In searching for cellular mechanisms by which nervous systems store learned information, it is helpful to differentiate properties which are genetically programmed from those which are conferred by the interaction of an organism with its environment. With mechanisms of learning, as with those underlying developmental transformations, a chronology must be reconstructed - a sequence of processes which arise as a consequence of preceding conditions. To construct such a chronology, the cellular site at which learned information is stored must be accessible to analysis so that we may watch it during sequential transformations, and so that we can measure with appropriate morphological, biophysical and biochemical techniques the nature and magnitude of the critical changes.

Accessibility to sites of associative learning has been elusive for vertebrates, in which extracellular potentials have been recorded as correlates of acquisition and retention of conditioned behaviour (Woody, Vassilevsky & Engel, 1970; Woody & Black-Cleworth, 1973; Woody etal. 1974; Berger, Alger & Thompson, 1976; Cohen & MacDonald, 1976; Berger & Thompson, 1977, 1978; Brons & Woody, 1980; Thompson et al. 1982; Woody, 1982). It has also been largely true for invertebrates such as the bee, locust and the gastropod molluscs Pleurobranchaea, Umax and Aplysia. Hoyle and his colleagues recorded changes of impulses in certain locust motor neurones during learning (Hoyle, 1982). However, these changes have only been measured unequivocally for minutes, and their site of origin, and thus their accessibility, have not been determined. Furthermore, the synaptic organization c® the neural elements which mediate the training response remains obscure.

Workers using Aplysia and Pleurobranchaea have had more success in identifying neurones within relevant sensory pathways (Castellucci, Pinsker, Kupferman & Kandel, 1970; Gillette & Davis, 1977; Gillette, Gillette & Davis, 1980). However, a close and specific correlation of neuronal changes with learned avoidance of food in Pleurobranchaea (as opposed to its being in a food-satiated state), has not been made, as it has for vertebrate conditioning. Nor has it been possible to measure correlated changes in identified neurones or neuronal aggregates of Limax or Aplysia during or after associative training. For dissected and reduced preparations, short-term correlations (Carew, Hawkins & Kandel, 1983) have been made, but their relevance to the neurophysiology of living animals must be established (see for example, Kanz, Eberley, Cobbs & Pinsker, 1979).

Furthermore, the type of learning involved with some of these gastropods has not been entirely resolved. Some assert that non-associative behavioural changes such as habituation and sensitization can underlie associative learning, such as classical conditioning (Kandel & Schwartz, 1982), although any relevance of non-associative to associative mechanisms remains to be demonstrated. Even those behavioural changes called ‘associative’ require further study before their ‘associative’ nature can be considered certain.

Mpitsos & Collins (1975) trained Pleurobranchaea to avoid food substances by pairing the food with shocks to the head. Unpaired presentations did not produce an effect. These experiments could be simplified by controlling the different states of satiation which might result from the training. Long-term sensitization of a neophobic reaction to the food substances by shock should also be examined. The recent experiments of Gelperin and his colleagues withLimax (Gelperin, Wieland & Barry, 1984; Sahley, Rudy & Gelperin, 1984) require these same controls, since all measures of avoidance behaviour following pairing of a food substance with quinine were made by comparison with responses to a ‘safe’ food. A neophobic reaction to a ‘safe’ food (on which the animal had been raised) might be entirely habituated, while quinine could sensitize the neophobic reaction to a food substance with which it was paired during training. An apparently associative behavioural change (i.e. one dependent on the pairing of a food substance with quinine or an electric shock) might be simulated by particular combinations of two non-associative behavioural effects such as habituation and sensitization.

In recent experiments on Aplysia, tactile stimulation of the gill withdrawal response, which habituates, has been paired with electrical stimulation of the tail, which causes sensitization (Carew, Walters & Kandel, 1981). This causes the animal to withdraw its gill more rapidly and reliably. However, the possibility exists that the shock-induced sensitization prevents the touch-induced habituation. This is suggested by the observation that repeated presentation of the shock alone produces, to a significant degree, the same effect as repeated, paired, presentations. Other characteristics of associative learning, such as the stimulus specificity shown for Aplysia (Carew et al. 1981), and the blocking shown forLimax (Sahley et al. 1984), have yet to be clearly distinguished from combinations of effects induced by single stimuli.

With these difficulties in mind, we have worked for some years with the nudibranch mollusc Hermissenda crassicomis to localize sites of neuronal change which could play a causal role in associative learning, and which are accessible to quantitative analysis. We first found sites of convergence between three sensory pathways: the visual, vestibular and chemosensory (Alkon & Fuortes, 1972; Alkon, 1973, 1974a, 1980a, 1983; Alkon, Akaike & Harrigan, 1978). With simultaneous intracellular recordings from identified pre-and postsynaptic elements in thousands of adult nervous systems we constructed a working blueprint of the circumoesophageal nervous system (Fig. 1). Within this blueprint, the flow of sensory information could be traced from the input stages through the integrative centres to the output motor cells of relevant muscle groups. The effects of sensory stimuli such as light (transduced by five photoreceptors in each eye) and rotation (a gravitational stimulus transduced by thirteen hair cells in each statocyst, a primitive vestibular organ) could be studied at every level of integration: sensory cells, interneurones and motor neurones (Fig. 2A). Based on the known convergences between the visual and statocyst pathways, and the specific responses of the network to light and rotational stimuli with specific temporal relationships, we hypothesized that Hermissenda could be conditioned by repetition of paired light and rotational stimuli.

Fig. 1.

Schematic summary of synaptic interactions in the neural systems of Hermissenda crassicomis. Interactions within and between three sensory pathways are included. The visual pathway begins with the five photoreceptors (two Type A and three Type B) of each eye. The afferent cells of the vestibular pathway, the thirteen hair cells in each statocyst, are labelled HC. The tentacle, represented as an intact structure in the diagram, has chemosensory receptors distributed on its surface. Inhibitory synaptic interactions are indicated by filled endings, excitatory interactions are indicated by open endings. Muscle groups, innervated by the motor neurones are pictured at the bottom of the figure. Each interaction represented was established to be reliably present in the adult nervous system by simultaneous pre- and postsynaptic intracellular recording. Not all known interactions are included (Alkon, 1982, 1983).

Fig. 1.

Schematic summary of synaptic interactions in the neural systems of Hermissenda crassicomis. Interactions within and between three sensory pathways are included. The visual pathway begins with the five photoreceptors (two Type A and three Type B) of each eye. The afferent cells of the vestibular pathway, the thirteen hair cells in each statocyst, are labelled HC. The tentacle, represented as an intact structure in the diagram, has chemosensory receptors distributed on its surface. Inhibitory synaptic interactions are indicated by filled endings, excitatory interactions are indicated by open endings. Muscle groups, innervated by the motor neurones are pictured at the bottom of the figure. Each interaction represented was established to be reliably present in the adult nervous system by simultaneous pre- and postsynaptic intracellular recording. Not all known interactions are included (Alkon, 1982, 1983).

Fig. 2.

(A) Schematic diagram of viaual pathway and its convergence with the statocyst pathway. The Type B photoreceptor (B) causes monosynaptic inhibition of the medial Type A photoreceptor (A). The medial Type A photoreceptor causes monosynaptic excitation of ipsilateral interneurones (I), which are also excited by ipsilateral hair cells (HC). Ipsilateral hair cell impulses and Type B impulses cause a transient inhibition (not shown here) and are followed by long-lasting effective excitation (+) of the S/E optic ganglion cell and thereby the Type B cell (Goh & Alkon, 1984). (B) Intracellular voltage recordings of Hermusenda neurones during and after light and rotation stimuli. Responses of a type B photoreceptor to the second of two succeeding 30-s light steps (with a 90-s interval intervening). The cell’s initial resting potential, preceding the first of the two light steps in (1), (2), and (3), is indicated by the dashed lines. Depolarisation above the resting level after the second of the two light steps is indicated by shaded areas. (1) Light steps (~104ergcm-2s-1) alternating with rotation (caudal orientation) generating ~ 1 -0 g. The end of the rotation stimulus preceded each light step by 10s. (2) Light steps alone. (3) Light steps paired with rotation. By 60s after the first and second light steps, paired stimuli cause the greatest depolarization and unpaired stimuli the least. The minimal depolarization was in part attributable to the hyperpolarizing effect of rotation. Depolarization after the second presentation of paired stimuli was greater than that after the first. (Alkon, 1980b).

Fig. 2.

(A) Schematic diagram of viaual pathway and its convergence with the statocyst pathway. The Type B photoreceptor (B) causes monosynaptic inhibition of the medial Type A photoreceptor (A). The medial Type A photoreceptor causes monosynaptic excitation of ipsilateral interneurones (I), which are also excited by ipsilateral hair cells (HC). Ipsilateral hair cell impulses and Type B impulses cause a transient inhibition (not shown here) and are followed by long-lasting effective excitation (+) of the S/E optic ganglion cell and thereby the Type B cell (Goh & Alkon, 1984). (B) Intracellular voltage recordings of Hermusenda neurones during and after light and rotation stimuli. Responses of a type B photoreceptor to the second of two succeeding 30-s light steps (with a 90-s interval intervening). The cell’s initial resting potential, preceding the first of the two light steps in (1), (2), and (3), is indicated by the dashed lines. Depolarisation above the resting level after the second of the two light steps is indicated by shaded areas. (1) Light steps (~104ergcm-2s-1) alternating with rotation (caudal orientation) generating ~ 1 -0 g. The end of the rotation stimulus preceded each light step by 10s. (2) Light steps alone. (3) Light steps paired with rotation. By 60s after the first and second light steps, paired stimuli cause the greatest depolarization and unpaired stimuli the least. The minimal depolarization was in part attributable to the hyperpolarizing effect of rotation. Depolarization after the second presentation of paired stimuli was greater than that after the first. (Alkon, 1980b).

In fact, it has been possible to demonstrate most of those features which characterize vertebrate classical conditioning. Rotation, which serves as an unconditioned stimulus, elicits a consistent and reliable ‘clinging’ response (Alkon, 19746) manifest by measurable contraction of the ‘foot’ (particularly the caudal half of the ventral surface). In addition, the animal moves towards a light source (Alkon, 1974b). This is accompanied by lengthening of its foot, again particularly the caudal half. After light and rotation are repeatedly paired (Alkon, 1974b) (i.e. maximal light precedes maximal rotation by TOs), light elicits an entirely new response: contraction of the foot (Fig. 3), the response reflexly elicited by rotation. As a result of the conditioning procedure, light has taken on the meaning of rotation as measured by a new learned response (Lederhendler, Gart & Alkon, 1983). This learning behaviour is associative in that it is not produced by explicitly unpaired light and rotation, or light and rotation occurring with a randomly-varying interval during training (Crow & Alkon, 1978). It is stimulus specific, increases as a function of practice (i.e. it shows acquisition), can be retained for many weeks, extinguishes (Richards, Farley & Alkon, 1983) and shows savings. It also has a requirement for contingency (as well as contiguity) of the associated stimuli (Farley & Kern, 1983). This was shown by the degradation of the learning by interpolation of unpaired light or rotation stimuli. Finally, there is no long-term habituation or sensitization following training.

Fig. 3.

(A) Time lapse measurement of Hermissenda foot contraction. On the left are actual photographs (from below) of Hermissenda foot immediately before and 3 a after the onset of rotation. On the right are traces of the photographs on the left to highlight the contraction of the caudal half of the foot within 3 s of rotation onset. This contraction, part of a ‘clinging response’ is reliably elicited by the UCS (rotation) before and after training. Light, the CS, only elicits this response after associative training in which light and rotation are repetitively paired (I. Lederhendler & D. L. Alkon, in preparation). (B) Median response ratios for acquisition, retention and re-acquisition of a long-term behavioural change in response to a light stimulus in Hermissenda (random rotation, random light, □; unpaired light and rotation, Δ; random light and rotation, ▴ ; nothing,▄; paired light and rotation, ◯). The response ratio [in the form of 1–A/(A+B)] compared the latency during the test (A) with the baseline response latency (B). Group data consist of two independent replications for all control groups and three independent replications for the experimental group (Crow & Alkon, 1978).

Fig. 3.

(A) Time lapse measurement of Hermissenda foot contraction. On the left are actual photographs (from below) of Hermissenda foot immediately before and 3 a after the onset of rotation. On the right are traces of the photographs on the left to highlight the contraction of the caudal half of the foot within 3 s of rotation onset. This contraction, part of a ‘clinging response’ is reliably elicited by the UCS (rotation) before and after training. Light, the CS, only elicits this response after associative training in which light and rotation are repetitively paired (I. Lederhendler & D. L. Alkon, in preparation). (B) Median response ratios for acquisition, retention and re-acquisition of a long-term behavioural change in response to a light stimulus in Hermissenda (random rotation, random light, □; unpaired light and rotation, Δ; random light and rotation, ▴ ; nothing,▄; paired light and rotation, ◯). The response ratio [in the form of 1–A/(A+B)] compared the latency during the test (A) with the baseline response latency (B). Group data consist of two independent replications for all control groups and three independent replications for the experimental group (Crow & Alkon, 1978).

Intracellular recordings at each level of integration within the visual system of conditioned and control animals uncovered many learning-correlated changes of neuronal properties and response characteristics (Crow & Alkon, 1980; Farley & Alkon, 1982; Lederhendler, Goh & Alkon, 1982; West, Barnes & Alkon, 1982). Such recordings were made from progressively reduced preparations including living animals, isolated nervous systems and individual neurones isolated from all others, of conditioned and control animals. By this means it was possible to reconstruct a sequence of responses and changes during training.

Paired light and rotation (but not unpaired stimuli) enhance and prolong the depolarization of Type B photoreceptors during and following a light step. Repeated pairings cause progressively greater light-elicited depolarizations, an accumulation of membrane depolarization (Alkon, 1980b) and an increased input resistance (Fig. 2B). After a training session (e.g. of 50 trials) the Type B cell remains depolarized for many minutes (Alkon, 1980b; Crow & Alkon, 1980). On days following training (the retention period) the Type B cell from conditioned animals is no longer depolarized but remains more excitable. In response to injection of positive current or to light stimuli (Fig. 4), the conditioned Type B cell shows a greater depolarization (West et al. 1982). The increased impulse activity accompanying light-evoked depolarization in turn causes more inhibition of the medial Type A photoreceptor and, thereby, decreases excitation of interneurones which receive monosynaptic EPSPs from it (Farley & Alkon, 1982; Lederhendler et al. 1982; Goh & Alkon, 1984). Motor neurones which are monosynaptically excited by these interneurones are inhibited in this way by the Type B photoreceptor. In conditioned animals, increased light-evoked impulses in Type B cells decreases the excitation of motor neurones which in living animals cause turning toward a light source.

Fig. 4.

LLD (long-lasting depolarization) responses of Type B photoreceptors. Values taken from actual voltage recordings at pre-chosen time points (0, 5, 15, 30, 60s) following the first light step. Note that the paired LLD values are significantly greater than random and control, using a two-tail Mann-Whitney U test. The significance levels indicated refer to comparison of paired with respect to random values (West, Barnes & Alkon, 1982). Inset: responses to first light step of Type B photoreceptors from paired, random and control groups. Shaded areas indicate LLD following the light step (monitored by top trace). Note that the paired LLD is clearly larger than random and control (Westet al. 1982).

Fig. 4.

LLD (long-lasting depolarization) responses of Type B photoreceptors. Values taken from actual voltage recordings at pre-chosen time points (0, 5, 15, 30, 60s) following the first light step. Note that the paired LLD values are significantly greater than random and control, using a two-tail Mann-Whitney U test. The significance levels indicated refer to comparison of paired with respect to random values (West, Barnes & Alkon, 1982). Inset: responses to first light step of Type B photoreceptors from paired, random and control groups. Shaded areas indicate LLD following the light step (monitored by top trace). Note that the paired LLD is clearly larger than random and control (Westet al. 1982).

A number of other experiments taken together provide strong evidence that changes of Type B cells have a causal role in the production of associative learning. For example, the changes in Type B cells predict motor neurone changes (Lederhendler et al. 1982). The changes are present in Type B cells of conditioned but not control animals, physically isolated from all impulses and synaptic interactions with other neurones - i.e. these changes are intrinsic to the soma membrane (West et al. 1982). When these same changes are produced in Type B cells of living animals by pairing current injections (simulating the synaptic effects of stimulus pairing) with light, the learned behaviour measured on subsequent days is produced (Farley et al. 1983). This behavioural change does not follow unpaired light and current, or sham procedures.

Given that the Type B cell is a site for storage of learned information, is it accessible to biophysical and biochemical analysis? Fortunately, substantial changes induced by the conditioning were found to be intrinsic to the soma membrane of the Type B cell. These changes were sufficiently large and the spike initiating zone and synaptic endings were electrically close enough, to account for many of the observed changes of impulse activity and thus for the modified response of the visual pathway to light.

Since the roughly spherical soma can be isolated from the small diameter (~l-2μm and short axon, it is well suited for voltage-clamp studies (Alkon, 1979). Pharmacological blockers and substitution of ions permitted the separation of six distinct ionic currents across the soma membrane of a Type B cell (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5.

Light-induced inward Na+ current and voltage-dependent outward K+ currents across the soma membrane of the Type B photoreceptor. Voltage clamp recordings from Type B photoreceptor. Top current recording shows inward Na+ current during a light step whose onset is indicated by arrow. Middle and bottom current recordings are of outward K+ currents (elicited in the dark by positive command pulses from –60 to –20 and from –60 to –10 mV), onsets also indicated by arrow (Alkon, 1982).

Fig. 5.

Light-induced inward Na+ current and voltage-dependent outward K+ currents across the soma membrane of the Type B photoreceptor. Voltage clamp recordings from Type B photoreceptor. Top current recording shows inward Na+ current during a light step whose onset is indicated by arrow. Middle and bottom current recordings are of outward K+ currents (elicited in the dark by positive command pulses from –60 to –20 and from –60 to –10 mV), onsets also indicated by arrow (Alkon, 1982).

  1. An early rapidly-activating and inactivating outward K+ current (Shoukimas & Alkon, 1980; Alkon, Lederhendler & Shoukimas, 1982zz), blocked by 1-3 mmol 1-1 4-aminopyridine. It resembles IA currents previously identified (Connor & Stevens, 1971).

  2. A slowly-activating outward K+ current with much less inactivation, blocked by lOOmmoll-1 tetraethylammonium ion. It is similar to delayed rectifying currents described for many neurones (Shoukimas & Alkon, 1980).

  3. A slowly-activating outward K+ current with substantial and prolonged inactivation (Fig. 6), which depends on elevation of intracellular Ca2+ and is therefore blocked by injection of Ca2+-chelators such as ethylene glycocol tetracetic acid. It can thus be termed a Caz+-dependent K+ current or Ic (Alkon, Farley, Hay & Shoukimas, 1983b; Alkon, Farley, Sakakibara & Hay, 1984).

  4. A rapidly-activating voltage-dependent inward Ca2+ current which shows no inactivation and is blocked by 2-4mmol I-1 Cd2+ (Fig. 7; Alkon et al. 1983b, 1984).

  5. A light-induced inward Na+ current which rapidly inactivates (Fig. 5; Alkon, 1979).

  6. A light-induced outward K+ current which substantially inactivates and results from a light-induced release of intracellular Ca2+ (Fig. 8). This current differs from the third one in that it arises from a light-induced rather than a voltage-dependent rise of intracellular Ca2+.

Fig. 6.

(A) Voltage-dependent outward currents across the membrane of the isolated Type B cell soma. From left to right, ASW, 3 mmol I-1 4-aminopyridine (4-AP) added to ASW, 4-AP and 100 mmol I-1 tetraethylammonium (TEA) ion added to ASW. Note that addition of 4-AP and TEA remove only a small portion of the late outward current elicited by command to 0 mV from a holding potential of -60 mV. The dashed lines indicate the level of the non-voltage-dependent or ‘leak’ current. (B) Voltage-dependent outward current in presence of 4-AP and TEA. Voltage-dependent activation of outward calcium-dependent K+ current (Ic) in Type B photoreceptor, and reduction by pre-pulse depolarization. Current (top) and voltage (bottom) records from voltage-clamp experiment illustrate that a 3 s depolarization to OmV elicits a small net outward current (top left), followed by a larger outward current when the membrane is stepped to +20mV (top middle). The pre-pulse depolarization reduced by –40 % the outward current normally evoked by an 80 mV step from –60 to +20 mV (top right) and slowed the rise time as well. Bathing solutions included 10 mmol I-1 4-AP and 100 mmol F1 TEA to block the fast (IA) and delayed (IK) K+ currents (Alkon, Farley & Hay, 1984).

Fig. 6.

(A) Voltage-dependent outward currents across the membrane of the isolated Type B cell soma. From left to right, ASW, 3 mmol I-1 4-aminopyridine (4-AP) added to ASW, 4-AP and 100 mmol I-1 tetraethylammonium (TEA) ion added to ASW. Note that addition of 4-AP and TEA remove only a small portion of the late outward current elicited by command to 0 mV from a holding potential of -60 mV. The dashed lines indicate the level of the non-voltage-dependent or ‘leak’ current. (B) Voltage-dependent outward current in presence of 4-AP and TEA. Voltage-dependent activation of outward calcium-dependent K+ current (Ic) in Type B photoreceptor, and reduction by pre-pulse depolarization. Current (top) and voltage (bottom) records from voltage-clamp experiment illustrate that a 3 s depolarization to OmV elicits a small net outward current (top left), followed by a larger outward current when the membrane is stepped to +20mV (top middle). The pre-pulse depolarization reduced by –40 % the outward current normally evoked by an 80 mV step from –60 to +20 mV (top right) and slowed the rise time as well. Bathing solutions included 10 mmol I-1 4-AP and 100 mmol F1 TEA to block the fast (IA) and delayed (IK) K+ currents (Alkon, Farley & Hay, 1984).

Fig. 7.

Steady-state current-voltage plot of voltage-dependent inward current present in Type B photoreceptor. Under conditions of high external Ba2+ (100 mmol I-1) and blockade of the voltagedependent K+ currents (○ – . – . – ○), the current is inward over the range of –40 to +40mV (absolute), reaching its peak at ~0 mV. Removal of Ba2+ and Ca2+ from the bath, and addition of the calcium-channel blocker Cd (10 mmol I’1) substantially reduced after 5 min the inward current at all levels of membrane potential (△ —— △). The residual deviations from the extrapolated leak current values (◻ – – – ◻) may be due to incomplete block of the inward current (Alkon, Farley & Hay, 1984).

Fig. 7.

Steady-state current-voltage plot of voltage-dependent inward current present in Type B photoreceptor. Under conditions of high external Ba2+ (100 mmol I-1) and blockade of the voltagedependent K+ currents (○ – . – . – ○), the current is inward over the range of –40 to +40mV (absolute), reaching its peak at ~0 mV. Removal of Ba2+ and Ca2+ from the bath, and addition of the calcium-channel blocker Cd (10 mmol I’1) substantially reduced after 5 min the inward current at all levels of membrane potential (△ —— △). The residual deviations from the extrapolated leak current values (◻ – – – ◻) may be due to incomplete block of the inward current (Alkon, Farley & Hay, 1984).

Fig. 8.

Light-induced Ca2+-dependent K+ current. Current records (in response to command depolarizations indicated on the left) obtained during voltage clamp of an isolated Type B photoreceptor soma in 0Na+, 300 mmol I-1 K+-artificial sea water. The light-induced current is inward below the equilibrium potential for potassium flux, approximately 0 mV. At 0 mV there is no appreciable light-induced current. This current was reduced by intracellular injection of EGTA but not affected by 5 mmol I’1 Cd24” in the external bathing medium, lowering of external Ca2+ or Na+, or substitution of Ba2+ for Ca2+. The trace beneath the current records indicates the duration of the light stimulus (–-10-3 Jcm-2s-1 at 510nm).

Fig. 8.

Light-induced Ca2+-dependent K+ current. Current records (in response to command depolarizations indicated on the left) obtained during voltage clamp of an isolated Type B photoreceptor soma in 0Na+, 300 mmol I-1 K+-artificial sea water. The light-induced current is inward below the equilibrium potential for potassium flux, approximately 0 mV. At 0 mV there is no appreciable light-induced current. This current was reduced by intracellular injection of EGTA but not affected by 5 mmol I’1 Cd24” in the external bathing medium, lowering of external Ca2+ or Na+, or substitution of Ba2+ for Ca2+. The trace beneath the current records indicates the duration of the light stimulus (–-10-3 Jcm-2s-1 at 510nm).

‘Blind’ experiments on Type B somata indicate that increase of excitability induced by conditioning arises from a persistent decrease of voltage-dependent K+ current (Fig. 9; Alkon et al. 1982a; Forman et al. 1984). These include the IA and the Ic*1+ - K+ outward K+ currents. A number of other studies were undertaken to determine some of the biophysical steps leading to this long-term reduction of K+ currents. It was already known that progressively enhanced light-evoked depolarization and cumulative membrane depolarization of the Type B cell occurs during acquisition of associative learning (Alkon, 1980b). During prolonged depolarization, prolonged elevation of intracellular Ca2+ was demonstrated by differential absorption spectrophotometry (Connor & Alkon, 1982, 1984). Elevation of intracellular Ca2+ by injection of Ca2+ causes prolonged reduction of both IA (Fig. 10; Alkon, Shoukimas & Heldman, 1982b) and currents (Alkon et al. 1984; Alkon & Sakakibara, 1984).

Fig. 9.

Outward K+ currents of Type B cell (A and B). Outward currents elicited by command pulses to OmV. Initial peaks are the early rapidly inactivating K+ currents (I/O-Late outward K+ currents, largely ICa2+K+ attain a maximum value approximately 300 ms after the onset of the command pulse. Paired I* is smaller than random IA for both first and second command pulses. The ratio of paired I A for second pulse to IA for the first pulse is smaller for paired than for random. Cartoon depicts impalement of medial Type B cell with two microelectrodes (Alkon, Lederhendler & ShoukimaB, 1982a).

Fig. 9.

Outward K+ currents of Type B cell (A and B). Outward currents elicited by command pulses to OmV. Initial peaks are the early rapidly inactivating K+ currents (I/O-Late outward K+ currents, largely ICa2+K+ attain a maximum value approximately 300 ms after the onset of the command pulse. Paired I* is smaller than random IA for both first and second command pulses. The ratio of paired I A for second pulse to IA for the first pulse is smaller for paired than for random. Cartoon depicts impalement of medial Type B cell with two microelectrodes (Alkon, Lederhendler & ShoukimaB, 1982a).

Fig. 10.

(A) Positive command pulses paired (upper records) and unpaired (lower records) with light steps. Each command step to 0 mV is followed after 30 gs by a brief step to +10 mV. The interstimulus interval illustrated here was used for paired vs unpaired comparisons (Alkon, Shoukimas & Heldman, 19826). (B) Rates of IA decrease and recovery during and following repetitive command depolarizations. Current responses to only three of the first five depolarizing steps used are shown. The steps following arrows were given at 40-s intervals after the five depolarizing steps used for quantitation of differences. These five command depolarizations (2·2 s) to 0 mV occurred with a cycle time of 4-0 s. Each 2-2s step was followed by a second command (800ms) to –10 mV. (B) Command depolarizations paired with light. A light step (2-0 s) was presented 150 ms after the onset of each command depolarization. Light intensity: 10-1·5 Jcm-2s. (C) Command depolarizations alone. Arrows indicate 40-s intervals. Lower rectangular traces under B and C indicate onset of 60 mV command steps. Note that IA (peak-inward currents) decreases to lower values in B than in C, and peak IA takes minutes to return to original values in B but not in C. The first three currents included in B and C are the first, second and fifth currents elicited by the five successive command steps (Alkon, Lederhendler & Shoukimas, 1982a).

Fig. 10.

(A) Positive command pulses paired (upper records) and unpaired (lower records) with light steps. Each command step to 0 mV is followed after 30 gs by a brief step to +10 mV. The interstimulus interval illustrated here was used for paired vs unpaired comparisons (Alkon, Shoukimas & Heldman, 19826). (B) Rates of IA decrease and recovery during and following repetitive command depolarizations. Current responses to only three of the first five depolarizing steps used are shown. The steps following arrows were given at 40-s intervals after the five depolarizing steps used for quantitation of differences. These five command depolarizations (2·2 s) to 0 mV occurred with a cycle time of 4-0 s. Each 2-2s step was followed by a second command (800ms) to –10 mV. (B) Command depolarizations paired with light. A light step (2-0 s) was presented 150 ms after the onset of each command depolarization. Light intensity: 10-1·5 Jcm-2s. (C) Command depolarizations alone. Arrows indicate 40-s intervals. Lower rectangular traces under B and C indicate onset of 60 mV command steps. Note that IA (peak-inward currents) decreases to lower values in B than in C, and peak IA takes minutes to return to original values in B but not in C. The first three currents included in B and C are the first, second and fifth currents elicited by the five successive command steps (Alkon, Lederhendler & Shoukimas, 1982a).

The biophysical sequence (Fig. 11) underlying the transformation of the Type B membrane during learning is most probably as follows: repeated pairings of light and rotation elicit cumulative membrane depolarization. This arises from (1) the genetically specified synaptic organization and (2) the response of this network during the interaction of the animal with its environment during the training stimuli. Cumulative depolarization is accompanied by prolonged elevation of the intracellular calcium within the soma, and perhaps in the axon and terminal branches. Elevated calcium causes inactivation of voltage-dependent K+ currents (IA and most probably ) which outlasts by many days the cumulative depolarization and elevation of intracellular Ca2+. Reduction of voltage-dependent K+ currents increases the excitability thus permitting an enhanced depolarization to a light stimulus. The greater depolarization and the greater number of impulses that will be generated cause (1) greater inhibition of the medial Type A photoreceptor, (2) increased inhibition of ipsilateral interneurones, (3) decreased depolarization of ipsilateral motor neurones and finally (4) decreased turning toward a light source.

Fig. 11.

(A) Regenerative synaptic and light induced excitation of the Type B photoreceptor. Light-induced depolarization facilitates synaptic excitation and vice-versa in response to temporally associated light and rotation. Analysed in biophysical terms, synaptic depolarization causes inactivation of IA and ICa2+K+ and enhancement of a voltage-dependent Ca2+ current. Increased intracellular Ca2+ causes further inactivation of IA and ICa2+K+ - K+ and thus a further increase of effective input resistance. These in turn cause more membrane depolarization. (B) Absorbance changes at 660-690 (monitoring intracellular Ca2+) and 630-690 nm wavelength pairs (top and bottom records) and membrane voltage response (middle) following a 0·3 s light flash (Connor & Alkon, 1984).

Fig. 11.

(A) Regenerative synaptic and light induced excitation of the Type B photoreceptor. Light-induced depolarization facilitates synaptic excitation and vice-versa in response to temporally associated light and rotation. Analysed in biophysical terms, synaptic depolarization causes inactivation of IA and ICa2+K+ and enhancement of a voltage-dependent Ca2+ current. Increased intracellular Ca2+ causes further inactivation of IA and ICa2+K+ - K+ and thus a further increase of effective input resistance. These in turn cause more membrane depolarization. (B) Absorbance changes at 660-690 (monitoring intracellular Ca2+) and 630-690 nm wavelength pairs (top and bottom records) and membrane voltage response (middle) following a 0·3 s light flash (Connor & Alkon, 1984).

The change from transient (lasting minutes to hours) cumulative depolarization and elevated intracellular Ca2+ to a longer lasting reduction of open voltagedependent K+ channels can most reasonably be thought to be mediated by biochemical steps. Ca2+-calmodulin phosphorylation of proteins which regulate or are a part of the K+ channels themselves is implicated. Electrophoretic analysis revealed a difference in phosphorylation of low molecular weight proteins from the eyes of conditioned but not control Hermissenda (Neary, Crow&Alkon, 1981). Phosphorylation of these proteins was in addition shown to be Ca2+-dependent (Alkon, 1983). Phosphorylation of one of these proteins (25 000 Mr) also changed under conditions which inactivated the same voltage-dependent K+ current (IA) that was reduced during days on which the associative learning was retained (Neary & Alkon, 1983). Finally, iontophoresis of a Ca2+-calmodulin-dependent protein kinase (phosphorylase kinase) caused a long-lasting reduction of the IA current (Fig. 12) which required some initial elevation of intracellular Ca2+ (Acosta-Urquidi, Alkon & Neary, 1982; Acosta-Urquidi, Neary & Alkon, 1984).

Fig. 12.

Family of K+ currents (IA traces at left and matched IB traces at right) illustrate the effect of a single PhK (phosphorylase kinase) injection (60nC) under two conditions: in darkness (Dark) and after a 20-s pairing (Pair 1 -3) of a light step with a variable command pulse to deliver a Ca2+ load. Control, control before injection. IA was elicited from a VH of –60 mV, and decays within ~1 s to the steady state. 1·9 s later, a second superimposed command pulse ( + 10 mV) elicits IB (delayed K+ current) uncontaminated by IA. (Full record only shown for control condition.) Note no effect of PhK injection under dark conditions but a reduction of IA and a small (but not statistically significant) reduction of IB after the first pairing to a +50mV command (Pair 1, 50D). Further reduction of IA and also IB occurred after two additional pairings associated with increasing command steps (60 and 75 D, respectively) (Acosta-Urquidi, Alkon & Neary, 1984).

Fig. 12.

Family of K+ currents (IA traces at left and matched IB traces at right) illustrate the effect of a single PhK (phosphorylase kinase) injection (60nC) under two conditions: in darkness (Dark) and after a 20-s pairing (Pair 1 -3) of a light step with a variable command pulse to deliver a Ca2+ load. Control, control before injection. IA was elicited from a VH of –60 mV, and decays within ~1 s to the steady state. 1·9 s later, a second superimposed command pulse ( + 10 mV) elicits IB (delayed K+ current) uncontaminated by IA. (Full record only shown for control condition.) Note no effect of PhK injection under dark conditions but a reduction of IA and a small (but not statistically significant) reduction of IB after the first pairing to a +50mV command (Pair 1, 50D). Further reduction of IA and also IB occurred after two additional pairings associated with increasing command steps (60 and 75 D, respectively) (Acosta-Urquidi, Alkon & Neary, 1984).

The essential link between the stimulus pattern (here paired light and rotation) and the change of ionic channels with learning is the training-induced voltage change of the Type B cell. This voltage change is an integrated effect of the entire visualstatocyst network as it responds to pattern repetition. This voltage change, and the calcium-mediated reduction of K+ channels it causes, contrasts with other neurohumorally-mediated phenomena. Such generalized physiological states as arousal are accompanied in vertebrates, for example, by increased levels of circulating epinephrine that exerts diffuse effects via neural receptors. In recent years neurohumoral agents such as octopamine, dopamine and serotonin have been suggested to have a similar role in invertebrates. Serotonin and octopamine, for example, have profound effects on the development and the ability to sustain muscular tension fcrayfish (Jacobs & Atwood, 1981), and on neuromuscular junctions of the lobster (Kravitz et al. 1980). Serotonin also produces effects on Aplysia similar to those observed after delivering a noxious stimulus such as shock or a ‘pinch’ (Camarado, Siegelbaum & Kandel, 1984).

These effects of pharmacological agents differ in many respects from the learning of an association between two specific stimuli as analysed for Hermissenda. First, a learned stimulus association is quite specific, while ‘state’ changes elicited by neurohumoral agents are generalized to many targets throughout the body. Second, the associative learning in Hermissenda persists for many weeks, while the pharmacological effects do not outlast the presence of the neurohumoral agent. Thus, there is no evidence that the neurohumorally-induced cellular changes involve storage of information, which persists beyond the initial experience of, for example, an arousal state. Nor mechanistically would the persistence of arousal effects be useful for the animal unless the arousing stimuli had been associated with other temporally-related events. Moreover, the effects of serotonin and octopamine are mediated by cyclic nucleotide phosphorylation (Batelle & Kravitz, 1978) rather than Ca2+-calmodulin-dependent phosphorylations involved in the learned association in Hermissenda (Acosta-Urquidi et al. 1982, 1984; Alkon et al. 1983a). The serotonin-induced effects on Aplysia neurones are also independent of calcium (Siegelbaum, Camardo & Kandel, 1982). It may, however, be premature to compare directly calcium-dependent modification of Hermissenda membrane channels during associative learning with mechanisms of arousal and sensitization. Thus, although serotonin has been thought to be responsible for long-term reduction of potassium channels during sensitization, their prolonged reduction has never been correlated with long-lasting sensitization of intact animals. Furthermore, it has been recently shown that serotonin is not released or synthesized from presynaptic interneurones which have long been thought to convey the sensitizing effects on the synaptic endings of the sensory cell (Hawkins et al. 1981; Kistler et al. 1983).

The integrated response of a population of neurones during conditioning results in long-term (days) changes of specific membrane currents within identified neurones. Prolonged elevation of intracellular calcium during conditioning causes a persistent increase of excitability by reducing K+ currents (IA and probably I(v+ - K+) in the membranes of identified somata. This Ca2+-mediated reduction of K+ currents, which encodes a learned stimulus association is thought to involve changes of Ca2+ -calmodulin - dependent phosphorylation of distinct membrane proteins. These changes are contrasted with the short-term regulation of currents by neurohormones during altered behavioural states such as arousal.

Ultimately some intelligent and testable extrapolation from relatively simple to more complex nervous systems may become possible. Most promising to date are the properties of ionic channels within neural membranes. The voltage-dependent calcium channels of the Hermissenda Type B photoreceptors have been found in Paramecium (Naitoh & Eckert, 1974), spinal cord motor neurones (Barrett & Barrett, 1976), dendrites of inferior olive neurones (Llinas & Yarom, 1981a,b) and pyramidal cells of the hippocampus (Clark & Wong, 1983). Voltage-dependent K+ currents such as IA and Ica2+-K+, are equally widespread. Prolonged membrane depolarization is known to occur in vertebrate nervous systems and could modify membrane excitability in exactly the same way as in Hermissenda. The necessary physiological prerequisites exist within vertebrate neural systems to use an invertebrate mechanism for associative learning. It will be extremely interesting to induce prolonged depolarization in cells with these prerequisites in brain slices to see if prolonged excitability changes, analogous to those of Hermissenda, would result. Encouragement has already been provided by the finding of prolonged elevation of intracellular calcium in somata of hippocampal pyramidal cells in response to a depolarizing stimulus (Krnjevic, Morris, Reifenstein & Ropert, 1982). It will be far more difficult, of course, to correlate such biophysical changes with associative learning of a cat or dog and then to prove that these changes are intrinsic to particular neurones. It will more difficult still to reach any conclusion as to what portion of a discrete association learned by a cat or dog resides within a given set of neurones and is critical for its recall.

Acosta-Urquidi
,
J.
,
Alkon
,
D. L.
&
Neary
,
J. T.
(
1984
).
Intrasomatic injection of a Ca2+-calmodulin dependent protein kinase simulates biophysical effects of associative learning in i Hermissenda photoreceptor
.
Science,N.Y. (in press)
.
Acosta-Urquidi
,
J.
,
Neary
,
J. T.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1982
).
Ca++ calmodulin dependent protein kinase effects on voltage-dependent K+ currents
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr
.
8
,
825
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1973
).
Intersensory interactions in Hermissenda
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
62
,
185
202
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1974a
).
Sensory interactions in the nudibranch mollusc Hermissenda crassicorms
.
Fedn Proc. Fedn Am. Socs exp. Biol
.
33
,
1083
1090
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1974b
).
Associative training of Hermissenda
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
64
,
70
84
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1979
).
Voltage-dependent calcium and potassium ion conductances: A contingency mechanism for an associative learning model
.
Science,N.Y
.
205
,
810
816
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1980a
).
Cellular analysis of a gastropod (Hermissenda crassicorns) model of associative learning
.
Biol. Bull. mar. biol. Lab.,Woods Hole
159
,
505
560
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1980b
).
Membrane depolarization accumulates during acquisition of an associative behavioral change
.
Science,N.Y
.
210
,
1375
1376
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1982
).
A biophysical basis for molluscan associative learning
.
In Conditioning: Representation of Involved Neural Functions
, (ed.
C. D.
Woody
) p. 147.
New York
:
Plenum Press
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1983
).
Learning in a marine snail
.
Scient. Am
.
249
,
70
84
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
,
Acosta-Urquidi
,
J.
,
Olds
,
J.
,
Kuzma
,
G.
&
Neary
,
J. T.
(
1983a
).
Protein kinase injection reduces voltage dependent potassium currents
.
Science,N.Y
.
219
,
303
306
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
,
Akaike
,
T.
&
Harrigan
,
J. F.
(
1978
).
Interaction of chemosensory, visual and statocyst pathways in Hermissenda
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
71
,
177
194
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
,
Farley
,
J.
,
Sakakibara
,
M.
&
Hay
,
B.
(
1984
).
Voltage-dependent calcium and calcium-activated potassium currents of a molluscan photoreceptor
.
Biophys. J. (in press)
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
,
Farley
,
J.
,
Hay
,
B.
&
Shoukimas
,
J. J.
(
1983b
).
Inactivation of Ca++ dependent K+ current can occur without significant Ca++ current inactivation
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr
.
9
,
1188
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
&
Fuortes
,
M. G. F.
(
1972
).
Responses of photoreceptors in Hermissenda
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
60
,
631
649
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
,
Lederhendler
,
I.
&
Shoukimas
,
J. J.
(
1982a
).
Primary changes of membrane currents during retention of associative learning
.
Science,N.Y
.
215
,
693
695
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
&
Sakakibara
,
M.
(
1984
).
Prolonged inactivation of a Ca2+-dependent K+ current but not Ca2+ current by light induced elevation of intracellular calcium
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. (in press)
.
Alkon
,
D. L.
,
Shoukimas
,
J. J.
&
Hkldman
,
E.
(
1982b
).
Calcium-mediated decrease of a voltage-dependent potassium current
.
Biophys. J
.
40
,
245
250
.
Barrett
,
E. F.
&
Barrett
,
J. N.
(
1976
).
Separation of two voltage-sensitive potassium currents, and demonstration of a tetrodotoxin-resistant calcium current in frog motorneurones
.
J. Physiol.,lond
.
255
,
737
774
.
Batelle
,
B. A.
&
Kravitz
,
E. A.
(
1978
).
Targets of octopamine action in the lobster: cyclic nucleotide changes and physiological effects in haemolymph, heart and exoskeletal muscle
.
J. Pharmac. exp. Ther
.
205
,
438
448
.
Berger
,
T. W.
,
Alger
,
B. E.
&
Thompson
,
R. F.
(
1976
).
Neuronal substrate of classical conditioning in the hippocampus
.
Science,N.Y
.
192
,
483
485
.
Berger
,
T. W.
&
Thompson
,
R. F.
(
1977
).
Limbic system interrelations: functional division among hippocampal-septal connections
.
Science,N.Y
.
197
,
587
589
.
Berger
,
T. W.
&
Thompson
,
R. F.
(
1978
).
Identification of pyramidal cells as the critical elements in hippocampal neuronal plasticity during learning
.
Proc,natn Acad. Sci. U.S A
..
75
,
1572
1576
.
Brons
,
J.
&
Woody
,
C. D.
(
1980
).
Long term changes in excitability of cortical neurons after Pavlovian conditioning and extinction
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
44
,
605
615
.
Camarado
,
J. S.
,
Siegelbaum
,
S.
&
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1984
).
Cellular and molecular correlates of sensitization in Aplysia and their implications for associative learning
.
In Primary Neural Substrates of Learning and Behavioral Change
, (eds
D. L.
Alkon
&
J.
Farley
).
New York
:
Cambridge University Press, (in press
).
Carew
,
T. J.
,
Hawkins
,
R. D.
&
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1983
).
Differential classical conditioning of a defensive withdrawal reflex in Aplysia califomica
.
Science,N.Y
.
219
,
397
400
.
Carew
,
T. J.
,
Walters
,
E. T.
&
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1981
).
Classical conditioning in a simple withdrawal reflex in Aplysia califomica
.
J. Neurosci
.
1
,
1426
1437
.
Castellucci
,
V. F.
,
Pinsker
,
H. M.
,
Kupferman
,
I.
&
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1970
).
Neuronal mechanisms of habituation and dishabituation of the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia
.
Science,N.Y
.
167
,
1745
1748
.
Clark
,
R. B.
&
Wong
,
R. K. S.
(
1983
).
Three components of outward current in isolated mammalian cortical F neuron"
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr
.
9
,
601
.
Cohen
,
D. H.
&
Macdonald
,
R. L.
(
1976
).
Involvement of the avian hypothalamus in defensively conditioned heart rate change
.
J. comp. Neurol
.
167
,
465
480
.
Connor
,
J. A.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1982
).
Voltage and light-dependence of intracellular Arsenazo signals
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr
.
8
,
944
.
Connor
,
J. A.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1984
).
Light- and voltage-dependent increases of calcium ion concentration in molluscan photoreceptors
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
51
,
745
752
.
Connor
,
J. A.
&
Stevens
,
C. F.
(
1971
).
Voltage clamp studies of a transient outward current in gastropod neural somata
.
J. Physiol.,Lond
.
213
,
21
30
.
Crow
,
T. J.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1978
).
Retention of an associative behavioral change inHermissenda crassicomis
.
Science,N.Y
.
201
,
1239
1241
.
Crow
,
T. J.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1980
).
Associative behavioral modification in Hermissenda: cellular correlates
.
Science,N.Y
.
209
,
412
414
.
Farley
,
J.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1980
).
Neural organization predicts stimulus specificity for a retained associative behavioral change
.
Science,N.Y
.
210
,
1373
1375
.
Farley
,
J.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1982
).
Associative neural and behavioral change inHermissenda: Consequences of nervous system orientation for light- and pairing specificity
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
48
,
785
807
.
Farley
,
J.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1984
).
Cellular analysis of gastropod learning
.
In Invertebrate Receptors
, (ed.
A. H.
Greenberg
).
New York
:
Marcel Dekker, (in press
).
Farley
,
J.
&
Kern
,
G.
(
1983
).
Contingency-sensitive behavioral change in Hermissenda: temporally-specific attenuation of conditioning
.
Anim. Learn. Behav. (in press)
.
Farley
,
J.
,
Richards
,
W. G.
,
Ling
,
L. J.
,
Liman
,
E.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1983
).
Membrane changes in a single photoreceptor cause associative learning in Hermissenda
.
Science,N.Y
.
221
,
1201
1203
.
Forman
,
R.
,
Alkon
,
D. L.
,
Sakakjbara
,
M.
,
Harrigan
,
J.
,
Lederhendler
,
I.
&
Farley
,
J.
(
1984
).
Changes in IA and Ic but not INA accompany retention of conditioned behaviour in Hermissenda
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. (in press)
.
Gelperin
,
A.
,
Wieland
,
S. J.
&
Barry
,
S. R.
(
1984
).
A strategy for cellular analysis of associative learning in a terrestrial mollusk
.
In Primary Neural Substrates of Learning and Behavioral Change
, (eds
D. L.
Alkon
&
J.
Farley
).
New York
:
Cambridge University Press, (in press
).
Gillette
,
R.
&
Davis
,
W. J.
(
1977
).
The role of the metacerebral giant neuron in the feeding behavior of Pieurobranchaea
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
166
,
129
159
.
Gillette
,
R.
,
Gillette
,
M. V.
&
Davis
,
W. J.
(
1980
).
Action-potential broadening and endogenously sustained bursting are substrates of command ability in a feeding neuron of Pieurobranchaea
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
43
,
669
685
.
Goh
,
Y.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1984
).
Sensory, intemeuronal and motor interactions within the Hermissenda visual pathway
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
52
,
156
169
.
Hawkins
,
R. D.
,
Castellucci
,
V. F.
&
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1981
).
Interneurons involved in mediation and modulation of gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. II. Identified neurons produce heterosynaptic facilitation contributing to behavioral sensitization
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
45
,
315
.
Hoyle
,
G.
(
1982
).
Cellular basis of operant conditioning of leg position
.
In Conditioning: Representation of Involved Neurid Functions
, (ed.
C. D.
Woody
), p.
197
.
New York
:
Plenum Press
.
Jacobs
,
J. R.
&
Atwood
,
H. L.
(
1981
).
Long term facilitation of tension in crustacean muscle and its modulation by temperature, activity and circulating amines
.
J, comp. Physiol
.
144
,
335
343
.
Kandel
,
E. R.
&
Schwartz
,
J. H.
(
1982
).
Molecular biology of learning: modulation of transmitter release
.
Science,N.Y
.
218
,
433
443
.
Kanz
,
J. E.
,
Eberley
,
L. B.
,
Cobbs
,
J. S.
&
Pinsker
,
H. M.
(
1979
).
Neuronal correlates of siphon withdrawal in freely behaving Aplysia
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
42
,
1538
1556
.
Kistler
,
H. B.
,
Hawkins
,
R. D.
,
Kuester
,
J.
,
Kandel
,
E. R.
&
Schwartz
,
J. H.
(
1983
).
Immunocytochemical studies of neurons producing presynaptic facilitation in the abdominal ganglion of Aplysia califomica
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr
.
9
,
915
.
Kravitz
,
E. A.
,
Glusman
,
S.
,
Harris-Warrick
,
R. M.
,
Livingstone
,
M. S.
,
Schwartz
,
T.
&
Goy
,
M. F.
(
1980
).
Amines and a peptide as neurohormones in lobsters: action on neuromuscular preparations and preliminary behavioural studies
.
J. exp. Biol
.
89
,
159
175
.
Krnjevic
,
K.
,
Morris
,
M. E.
,
Reifenstein
,
R. J.
&
Robert
,
N.
(
1982
).
Depth distribution and mechanism of changes in extracellular K+ and Ca2+ concentrations in the hippocampus
.
Can. J. Physiol. Pharmac
.
60
,
1658
1671
.
Lederhendler
,
I.
,
Gart
,
S.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1983
).
Associative learning in Hermissenda crassicomis (Gastropoda): evidence that light (the CS) takes on characteristics of rotation (the UCS)
.
Biol. Bull. mar. biol. Lab.,Woods Hole,Abstr
.
165
,
528
.
Lederhendler
,
I. L
,
Goh
,
Y.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1982
).
Type B photoreceptor changes predict modification of motomeuron responses to light during retention of Hermissenda associative conditioning
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr
.
8
,
824
.
Llinas
,
R.
&
Yarom
,
Y.
(
1981a
).
Electrophysiology of mammalian inferior olivary neurones in vitro. Different types of voltage-dependent ionic conductances
.
J. Physiol.,Lond
.
315
,
549
567
.
Llinas
,
R.
&
Yarom
,
Y.
(
1981b
).
Properties and distribution of ionic conductances generating electrorespom siveness of mammalian inferior olivary neurones in vitro
.
J. Physiol.,Lond
.
315
,
569
584
.
Mpitsos
,
G. J.
&
Collins
,
S. D.
(
1975
).
Learning: rapid aversive conditioning in the gastropod mollusc Pleurobranchaea
.
Science,N.Y
.
188
,
954
956
.
Naitoh
,
Y.
&
Eckert
,
R.
(
1974
).
The control of ciliary activity in protozoa
.
In Cilia and Flagella
, (ed.
M.
Sleigh
).
New York
:
Academic Press
.
Neary
,
J. T.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1983
).
Protein phosphorylation/dephosphorylation and the transient, voltagedependent potassium conductance in Hermissenda crassicomis
.
J. biol. Chem
.
258
,
8979
8983
.
Neary
,
J. T.
,
Crow
,
T. J.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1981
).
Change in a specific phosphoprotein band following associative learning in Hermissenda
.
Nature,Lond
.
293
,
658
660
.
Richards
,
W.
,
Farley
,
J.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1983
).
Extinction of associative learning in Hermissenda-. Behavior and neural correlates
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr
.
9
,
916
.
Sahley
,
C.
,
Rudy
,
J. W.
&
Gelperin
,
A.
(
1984
).
Associative learning in a mollusk: A comparative analysis
.
In Primary Neural Substrates of Learning and Behavioral Change
, (eds
D. L.
Alkon
&
J.
Farley
).
New York
:
Cambridge University Press, (in press
).
Shoukimas
,
J. J.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1980
).
Voltage-dependent, early outward current in a photoreceptor of Hermissenda crassicomis
.
Soc. Neurosci. Abstr
.
6
,
17
.
Siegelbaum
,
S. A.
,
Camarado
,
J. S.
&
Kandel
,
E. R.
(
1982
).
Serotonin and cyclic AMP close single potassium channels in Aplysia sensory neurons
.
Nature,Lond
.
299
,
413
417
.
Thompson
,
R. F.
,
Berger
,
T. W.
,
Berry
,
S. D.
,
Clark
,
G. A.
,
Kettner
,
R. N.
,
Lavond
,
D. G.
,
Mauk
,
M. D.
,
Mccormick
,
D. A.
,
Solomon
,
P. R.
&
Weisz
,
D. J.
(
1982
).
Neuronal substrates of learning and memory: hippocampus and other structures
.
In Conditioning: Representation of Involved Neural Functions
, (ed.
C. D.
Woody
), pp.
115
129
.
New York
:
Plenum Press
.
West
,
A.
,
Barnes
,
E. S.
&
Alkon
,
D. L.
(
1982
).
Primary changes of voltage responses during retention of associative learning
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
48
,
1243
1255
.
Woody
,
C. D.
(
1982
).
Memory,Learning and Higher Function
.
New York
:
Springer-Verlag
.
Woody
,
C. D.
&
Black-Cleworth
,
P.
(
1973
).
Differences in excitability of cortical neurons as a function of motor projection in conditioned cats
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
36
,
1104
1116
.
Woody
,
C. D.
,
Vassilevsky
,
N. N.
&
Engel
,
J.
(
1970
).
Conditioned eye blink: unit activity at coronal-precruciate cortex of the cat
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
33
,
851
864
.
Woody
,
C. D.
,
Yarowsky
,
P.
,
Owens
,
J.
,
Black-Cleworth
,
P.
&
Crow
,
T.
(
1974
).
Effect of lesions of cortical motor areas on acquisition of eyeblink in the cat
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
37
,
385
394
.