Mutations that affect the physiological properties of neurones or the development of neuronal circuits are likely to have profound effects on sensory and motor pathways. We have examined the effects of mutations in two loci -the Shaker complex and shaking-B -on the taste pathway of Drosophila melanogaster Meigen at the behavioural and electrophysiological levels. The Shaker locus encodes a variety of A-type potassium channels that are likely to be tissue-and stage-specific. Flies containing the different Shaker alleles examined exhibit a variety of defects in their gustatory responses to sucrose, NaCl and KCl. The firing patterns of the labellar chemosensory neurones in response to these stimuli are normal. This suggests that the channels encoded by the Shaker locus are probably not involved in taste transduction, but affect central gustatory circuits. The shaking-B locus affects neuronal connectivity, though its molecular nature is unknown. Mutants at this locus show increased thresholds for detection of sucrose and fructose and lack the attraction response to 0.1 mol 1-1 sodium chloride that is exhibited by the wild-type fly.

An animal perceives the external world through its sensory systems. These transduce external stimuli into electrical information, which is further processed and decoded, leading to a behavioural response. The processing of sensory information involves several steps: stimulus recognition, signal transduction, integration and motor output. The gustatory pathway of Drosophila melanogaster is a promising system in which these processes may be analysed at the molecular level.

The fly detects taste stimuli by means of external sensilla located on its proboscis and tarsi (Dethier, 1976). A labellar sensillum in Drosophila typically contains four functionally distinct chemosensory neurones: an S neurone, which responds to sugars; LI and L2, which are salt-sensitive; and a W or water neurone (Falk et al. 1976; Rodrigues and Siddiqi, 1978; Fujishiro et al. 1984). In Drosophila and other dipterans, these neurones project to the suboesophageal ganglion (Nayak and Singh, 1985).

The genetic approach to the analysis of gustation involves the isolation and study of single-gene mutations with the aim of pinpointing molecules important for the development and function of the pathway. A number of mutants showing defective responses to taste stimuli have been described (Isono and Kikuchi, 1974; Falk and Atidia, 1975; Rodrigues and Siddiqi, 1978; Tompkins et al. 1979; Tanimura et al. 1982; Morea, 1985). A complementary approach is to study the function of identified neuronal molecules in the gustatory pathway.

In recent years, several genes that affect nervous system function or development have been studied and their products analysed. The molecules that they encode include channel proteins, for example Shaker (Iverson et al. 1988; Pongs et al. 1988; Timpe et al. 1988), potential nuclear regulatory factors, such as daughterless (Caudy et al. 1988), hairy (Rushlow et al. 1989) and achaete-scute (Ghysen & Dambly-Chaudiere, 1989), transmembrane proteins, such as Notch (Wharton et al. 1985) and Delta (WVässin et al. 1987), and synaptic molecules, such as ard (Hermans-Borgmeyer et al. 1986). These molecules are often expressed in a wide range of tissues, and mutations in genes coding for them are likely to have pleiotropic effects, daughterless is a well-known example of a gene that plays a role in sex determination and peripheral neurogenesis (Cline, 1989).

There are fewer examples of molecules specifying neuronal connectivity. The mutations disconnected (Steller et al. 1987), bendless and Passover (Wyman and Thomas, 1983) presumably affect genes that encode molecules involved in connectivity.

An analysis of the effects of neurogenic mutations on the function of diverse sensory pathways may provide interesting information on how neural systems are built. The magnitude and specificity of these effects will depend on the nature of the mutation and the requirement of the gene product in the specific circuit. As a first step in this direction, we have examined the effects of mutations in two loci, Shaker and Passover (shaking-B), on the gustatory responses of the fly. The behavioural defects observed are viewed in the light of our current understanding of the function of gustatory circuits in dipterans.

Mutations in the Shaker locus affect neuronal excitability, action potential frequency and repolarization, and synaptic transmission (Jan et al. 1977; Tanouye et al. 1981). These mutations affect a fast, transient outward potassium current, IA (Salkoff and Wyman, 1983). The Shaker locus has been cloned and shown to be the structural gene for the A-subtype of potassium channel (Iversen et al. 1988; Timpe et al. 1988b). The locus is complex, encoding at least 21 different transcripts (Kamb et al. 1988). Of these, five that have been analysed by in vitro transcription in Xenopus oocytes give rise to kinetically distinct populations of A-type potassium channels (Timpe et al. 1988a).

Mutations giving rise to abnormalities in ion channels could affect gustatory behaviour by altering the peripheral detectors, higher-order neurones or both. There is some evidence for the involvement of potassium channels in gustatory transduction in vertebrates. A voltage-insensitive cyclic-AMP-modulated channel has been implicated in sucrose reception and a voltage-sensitive channel appears to be involved in KCl transduction (Avenet et al. 1988; Kinnamon and Roper, 1988).

The properties of neuronal circuits are largely dictated by the cellular localization of channels with different properties. The IA current is known to play an important role in the modulation of neuronal signals in different circuits (Getting, 1983). The spatial and temporal patterns of expression of the Shaker transcripts are therefore likely to have profound effects on the processing of sensory information.

Passover (Pas) is an allele of a previously isolated mutation shaking-B2 (Homyk et al. 1980; Baird, 1986). This locus will therefore be referred to as shaking-B (shak-B) (J. Davies, personal communication). Mutations in the shaking-B locus cause a defect in the visually elicited jump response of Drosophila melanogaster (Wyman and Thomas, 1983). This response is mediated by a pair of neurones, the giant fibre neurones. The two giant fibres descend from the brain to the thoracic ganglion; there the giant fibre of each side synapses with two neurones -the tergotrochanteral motor neurone (TTMN) and the peripherally synapsing interneurone (PSI). The TTMN drives the TTM or jump muscle, resulting in extension of the mesothoracic leg during jumping. The PSI synapses with the motor neurones innervating the dorsal longitudinal muscles (DLMs) or wing depressors. A sudden light-off stimulus causes the giant fibre (GF) to fire, driving the TTMs and the DLMs, resulting in jumping and flight initiation (Wyman and Thomas, 1983). In Pas and shak-B strains, the synapse between the GF and TTMN does not form. In addition, the synapses in the GF-PSI-DLMN part of the pathway do not function normally. Our analysis of the gustatory responses of Pas and shaking-B2 strains raises the possibility that this locus is also involved in specifying connectivity in the taste pathway of Drosophila melanogaster.

Strains

The genotypes and sources of the strains used in this study are listed in Table 1. Drosophila cultures were maintained on standard corn meal/yeast medium at 23-25 °C.

Table 1.

List of strains used and their sources

List of strains used and their sources
List of strains used and their sources

Chemicals

Sucrose, fructose and sodium chloride were obtained from Sigma, St Louis, MO; potassium chloride was from BDH, India Ltd; agar was from SISCO, Bombay, India, and Carmoisine Red from Anand Dyes and Co., Ltd, Bombay, India. All solutions were made in double distilled water.

The feeding preference test

The test designed by Tanimura et al. (1982) was used with minor modifications. Flies to be tested were reared in uncrowded culture bottles at 25±1°C. After emergence, they were placed in fresh culture bottles and allowed to age for 2-3 days. They were then transferred to flasks containing filter paper soaked in distilled water and starved for 18 h. Testing was carried out in 6x 10-well microtitre plates (Laxbro, Pune, India) with a well capacity of approximately 0.1ml. The response to both attractants and repellents can be assayed in this test. When the response to attractants was being assayed, the stimulus was prepared in 1 % agar and placed in alternate wells of the plate. The remaining wells were filled with 1 % agar containing 0.2% of the food dye Carmoisine Red. For repellents, the stimulus was prepared in agar that also contained the food dye.

Approximately 100 flies were introduced into each plate by means of a hole in the lid. At no stage were the flies anaesthetized in any way. The plates were left undisturbed in the dark at 25°C for 1 h. The flies were scored for the red colour in their abdomens by inspection under a dissection microscope. The response was calculated from the fraction of flies feeding from the stimulus-containing wells. Thus, percentage attraction=number of uncoloured flies/total number of flies; and percentage tolerance=number of coloured flies/total number of flies.

Whenever the response of mutants was being assayed, the wild type was tested in parallel. Means and standard deviations were calculated from data from several independent plates run with different batches of flies on different days. The calculation of acceptance and repulsion responses is affected by the population of ‘non-eaters’, which appear uncoloured. The number of non-eaters would be expected to vary depending on the concentration of the stimulus being tested. To obtain an estimate of this, a control was carried out at each concentration wherein all wells contained coloured agar, but alternate wells contained stimulus as well.

The proportion of uncoloured flies in these tests indicates the fraction of non-eaters. This proportion has been found to decrease from 20-25 % for no stimulus to 0% at high concentrations (refer to Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Gustatory responses of Canton S (CS, □) and Oregon R (OR, ♦) strains. Responses to sucrose (A), fructose (B) and NaCl (C and D) were assayed in the feeding preference test. Acceptance responses to NaCl (C) were measured with the stimulus in the uncoloured wells. A blank plate (B) was run where no stimulus was added. For assay of tolerance (D), the salt was added to the wells that also contained the food dye. Tolerance data for males and females are presented separately. In all other tests, there was no sexual dimorphism of the response. The dashed line in A indicates the number of flies that fail to eat and therefore appear uncoloured. To measure non-eaters, all wells were filled with food dye and only alternate wells contained the stimulus. All values are the means and standard deviations of at least 10 readings.

Fig. 1.

Gustatory responses of Canton S (CS, □) and Oregon R (OR, ♦) strains. Responses to sucrose (A), fructose (B) and NaCl (C and D) were assayed in the feeding preference test. Acceptance responses to NaCl (C) were measured with the stimulus in the uncoloured wells. A blank plate (B) was run where no stimulus was added. For assay of tolerance (D), the salt was added to the wells that also contained the food dye. Tolerance data for males and females are presented separately. In all other tests, there was no sexual dimorphism of the response. The dashed line in A indicates the number of flies that fail to eat and therefore appear uncoloured. To measure non-eaters, all wells were filled with food dye and only alternate wells contained the stimulus. All values are the means and standard deviations of at least 10 readings.

Electrophysiological recordings from the labellar chemoreceptors

The tip recording method of Hodgson et al. (1955) was modified for use in Drosophila (Rodrigues and Siddiqi, 1978; Fujishiro et al. 1984; Arora et al. 1987). Three-to four-day-old flies were immobilized by cooling and introduced into plastic micropipette tips (Gilson P-200). The tapered ends of the tips were cut to hold the fly snugly at the thorax, allowing only the head to protrude. The head was held in place using myristic acid wax (melting point 58.5°C). The proboscis was immobilized by a minute drop of wax applied at the haustellum. The wax was applied with a tungsten wire loop whose temperature could be controlled by varying the current passed through the wire. The flies were allowed 15 min to recover from the treatment.

A Ag/AgCl indifferent electrode filled with Drosophila saline was introduced into the thorax of the fly. The recording electrode was a tapered glass capillary connected to a Ag/AgCl electrode and filled with the stimulus. Signals recorded from the labellar hairs were passed through a high-impedance preamplifier and a low-frequency filter (50 Hz) before being displayed on a storage oscilloscope (Tektronix 5113). Four large medial crest bristles and six mediolateral bristles were sampled in each preparation (Falk et al. 1976). Each hair was stimulated three times with a 2 min interval between recordings. Spike frequencies were estimated from a 450 ms interval beginning 50 ms after the initiation of the response. The spike frequency of the sensillum was obtained as an average of the three trials. Recordings from different sensilla were pooled to give the mean and standard deviation of the response.

Genetic analysis

Complementation

Reciprocal crosses were set up between the males and virgin females of the different mutant strains. The Fl progeny were tested for their gustatory responses in the feeding preference test. The responses of the heterozygous females were compared with those of both mutants/+ and the homozygous mutants. The mutations were considered to be non-complementing when the percentage response of the heterozygote was comparable to that of homozygous mutants and significantly different from mutant/+. The males served as internal controls.

Cytological mapping

The mutants were crossed to a set of deficiency-bearing strains with rearrangements in the appropriate regions of the chromosome. Progeny of the relevant genotype were assayed for their taste responses in the feeding preference test.

The response of mutant/deficiency was compared with that of mutant/+ and deficiency/+.

The feeding preference test has been used to characterise the gustatory behaviour of mutant alleles in two loci: Shaker and shaking-B. We used several criteria to verify that the observed defects were in fact due to lesions in these loci rather than to other mutations arising in the genetic background of the strains. The phenotypes of several independently isolated alleles at each of the loci were examined. Combinations of these alleles were tested for the gustatory behaviour of their phenotypes in complementation tests. The gustatory defects in these strains were mapped to a cytological position using deficiency and duplication strains.

Responses of the wild-type strains: Canton S and Oregon R

The gustatory responses of Canton Special (CS) and Oregon R (OR) are shown in Fig. 1, illustrating the variation in behavioural responses between different wild-type strains. These strains have been chosen since the mutants analysed in this study have been isolated on CS or OR backgrounds. However, the genetic background of the different mutant strains is no longer strictly comparable to that of either wild type, as a result of out-crossing. Thus, CS is used as the standard wild-type strain. Comparisons with OR have been made only to emphasize that the mutant phenotype is detectable even when compared to a less responsive wild-type strain. Differences in gustatory behaviour between these two strains have already been reported (Tompkins et al. 1979; Tanimura et al. 1982). OR flies showed a reduced response to sugars compared with CS flies (Fig. 1A,B). They were also more salt-tolerant than CS flies (Fig. 1D). The attraction response to low concentrations of NaCl was comparable in the two strains (Fig. 1C). In no case were sex-specific differences in response observed.

The Shaker complex

Mutations at this locus cause vigorous leg-shaking under ether anaesthesia (Kaplan and Trout, 1969). Shaker has been localized to the 16F region of the X chromosome. 3-Ethylmethanesulphonate (EMS)-induced alleles, Sh5, Sh102 and ShKS133, and three translocation breakpoints, T(X;Y)B55, W32 and V7, all map within this complex (Table 1) (Tanouye et al. 1981). The translocations show the leg-shaking phenotype typical of the Shaker mutants.

Gustatory phenotypes of the Shaker alleles

We have tested the responses of the three EMS-induced alleles and the three translocation strains to sugars and salts using the feeding preference test. At 1 mmol 1-1 sucrose, 85-90 % of wild-type flies eat preferentially from the stimulus-containing wells. Males from each of the six strains showed a reduced preference for sucrose (Fig. 2A). The severity of the defect can be ordered as: B55> Sh5=Sh102>W32>V7>ShKSI33. Females of Sh5, Sh102 and ShKS133 show comparable responses. Only the males of the translocation strains were tested. The dose-response curve for sucrose suggested that the defect in all six strains is a shift in the threshold of detection (data not shown).

Fig. 2.

Responses of the six Shaker strains and Canton S (CS) to gustatory stimuli. Acceptance responses to sucrose (A) and NaCl (B) and tolerance responses to NaCl (C) and KO (D) were assayed in the feeding preference test. Means and standard deviations were calculated from at least 10 readings.

Fig. 2.

Responses of the six Shaker strains and Canton S (CS) to gustatory stimuli. Acceptance responses to sucrose (A) and NaCl (B) and tolerance responses to NaCl (C) and KO (D) were assayed in the feeding preference test. Means and standard deviations were calculated from at least 10 readings.

Wild-type flies exhibit a weak attraction to 100 mmol 1-1 NaCl (Fig. 2B). The Shaker strains vary in their NaCl attraction response. Flies containing two alleles, Sh102 and ShKSI33, show no attraction to sodium chloride. The response is well below the cut-off values for inherent error in the test due to non-eaters. Sh5 flies show an enhanced acceptance of NaCl (P<0.01). The responses of the translocation strains B55 and W32 are not significantly different from those of the wild type (P>0.01). V7 flies shows a small increase in their attraction response (P<0.01).

High concentrations of NaCl or KCl are strongly repellent to wild-type flies. Less than 10% of CS flies are tolerant of 0.5 mol 1-1 NaCl or 0.2 mol 1-1 KCl. Strains containing different alleles differed in their degree of tolerance to 0.5 mol 1-1 NaCl and may be ordered as follows: B55>Sh’02—Sh5> V7=W32>ShKSI33 (Fig. 2C). KCl tolerance may be ordered as: B55= ShI02=ShKS133>V7>Sh5. T(X;Y)W32 flies behave like the wild type at 0.2moll-1 KCl. The tolerance to NaCl and KCl observed in these strains is due to an increase in the threshold of repulsion. Repulsion responses can be elicited at high stimulus concentrations (data not shown).

The labellar chemoreceptors of strains containing the Shaker alleles show normal physiology

We measured the firing frequency of the labellar chemosensory neurones using the tip recording method (Hodgson et al. 1955). Stimulation with NaCl and KCl at 0.1 mol 1-1 and 0.5 moll-1 results in responses from two salt neurones (LI and L2). Spikes from LI are 1.0-1.5 mV in amplitude. The activity of the LI neurone increases in a dose-dependent manner with a threshold at 1 mmol 1-1 and saturates at concentrations above 0.1 moll-1 (data not shown). L2 spikes are larger in amplitude, but the responses from this cell are erratic and could not be elicited from all hairs. This neurone exhibits a high-threshold, low-frequency response (Siddiqi et al. 1989). The responses of strains containing three Shaker alleles, Sh102, ShKS133 and Sh5, are summarized in Table 2. These strains did not show any alteration in firing frequencies of the labellar chemoreceptors to NaCl or KCl. The spike amplitudes and shapes were comparable to those of the wild-type strain.

Table 2.

Electrophysiological responses to salts of the labellar chemoreceptors of file containing Shaker alleles

Electrophysiological responses to salts of the labellar chemoreceptors of file containing Shaker alleles
Electrophysiological responses to salts of the labellar chemoreceptors of file containing Shaker alleles

Stimulation of the labellar hairs with sucrose resulted in activity in the S neurone (Rodrigues and Siddiqi, 1978; Fujishiro et al. 1984). Neurones from wild-type flies fire at a frequency of 42±3.5 spikes per 450 ms in response to 100 mmol 1-1 sucrose. The firing frequency of neurones of the Shaker alleles is not significantly different from those of the CS strain (data not shown).

Complementation analysis of the Shaker alleles with respect to taste function

The defects in taste function observed in flies containing the three EMS-induced alleles of Shaker are fully recessive when tested with 1 mmol 1-1 sucrose (Table 3). The ether-induced leg-shaking behaviour is dominant.

Table 3.

Dominance testing of the Shaker alleles

Dominance testing of the Shaker alleles
Dominance testing of the Shaker alleles

We tested the gustatory responses of flies containing trans combinations of the Shaker alleles. The data are summarized in Table 4. Heterozygotes of Sh5/ Sh102, ShKS133/Sh5 and Sh102/ ShKSl33 all show a reduced response at 1 mmol 1-1 sucrose compared to the wild-type/S/m/cer flies (P⪡0.01 in all cases). Interestingly, ShKS133/Sh5 flies exhibit a response that is significantly lower than that of ShKS33 homozygotes (P<0.01).

Table 4.

Complementation analysis between Shaker alleles

Complementation analysis between Shaker alleles
Complementation analysis between Shaker alleles

T(X;Y)B55, T(X;Y)W32 and T(X;Y)V7 are independently isolated translocation strains with breakpoints in the 16F region of the X chromosome. Hemizygous males would therefore be null for a small region of the Shaker complex (Table 1). The observation that these males exhibit defects in response to taste stimuli maps these lesions to within the Shaker complex.

The shaking-B locus

Pas and shak-B are viable alleles at this locus. Pas is a semi-dominant allele isolated on a Canton S background (Thomas and Wyman, 1984); shak-B is a recessive allele derived from Oregon R (Homyk et al. 1980). Both Pas and shak-B show a connectivity defect in the giant fibre pathway (Wyman and Thomas, 1983; Baird, 1986). R-9-29 and EF535 are two of the lethal alleles at this locus.

Gustatory defects in Pas and shak-B

The Pas and shak-B mutations resulted in a reduced attraction response to sucrose and fructose, an abolition of the attraction response to 0.1 mol 1-1 NaCl and a small increase in tolerance to 0.5moll-1 NaCl (Figs 3,4). The reduction in the response to sugars was evident at threshold concentrations; at 0.1 mmol 1-1 and 1 mmol 1-1 sucrose, there was a significant difference between Pas and CS (P<0.01) and between OR and shak-B flies (P<0.01). At saturating concentrations (>10mmoll-1) there was no significant difference between mutant and wild-type strains (P>0.01). Thus, the defect is essentially a shift in the threshold of detection. Fig. 4 shows the lack of an attraction response to 0.1 mol 1-1 NaCl in the mutants (P<0.01). Since the response is indicated by the number of uncoloured flies in the plate, its value will be affected by the number of ‘non-eaters’. The acceptance of 10-15 % by mutant flies is below the value normally obtained for non-eaters. Pas and shak-B flies are slightly more tolerant than CS to 0.5 mol 1-1 NaCl (P<0.0l).

Fig. 3.

Stimulus-response curves of Pas and shak-B strains to sugars. The responses of the Pas strain to sucrose (A) and fructose (B) were compared to those of the CS strain, whereas those of the shak-B strain (C, sucrose; D, fructose) were compared to those of the OR strain. Values are the means and standard deviations of at least 10 readings. Sucrose and fructose concentration is in moll-1.

Fig. 3.

Stimulus-response curves of Pas and shak-B strains to sugars. The responses of the Pas strain to sucrose (A) and fructose (B) were compared to those of the CS strain, whereas those of the shak-B strain (C, sucrose; D, fructose) were compared to those of the OR strain. Values are the means and standard deviations of at least 10 readings. Sucrose and fructose concentration is in moll-1.

Fig. 4.

Responses of Pas and shak-B strains to NaCl. (A) Acceptance of 0.1 mol 1-1 NaCl; (B) tolerance to 0.5 mol 1-1 NaCl. Responses were assayed in the feeding preference test. Values are the means and standard deviations of at least 10 observations.

Fig. 4.

Responses of Pas and shak-B strains to NaCl. (A) Acceptance of 0.1 mol 1-1 NaCl; (B) tolerance to 0.5 mol 1-1 NaCl. Responses were assayed in the feeding preference test. Values are the means and standard deviations of at least 10 observations.

Complementation analysis of the shak-B alleles

Pas and shak-B flies behave as fully recessive alleles when assayed for their acceptance response to 1 mmol 1-1 sucrose (Table 5). The responses of flies of genotype Pas/CS and shak-B/OR are comparable to those of CS and OR, respectively (P=0.5). These alleles fail to complement one another for their gustatory behaviour (Table 6). We tested the effects of two lethal alleles at the shak-B locus, R-9-29 and EF535, on taste function. Both mutations show no behavioural abnormalities in trans combination with a wild-type chromosome (Table 5). R-9-29 fails to complement Pas and shak-B flies for their gustatory defect (Table 6). The phenotypes of Pas/R-9-29 and Pas/EF535 heterozygotes are more extreme than that of the homozygous strain (P<0.01). shak-B/EF535 is significantly different from EF535/ + (P<0.01), but not from shak-B/ + (P=0.4). The reason for this discrepancy is not clear.

Table 5.

Dominance testing of the shaking-B alleles

Dominance testing of the shaking-B alleles
Dominance testing of the shaking-B alleles
Table 6.

Complementation analysis between shak-B alleles and their interaction with a neighbouring complementation group R-9-28

Complementation analysis between shak-B alleles and their interaction with a neighbouring complementation group R-9-28
Complementation analysis between shak-B alleles and their interaction with a neighbouring complementation group R-9-28

Interactions of shak-B alleles with members of adjacent complementation groups

Two complementation groups which flank shak-B are runt, a segmentation gene in 19E2, and R-9-28, a lethal group in 19E4 (D. H. Baird, R. J. Wyman, J. A. Davies and G. L. G. Miklos, unpublished results). We examined the effects of alleles at these loci in trans combination with the shak-B alleles.

runt fully complements the taste defect of shak-B (P=0.5) (Table 6). The lethal allele R-9-28 interacted with both Pas and shak-B (Table 6). The response of Pas/R-9-28 flies to sucrose was comparable to that of the homozygous mutant. The phenotype of shak-B/R-9-28 was intermediate between that of shak-B/+ and shak-B/shak-B. These results suggest that alleles of shak-B interact with R-9-28 in specifying components of the gustatory pathway. The quantitative effects of this interaction on taste behaviour are allele-specific.

Cytological localization of the taste lesion in Pas and shak-B

The shak-B locus maps to the 19E3-4 region of the X chromosome (Miklos et al. 1987). We used a set of deficiencies in the 19E region (Fig. 5), to map the gustatory lesion on the salivary gland chromosome map. In each case, a heterozygote of the deficiency chromosome with Pas and shak-B was constructed and tested for its response to 1 mmol 1-1 sucrose in the preference feeding assay. The response obtained was compared to that of deficiency/+ flies.

Fig. 5.

Cytological map of the 19D3;F1 region of the X chromosome. The deficiency strains used to locate the gustatory lesions in Pas and shak-B are shown.

Fig. 5.

Cytological map of the 19D3;F1 region of the X chromosome. The deficiency strains used to locate the gustatory lesions in Pas and shak-B are shown.

Results summarized in Table 7 indicate that the gustatory lesions in Pas and shak-B are uncovered by deficiencies 16-3-22 (19D1;2OA2), B57 (19E1-2;19F1) and 16-3-35 (19D3;19E3). These deficiencies overlap in the 19E3-4 bands, to which the jump phenotype has also been mapped (Baird, 1986). The phenotype of Pas/Df is more extreme than that of the homozygous mutant strain (P<0.01). Df(l)A118 (19E4-5;19E8) shows an extreme taste phenotype in combination with shak-B and a weak phenotype in combination with Pas. This deficiency does not uncover the shak-B locus but spans the R-9-28 complementation group with which shak-B interacts for its function in the taste pathway. Df(l)T2-14A. (19E5;19E7-8) does not uncover the taste phenotype of either Pas or shak-B.

Table 7.

Cytological mapping of the gustatory phenotype in the Pas and shak-B strains

Cytological mapping of the gustatory phenotype in the Pas and shak-B strains
Cytological mapping of the gustatory phenotype in the Pas and shak-B strains

We have examined the gustatory properties of mutants in two different loci, Shaker and shaking-B. Their phenotypes will be discussed in the context of our working model of the gustatory pathway, which is schematically represented in Fig. 6. This simplistic model has been postulated to fit behavioural and electrophysiological data obtained from wild-type and mutant flies.

Fig. 6.

Comparator model to explain taste function in Drosophila. The figure illustrates a simplistic model which attempts to explain gustatory behaviour. The sugar-sensitive neurone (S) has three acceptor sites for pyranose (Pyr), furanose (Fur) and trehalose (Tre). The salt-sensitive cells (LI and L2) are responsive to Na+ and K+.

Fig. 6.

Comparator model to explain taste function in Drosophila. The figure illustrates a simplistic model which attempts to explain gustatory behaviour. The sugar-sensitive neurone (S) has three acceptor sites for pyranose (Pyr), furanose (Fur) and trehalose (Tre). The salt-sensitive cells (LI and L2) are responsive to Na+ and K+.

Adult Drosophila are strongly attracted to a variety of different sugars. Sugars are detected by the S neurone, which has been shown to possess independent molecular receptors for pyranose, furanose and trehalose (Tanimura and Shimada, 1981; Rodrigues and Siddiqi, 1981; Tanimura et al. 1982). Low concentrations of sodium chloride are accepted by the fly in feeding preference as well as proboscis extension assays (Arora et al. 1987). At these concentrations, only one of the salt-sensitive neurones (LI) is active. This neurone detects NaCl at a threshold of 1 mmol 1-1 and the response saturates at 100 mmol 1-1. The attraction response to this salt is in this concentration range. Since both S and LI neurones mediate behavioural attraction, we postulate that they are ‘wired’ to a common centre mediating attraction responses.

At higher concentrations, NaCl as well as KCl are strongly repellent to the fly. At these concentrations, the LI response is maximal. In addition, activity is also detected from the second salt-sensitive cell (L2). The response of this cell is, however, very erratic and is therefore unlikely to contribute solely to detection of salts at high concentration. Estimation of the concentration of stimulus can be more efficiently achieved by a comparison of activity in two neurones. We postulate wiring of LI and L2 to a common ‘repulsion’ centre. Information from the attraction and repulsion centres are compared to give rise to a motor response (Dethier, 1976).

The phenotypes of the Shaker and shaking-B alleles are compared in Figs 7 and 8 with those of some gustatory mutants isolated in our laboratory (Siddiqi et al. 1989; V. Rodrigues, S. Sathe, L. Pinto, R. Balakrishnan and O. Siddiqi, in press). In none of these mutants do the changes in peripheral physiology correlate with the behavioural defects, suggesting that the lesions are at more central steps in the gustatory pathway. In this context, the lack of defects in firing patterns of the labellar chemosensory neurones in flies containing the three Shaker alleles is striking in view of their behavioural defects. This result indicates that the family of potassium channels encoded by the Shaker locus is not involved in taste transduction; however, these channels affect the central processing of gustatory information.

Fig. 7.

Schematic representation of the acceptance response of different taste mutants to sucrose (1 mmol 1-1) and NaCl (100 mmol 1-1). The vertical lines indicate the percentage acceptance of the stimulus measured by the feeding preference test. The height of the lines is used to compare the responses of the different mutant strains and does not indicate the quantitative value of the response. The response of the wild-type strain (CS) is given by the dotted horizontal line.

Fig. 7.

Schematic representation of the acceptance response of different taste mutants to sucrose (1 mmol 1-1) and NaCl (100 mmol 1-1). The vertical lines indicate the percentage acceptance of the stimulus measured by the feeding preference test. The height of the lines is used to compare the responses of the different mutant strains and does not indicate the quantitative value of the response. The response of the wild-type strain (CS) is given by the dotted horizontal line.

Fig. 8.

Schematic representation of the tolerance response of different taste mutants to NaCl (0.5 mol 1-1) and KCl (0.2 mol 1-1). The percentage tolerance of each different mutant strain to salts is indicated as vertical lines. The height of the lines qualitatively represents the values obtained from the feeding preference test. The wild-type response (CS) is given by the dotted horizontal line.

Fig. 8.

Schematic representation of the tolerance response of different taste mutants to NaCl (0.5 mol 1-1) and KCl (0.2 mol 1-1). The percentage tolerance of each different mutant strain to salts is indicated as vertical lines. The height of the lines qualitatively represents the values obtained from the feeding preference test. The wild-type response (CS) is given by the dotted horizontal line.

Several of the mutants, gustC, GT-1, GustR and Sh5, show an increased attraction to 100 mmol 1-1 NaCl together with an increased tolerance to high concentrations of NaCl. This suggests that attraction and repulsion information is compared and integrated, leading to the final response (Fig. 6). In addition, all these mutants also show a decrease in their attraction response to sugars with no correlating electrophysiological defects in the firing of the S neurone. If the information in the S and LI neurones is indeed compared, this would suggest that an increased ‘weight’ given to the information content in LI in the mutants occurs at the expense of that of the S cell.

The comparator model of the taste pathway is further supported by the phenotype of another taste mutant gustE (data not shown; Siddiqi et al. 1989), in which loss of the NaCl attraction response at low concentrations is coupled with an increased repulsion to higher concentrations of NaCl. The defect in this mutation has a peripheral correlate in that the LI neurone is less sensitive to NaCl.

However, the attraction and repulsion of NaCl is not tightly linked and has been uncoupled in several mutations. ShJ02 flies have lost the attraction response, but show a greatly increased tolerance to NaCl and KCl. In the translocation strain B55, the tolerance response is increased with no change in the attraction to NaCl. These phenotypes are more difficult to explain, since the Shaker mutations may be affecting both attraction and repulsion pathways at different points in the circuitry.

The comparator model also suggests that NaCl and KCl are detected by common receptor cells and predicts that defects in response to these salts should be linked. Most of the mutations shown in Figs 7 and 8 satisfy this prediction. ShKSI33 and T(X;Y)W32 are notable exceptions to this rule. T(X;Y)W32 causes a small increase in NaCl tolerance with no significant effect on KCl. ShKS133 causes an increased threshold of repulsion for KCl with little effect on that for NaCl. The firing patterns of the labellar taste neurones in response to both stimuli are unaltered. This suggests that the nervous system must receive information about KCl that is distinct from that for NaCl. The mechanism by which this information is conveyed needs to be examined in detail. There is behavioural evidence to suggest that these two salts are discriminated by the fly; although NaCl at low concentrations is accepted by CS flies, KCl is not attractive at any concentration.

The two viable alleles at the shaking-B locus, Pas and shak-B, cause an increased threshold of detection of sugars and a loss in the attraction response to NaCl. We speculate that the lesion in these strains could be in the ‘attraction’ centre. Genetic analysis indicates that shak-B interacts with an independent complementation group R-9-28 to specify components of the gustatory pathway. This observation is preliminary and the interaction of other alleles at R-9-28 with shak-B needs to be examined. The molecular nature of the shak-B locus is unknown. Alleles at this locus have been shown to cause a defect in the ability of the giant fibre pathway to establish synaptic connections (Wyman and Thomas, 1983). The neuronal basis of the taste defect in these mutant strains is still to be investigated. The various Shaker alleles cause diverse behavioural abnormalities. This locus generates a variety of different transcripts which are translated into distinct protein products (Schwartz et al. 1988). Mutations that affect specific Shaker isoforms could result in the behavioural patterns seen in flies containing the alleles at this locus. Antibodies directed against the variable and constant regions of the Shaker protein have provided evidence for the differential distribution of the different splice variants in different regions of the brain (Schwartz et al. 1990). Deletion of the Shaker gene affects the transient K current in only a minority of neurones (Baker and Salkoff, 1990). However, the properties of a particular cell in a circuit are determined not only by the presence or absence of a particular type of channel but also its topography on that cell.

An analysis of the effects of mutations in genes that encode channel or synaptic components on behaviour and physiology can allow dissection of circuits involved in gustation and other functions. Many such loci have been defined and thus allow such a study (Ganetzky and Wu, 1986). Analysis of suppressors and enhancers of gustatory phenotypes will also serve to define new interacting genes. Our model, therefore, serves as a starting point for unravelling processing circuits and its simplistic features will be modified as we learn more about how the fly analyses its taste inputs.

We are grateful to Swati Sathe for helping us with many of the techniques and to K. VijayRaghavan for stimulating discussions. We express our thanks to Jane Davies for her interest in this work and for sharing her unpublished data with us. We thank her, Doug Baird and the Tanouye laboratory (CalTech) for sending us several of the stocks.

Arora
,
K.
,
Rodrigues
,
V.
,
Joshi
,
S.
,
Shanbhag
,
S.
and
Siddiqi
,
O.
(
1987
).
A gene affecting the specificity of chemosensory neurons of Drosophila
.
Nature
330
,
62
63
.
Avenet
,
P.
,
Hofmann
,
F.
and
Lindemann
,
B.
(
1988
).
Transduction in taste receptor cells requires cAMP-dependent protein kinase
.
Nature
331
,
351
354
.
Baird
,
D. H.
(
1986
).
Genetic mapping, alleles and gene dosage effects of Passover, a Drosophila mutant altering synaptic connections
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr
.
12
,
1164
.
Baker
,
K.
and
Salkoff
,
L.
(
1990
).
The Drosophila Shaker gene codes for a distinctive K+ current in a subset of neurons
.
Neuron
4
,
129
140
.
Caudy
,
M.
,
Vässin
,
H.
,
Brand
,
M.
,
Tuma
,
R.
,
Jan
,
L. Y.
and
Jan
,
Y. N.
(
1988
).
daughterless, a Drosophila gene essential for both neurogenesis and sex determination, has sequence similarities to myc and the achaete-scute complex
.
Cell
55
,
1061
1067
.
Cline
,
T. W.
(
1989
).
The affairs of daughterless and the promiscuity of developmental regulators
.
Cell
59
,
231
234
.
Dethier
,
V. G.
(
1976
).
The Hungry Fly
.
Cambridge, MA
:
Harvard University Press
.
Falk
,
R.
and
Atidia
,
J.
(
1975
).
Mutation affecting taste perception in Drosophila melanogaster
.
Nature
254
,
325
326
.
Falk
,
R.
,
Bleiser-Avivi
,
N.
and
Atidia
,
J.
(
1976
).
Labellar taste organs of Drosophila melanogaster
.
J. Morph
.
150
,
327
341
.
Fujishiro
,
N.
,
Kijima
,
H.
and
Morita
,
H.
(
1984
).
Impulse frequency and action potential amplitude in labellar chemosensory neurons of Drosophila melanogaster
.
J. Insect Physiol
.
30
,
317
325
.
Ganetzky
,
B.
and
Wu
,
C. F.
(
1986
).
Neurogenetics of membrane excitability in Drosophila
.
A. Rev. Genet
.
20
,
3
44
.
Getting
,
P. A.
(
1983
).
Mechanisms of pattern generation underlying swimming in Tritonia. III. Intrinsic and synaptic mechanisms for delayed excitation
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
49
,
1036
1051
.
Ghysen
,
A.
and
Dambly-Chaudiere
,
C.
(
1989
).
Genesis of the Drosophila peripheral nervous system
.
Trends Genet
.
5
,
251
255
.
Hermans-Borgmeyer
,
I.
,
Zopf
,
D.
,
Ryseck
,
R. P.
,
Hovemann
,
B.
,
Betz
,
H.
and
Gundelfinger
,
E. D.
(
1986
).
Primary structure of a developmentally-regulated nicotinic acetylcholine receptor protein in Drosophila
.
EMBO J
.
5
,
1503
1508
.
Hodgson
,
E. S.
,
Lettvin
,
J. Y.
and
Roeder
,
K. D.
(
1955
).
Physiology of a primary chemoreceptor unit
.
Science
122
,
417
418
.
Homyk
,
T.
, Jr
,
Szidonya
,
J.
and
Suzuki
,
D. T.
(
1980
).
Behavioral mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. I. Isolation and mapping by direct visual observation of behavioral phenotypes
.
Molec. gen. Genet
.
177
,
553
565
.
Isono
,
K.
and
Kikuchi
,
T.
(
1974
).
Autosomal recessive mutation in sugar response of Drosophila
.
Nature
248
,
243
244
.
Iverson
,
L. E.
,
Tanouye
,
M. A.
,
Lester
,
H. A.
,
Davidson
,
N.
and
Rudy
,
B.
(
1988
).
A-type potassium channels expressed from Shaker cDNAs
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
85
,
5723
5727
.
Jan
,
Y. N.
,
Jan
,
L. Y.
and
Dennis
,
M. J.
(
1977
).
Two mutations of synaptic transmission in Drosophila
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
198
,
87
108
.
Kamb
,
A.
,
Tseng-Crank
,
J.
and
Tanouye
,
M. A.
(
1988
).
Multiple products of the Shaker gene may contribute to potassium channel diversity
.
Neuron
1
,
421
430
.
Kaplan
,
W. D.
and
Trout
,
W. E.
, III
(
1969
).
The behaviour of four neurological mutants of Drosophila
.
Genetics
61
,
399
409
.
Kinnamon
,
S.
and
Roper
,
S. D.
(
1988
).
Evidence for a role of voltage-sensitive apical K+ channels in sour and salt taste transduction
.
Chem. Senses
3
,
115
121
.
Miklos
,
G. L. G.
,
Kelly
,
L. E.
,
Coombe
,
P. E.
,
Leeds
,
C.
and
Lefevre
,
G.
(
1987
).
Localization of the genes shaking-B, small optic lobes, sluggish-A, stoned and stress-sensitive- C to a well-defined region on the X-chromosome of Drosophila melanogaster
.
J. Neurogenet
.
4
,
1
19
.
Morea
,
M.
(
1985
).
Deletion mapping of a new gustatory mutant in Drosophila melanogaster
.
Experientia
41
,
1381
1384
.
Jnayak
,
S. V.
and
Singh
,
R. N.
(
1985
).
Primary sensory projections from the labella to the brain of Drosophila melanogaster Meigen (Díptera: Drosophilidae)
.
Int. J. Insect Morph. Embryol
.
14
,
115
129
.
Pongs
,
O.
,
Kecskemethy
,
N.
,
Mueller
,
R.
,
Krah-Jentgens
,
I.
,
Baumann
,
A.
,
Kiltz
,
H.
,
Canal
,
A.
,
Llamazares
,
S.
and
Ferrus
,
A.
(
1988
).
Shaker encodes a family of putative potassium channel proteins in the nervous system of Drosophila
.
EMBO J
.
7
,
1087
1096
.
Rodrigues
,
V.
and
Siddiqi
,
O.
(
1978
).
Genetic analysis of chemosensory pathway
.
Proc. Indian Acad. Sci. B
87
,
147
160
.
Rodrigues
,
V.
and
Siddiqi
,
O.
(
1981
).
A gustatory mutant of Drosophila defective in pyranose receptors
.
Molec. gen. Genet
.
181
,
406
408
.
Rushlow
,
C. A.
,
Hogan
,
A.
,
Pinchin
,
S. M.
,
Howe
,
K. M.
,
Lardelli
,
M.
and
Ish-Horowicz
,
D.
(
1989
).
The Drosophila hairy protein acts in both segmentation and bristle patterning and shows homology to n-myc
.
EMBO J
.
8
,
3095
3101
.
Salkoff
,
L.
and
Wyman
,
R. J.
(
1983
).
Ion channels in Drosophila muscle
.
Trends Neurosci
.
6
,
128
133
.
Schwartz
,
T. L.
,
Papazian
,
D. M.
,
Carretto
,
R. C.
,
Jan
,
Y. N.
and
Jan
,
L. Y.
(
1990
).
Immunological characterization of potassium channel components from the Shaker locus and differential distribution of splicing variants in Drosophila
.
Neuron
4
,
119
127
.
Schwartz
,
T. L.
,
Tempel
,
B. L.
,
Papazian
,
D. M.
,
Jan
,
Y. N.
and
Jan
,
L. Y.
(
1988
).
Multiple potassium channel components are produced by alternative splicing at the Shaker locus in Drosophila
.
Nature
331
,
137
142
.
Siddiqi
,
O.
,
Joshi
,
S.
,
Arora
,
K.
and
Rodrigues
,
V.
(
1989
).
Genetic investigation of salt reception in Drosophila melanogaster
.
Genome
31
,
646
651
.
Steller
,
H.
,
Fischbach
,
K.-F.
and
Rubin
,
G. M.
(
1987
).
disconnected: a locus required for neuronal pathway formation in the visual system of Drosophila
.
Cell
50
,
1139
1153
.
Tanimura
,
T.
,
Isono
,
K.
,
Takamura
,
T.
and
Shimada
,
I.
(
1982
).
Genetic dimorphism in the taste sensitivity to trehalose in Drosophila melanogaster
.
J. comp. Physiol. A
147
,
433
437
.
Tanimura
,
T.
and
Shimada
,
I.
(
1981
).
Multiple receptor proteins for sweet taste in Drosophila discriminated by papain treatment
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
147
,
433
437
.
Tanouye
,
M. A.
,
Ferrus
,
A.
and
Fujita
,
S. C.
(
1981
).
Abnormal action potentials associated with the Shaker complex locus of Drosophila
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
78
,
6548
6552
.
Thomas
,
J. B.
and
Wyman
,
R. J.
(
1984
).
Mutations altering synaptic connectivity between identified neurons in Drosophila
.
J. Neurosci
.
4
,
530
538
.
Timpe
,
L. C.
,
Jan
,
Y. N.
and
Jan
,
L. Y.
(
1988a
).
Four cDNA clones from the Shaker locus of Drosophila induce kinetically distinct A-type potassium currents in Xenopus oocytes
.
Neuron
1
,
659
667
.
Timpe
,
L. C.
,
Schwarz
,
T. L.
,
Tempel
,
B. L.
,
Papazian
,
D. M.
,
Jan
,
Y. N.
and
Jan
,
L. Y.
(
1988b
).
Expression of functional potassium channels from Shaker cDNA in Xenopus oocytes
.
Nature
331
,
143
145
.
Tompkins
,
L.
,
Jane Cardosa
,
M.
,
White
,
F. V.
and
Sanders
,
T. G.
(
1979
).
Isolation and analysis of chemosensory behavior mutants in Drosophila melanogaster
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
76
,
884
887
.
Vàssin
,
H.
,
Bremer
,
K. A.
,
Knust
,
E.
and
Campos-Ortega
,
J. A.
(
1987
).
The neurogenic locus Delta of Drosophila melanogaster is expressed in neurogenic territories and encodes a putative transmembrane protein with EGF-like repeats
.
EMBO J
.
11
,
3431
3440
.
Wharton
,
K. A.
,
Johansen
,
K. M.
,
Xu
,
T.
and
Artavanis-Tsakonas
,
S.
(
1985
).
Nucleotide sequence from the neurogenic locus Notch implies a gene product that shares homology with proteins containing EGF-like repeats
.
Cell
43
,
567
581
.
Wyman
,
R. J.
and
Thomas
,
J. B.
(
1983
).
What genes are necessary to make an identified synapse?
Cold Spring Harb. Symp. quant. Biol
.
18
,
611
652
.