.
Sakmann
and
E.
Neher
), pp.
409
422
.
New York
:
Plenum Press
.
Castellucci
,
V.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1976
).
Presynaptic facilitation as a mechanism for behavioral sensitization in Aplysia
.
Science
194
,
1176
1178
.
Colebrook
,
E.
and
Lukowiak
,
K.
(
1988
).
Learning by the Aplysia model system: lack of correlation between gill and gill motor neurone responses
.
J. exp. Biol
.
135
,
411
429
.
Connor
,
J. A.
and
Stevens
,
C. F.
(
1971
).
Voltage clamp studies of a transient outward membrane current in gastropod neural somata
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
213
,
21
30
.
Cruz
,
L. J.
,
Desantes
,
V.
,
Zafaralla
,
G. C.
,
Ramilo
,
C. A.
,
Zeikus
,
R.
,
Gray
,
W. R.
and
Olivera
,
B. Mn.
(
1987
).
Invertebrate vasopressin/oxytocin homologs. Characterization of peptides from Conus geographus and Conus striatus venoms
.
J. biol. Chem
.
262
,
821
824
.
Dale
,
N.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1990
).
Facilitatory and inhibitory transmitters modulate spontaneous transmitter release at cultured Aplysia sensorimotor synapses
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
421
,
203
222
.
Edstrom
,
J. P.
and
Lukowiak
,
K.
(
1985
).
Frequency-dependent action potential prolongation in Aplysia pleural sensory neurones
.
Neuroscience
16
,
451
460
.
Frost
,
W. N.
,
Clark
,
G. A.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1988
).
Parallel processing of short-term memory for sensitization in Aplysia
.
J. Neurobiol
.
19
,
297
334
.
Goldberg
,
J.
,
Colmers
,
W.
,
Edstrom
,
J.
and
Lukowiak
,
K.
(
1987
).
Suppression of sensory to motor synaptic transmission and narrowing of the sensory neurone action potential by arginine-vasotocin in Aplysia californica
.
J. exp. Biol
.
128
,
47
62
.
Hagiwara
,
S.
,
Fukuda
,
J.
and
Eaton
,
D. C.
(
1974
).
Membrane currents carried by Ca2+, Sr2+ and Ba2+ in barnacle muscle fiber during voltage clamp
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
63
,
564
578
.
Hermann
,
A.
and
Gorman
,
A. L. F.
(
1981a
).
Effects of 4-aminopyridine on potassium currents in a molluscan neuron
.
J. gen. Physiol.
18
,
63
86
.
Hermann
,
A.
and
Gorman
,
A. L. F.
(
1981b
).
Effects of tetraethylammonium on potassium currents in a molluscan neuron
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
78
,
87
110
.
Hochner
,
B.
,
Klein
,
M.
,
Schacher
,
S.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1986a
).
Action potential duration and the modulation of transmitter release from the sensory neurons of Aplysia in presynaptic facilitation and behavioral sensitization
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
83
,
8410
8414
.
Hochner
,
B.
,
Klein
,
M.
,
Schacher
,
S.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1986b
).
Additional components in the cellular mechanism of presynaptic facilitation contributes to behavioral dishabituation in Aplysia
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
83
,
8794
8798
.
Ichinose
,
M.
,
Sawada
,
M.
,
Maeno
,
T.
and
McAdoo
,
D. J.
(
1989
).
Effect of acetylcholine on ventrocaudal sensory neurons in the pleural ganglia of Aplysia
.
Cell, molec. Neurobiol.
9
,
233
245
.
Jack
,
J. J. B.
,
Noble
,
D.
and
Tsien
,
R. W.
(
1975
).
Electric Current Flow in Excitable Cells
.
Oxford
:
Oxford University Press
.
Klein
,
M.
,
Camardo
,
J.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1982
).
Serotonin modulates a specific potassium current in the sensory neurons that show presynaptic facilitation in Aplysia
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
79
,
5713
5717
.
Klein
,
M.
,
Hochner
,
B.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1986
).
Facilitatory transmitters and cAMP can modulate accommodation as well as transmitter release in Aplysia sensory neurons: evidence for parallel processing in a single cell
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci
.
83
,
7994
7998
.
Klein
,
M.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1978
).
Presynaptic modulation of voltage-dependent Ca2+ current: mechanism for behavioral sensitization in Aplysia californica
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
75
,
3512
3516
.
Klein
,
M.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1980
).
Mechanism of calcium current modulation underlying presynaptic facilitation and behavioral sensitization in Aplysia
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
77
,
5713
5719
.
Koester
,
J.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1977
).
Further identification of neurons in the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia using behavioral criteria
.
Brain Res
.
121
,
1
20
.
Krontiris-Litowitz
,
J. K.
,
Cooper
,
B. F.
and
Walters
,
E. T.
(
1989
).
Humoral factors released during trauma of Aplysia body wall. 1. Body wall contraction, cardiac modulation, and central reflex suppression
.
J. comp. Physiol. B
159
,
211
223
.
Mackey
,
S. L.
,
Glanzman
,
D. L.
,
Small
,
S. A.
,
Dyke
,
A. M.
,
Kandel
,
E. R.
and
Hawkins
,
R. D.
(
1987
).
Tail shock produces inhibition as well as sensitization of the siphon-withdrawal reflex of Aplysia’. Possible behavioral role for presynaptic inhibition mediated by the peptide Phe-Met-Arg-Phe-NH2
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
84
,
8730
8734
.
Marcus
,
E. A.
,
Nolen
,
T. G.
,
Rankin
,
C. H.
and
Carew
,
T. G.
(
1988
).
Behavioral dissociation of dishabituation, sensitization and inhibition in Aplysia
.
Science
241
,
210
213
.
Martínez-Padrón
,
M.
,
Gray
,
W.
and
Lukowiak
,
K.
(
1992
).
Conopressin G, a molluscan vasopressin-like peptide, alters gill behaviors in Aplysia
.
Can. J. Pharmac. Physiol
.
70
,
259
267
.
McMaster
,
D.
,
Kobayashi
,
Y.
and
Lederis
,
K.
(
1989a
).
A peptide of the vasopressin-oxytocin family in Aplysia kurodai. HPLC and RIA characterization
.
Can. J. Physiol. Pharmac
.
67
,
Axxvi
.
McMaster
,
D.
,
Kobayashi
,
Y.
and
Lederis
,
K.
(
1989b
).
A conopressin-like peptide in Aplysia kurodai. HPLC and RIA characterization
.
32nd Annual Meeting of the Canadian Federation of Biological Societies
. Abstract 023.
Neher
,
E.
(
1971
).
Two fast transient current components during voltage clamp on snail neurons
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
58
,
36
53
.
Paupardin-Tritsch
,
D.
,
Deterre
,
P.
and
Gershenfeld
,
H. M.
(
1981
).
Relationship between two voltage-dependent serotonin responses of molluscan neurones
.
Brain Res
.
217
,
201
206
.
Peretz
,
B.
and
Lukowiak
,
K.
(
1975
).
Age-dependent CNS control of the habituating gill withdrawal reflex and correlated activity in identified neurons in Aplysia
.
J. comp Physiol
.
103
,
1
17
.
Pollock
,
J. D.
,
Bernier
,
L.
and
Camardo
,
J. S.
(
1985
).
Serotonin and cyclic adenosine 3′:5′-monophosphate modulate the potassium current in tail sensory neurons in the pleural ganglion of Aplysia
.
J. Neurosci
.
5
,
1862
1871
.
Shuster
,
M. J.
and
Siegelbaum
,
S. A.
(
1987
).
Pharmacological characterization of the serotonin-sensitive potassium channel of Aplysia sensory neurons
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
90
,
587
608
.
Siegelbaum
,
S. A.
(
1987
).
The S-current: a background potassium current
.
In Neuromodulation
(ed.
L. K.
Kaczmarek
and
I. B.
Levitan
), pp.
187
205
.
New York
:
Oxford University Press
.
Siegelbaum
,
S. A.
,
Camardo
,
J. S.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1982
).
Serotonin and cyclic AMP close single K+ channels in Aplysia sensory neurones
.
Nature
299
,
413
417
.
Thompson
,
S. H.
(
1977
).
Three pharmacologically distinct potassium channels in molluscan neurones
.
J. Physiol. Lond
.
265
,
465
488
.
Thornhill
,
J. A.
,
Lukowiak
,
K.
,
Cooper
,
K. E.
,
Veale
,
W. L.
and
Edstrom
,
J. P.
(
1981
).
Arginine vasotocin, an endogenous neuropeptide of Aplysia, suppresses the gill withdrawal reflex and reduces the evoked synaptic input to central gill motor neurons
.
J. Neurobiol
.
12
,
533
544
.
Walsh
,
J. P.
and
Byrne
,
J. H.
(
1989
).
Modulation of a steady-state Ca2+-activated K+ current in tail sensory neurons of Aplysia’. Role of serotonin and cAMP
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
61
,
32
44
.
Walters
,
E. T.
(
1987
).
Multiple sensory neuronal correlates of site-specific sensitization in Aplysia
.
J. Neurosci
.
7
,
408
417
.
Walters
,
E. T.
,
Byrne
,
J. H.
,
Carew
,
T. J.
and
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1983
).
Mechanoafferent neurons innervating tail of Aplysia. I. Response properties and synaptic connections
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
50
,
1522
1542
.