1. The response of Onchidoris fusca to monochromatic light in the range 400·650 m· was measured in behaviour experiments with two opposing lights.

  2. The peak sensitivity was found to lie at 500−505 . The thick, brown, dorsal skin of Onchidoris has the effect of reducing the sensitivity of the eye to ultraviolet and blue light.

  3. After allowing for a 90% absorption of light by the dorsal skin, the absolute threshold of the naked eye of Onchidoris (at 501 mμ) was found to lie between 81 and 106 quanta/sec./eye.

  4. If the lens and cornea absorb a further 10% of the available light, then each of five receptor cells in the eye of Onchidoris may be capable of responding to 12−18 quanta/sec. However, the effective threshold of the eye of Onchidoris is much higher, lying at 881−1148 quanta/sec.

In general three methods have been used to determine the spectral sensitivity of invertebrates. The first is to measure the behavioural response to light over a range of wavelengths and intensities. This can be achieved by using the movement of the whole animal as an index of responsiveness, as in Dendrocoelum (Marriott, 1958, 1960), Littorina (Charles, 1961), calliphora maggots (Strange, 1961) and barnacle pauplii (Siddle, 1968). Alternatively, response can be gauged from studying movement of parts of the body, such as the closing of the valves of Pecten (Cronly-Dillon, 1966), and the withdrawal of the siphons of Mya (Hecht, 1920−21) and Pholas (Hecht, 1927).

A second method is to record the electrical response produced by the eye after stimulation with equal intensities of light at different wavelengths. Electrophysiological responses have been measured directly from the eye in Hermissenda (Dennis, 1967), and from the optic nerve in Aplysia (Waser, 1968) and Otala (Gilliary & Wolbarsht, 1967).

The third method is mainly confined to studies on animals with large eyes. Here, the photolabile pigment of the eye is extracted and its absorption spectrum measured in vitro. With a pure solution of the photopigment the absorption spectrum can be read directly, but with an impure solution the absorption spectrum must be measured before and after exposure to light. A ‘difference spectrum’ due to the bleached photopigment is thus calculated. The spectral sensitivity of the rhodopsins of squids (Brown & Brown, 1958; Hubbard & St George, 1958; Kropf, Brown & Hubbard, 1959), octopuses (Brown & Brown, 1958; Kropf et al. 1959) and Eledone (Hamdorf, Schweimer & Taiiber, 1968) have been determined by various extraction techniques.

Comparison of the three methods may provide valuable information about the biology of the species studied. Whereas the chemical method can only show the sensitivity of the isolated pigment, and the electrophysiological technique that of the eye, the behavioural studies reveal the sensitivity of the entire animal as it is in the field. The present study is concerned with the spectral responses of the intact animal. The results are then compared with those found by different methods for related animals.

Animals

Onchidoris fusca is a dorid nudibranch with paired eyes sunk beneath thick pigmented skin. It is commonly found on sublittoral barnacle-encrusted mussels in the Menai Straits. Numerous specimens were collected at regular intervals during August-December 1967−8 from freshly dredged mussels at the Severnside Oyster Company (Bangor) by kind permission of Mr R. Baird.

Apparatus

The covered experimental tank measured 61 cm. long by 11·5 cm. wide by 14·5 cm. deep and was made of black Perspex. It was fitted inside a larger clear Perspex tank which served as an insulator when filled with a mixture of ice and water (Fig. 1).

The inner experimental tank was filled with fresh, clean sea water to a depth of 5 cm. before each experiment; the horizontally directed beams of light entered through two apertures of 3·8 cm. in diameter (Ci and Cii, Fig. 1) which were just covered by the seawater.

A ‘standard’ lamp (at Ci) was provided by an Osram sodium bulb (12 V., 100 W.) run through a Phywe universal choke from the mains supply. The sodium beam was concentrated with a collimating lens and dimmed when necessary with neutral density filters (Ilford, 5% instrument quality glass). At the opposite end of the tank (Cii) a second monochromatic light source was obtained with a fan-cooled Philips quartziodine projector lamp, used in conjunction with interference filters, two collimating lenses and neutral density filters. The interference filters covered the visible spectrum from 400 to 650 at intervals of 10−20 mµ filters of wavelength 409 (406), 419 (421), 442 (442) and 519 (519) were obtained from Grubb Parsons; those of wavelength 461 (462), 480 (480), 491 (491), 500 (501), 540 (538), 559 (562), 579 (581), 598 (601) and 649 (649) were made by Balzars. The figures in parentheses given above refer to the peak wavelengths of the interference filters measured by the spectrophotometer, and those outside parentheses are the maker’s specifications. The characteristics of the interference filters and the neutral density filters were measured on a Unicam S.P. 800 automatic spectrophotometer, and the values thus obtained were used in the present calculations. The intensity of the lamps combined with the various filters was measured in µvolts in the experimental tank with a bolometer (Hilger-Schwarz thermopile No. F.T. 17·1/454) connected to an Airmec ‘Galvamp’ galvanometer. All measurements were taken in air, and allowance was made for the differential absorption of light by sea water (Defant, 1961). The calculated flux of the undiminished sodium bulb in the centre of the tank when filled with sea water was 29·41 μW./cm.2; similarly the quartz-iodine lamp with the interference filter at 501 was 85·37 μW./cm.2.

No opal glass or frosted filter was used to scatter the image of the lamps (unlike Strange, 1961) because the eyes of Onchidoris are already covered with an opaque skin which prevents them from forming an image (Holmes, 1968).

Methods

Spectral sensitivity

Dark-adapted animals were placed in the centre of the tank and two horizontal lights of known wavelength and intensity were beamed upon them. Since Onchidoris has a positive response to light, the animals moved towards the end of the tank which was most brightly illuminated. In subsequent experiments the brighter lamp was dimmed in steps of 0·1–0·2 log units to a point where the majority of animals were attracted towards the opposite lamp. This procedure was repeated until the sodium lamp had been tested against all the different wavelengths provided by the interference filters, thus enabling a picture of the relative spectral sensitivity of the animal to be built up.

Between 20 and 50 animals were dark adapted for at least 1 hr., placed in the centre of the experimental tank and simultaneously illuminated by both standard and spectral lamps for 15 min. At the end of each experiment the animals which had moved more than 4 cm. away from the centre of the tank were regarded as being attracted to the lamps. The numbers of animals in each end of the tank were counted and expressed as a percentage to the total number in both ends, the ones in the centre being disregarded. The number of replicate experiments depended on the number of animals used in each, but at least 100 animals were tested at each intensity level.

For each filter the percentage response to the standard lamp was then plotted against the intensity of the spectral lamp (Fig. 2), and the intensity for each filter at which there was a 50% response to the standard lamp was used to plot a curve of relative spectral sensitivity on an equal energy basis (Fig. 3). For those wavelengths where the intensity of the standard lamp was reduced (402, 419, 442 and 649 mµ), calculations were made to bring the results to the same energy level as the other wavelengths. A graph of relative quantum sensitivity was plotted by dividing the 50% energy values (Fig. 2) for each filter (in ergs/sec./cm.2) by the energy of one quantum at that wavelength. Since this calculation gave a curve of negative slope, the quantum values were then expressed as a percentage of the minimum value at 501 mµ (Figs. 4, 5) to give a curve of percentage quantum sensitivity with a positive slope. When plotted linearly the percentage quantum sensitivity curve can be considered as a first approximation to the absorption spectrum of the photopigment of the eye (Strange,1961).

The thick dorsal skin of Onchidoris was measured on the S.P. 800 for its relative spectral absorption, and the figures produced were used to calculate the sensitivity curve for the naked eye.

Absolute threshold

To investigate the absolute threshold of Onchidoris, one lamp of wavelength 501 m/i was used to stimulate the animals, and the opposite aperture (Ci) was blocked. The intensity of the beam was successively reduced in steps of 0·1−0·5 log units. About 100 animals were tested in a series of 30-min. experiments at each light level. After the experiments at each light intensity had been completed, the number of animals which had reacted positively to the light was expressed as a percentage of the number of animals which moved during the experiments (Fig. 6). When the intensity of the beam fell below the sensitivity of the bolometer, the flux was calculated by extrapolation from a graph of percentage transmission of the neutral density filter against optical density. Quanta/sec./cm.2 at the threshold intensity were calculated from the threshold energy measured in ergs/sec/cm2, divided by the energy of one quantum at 501 mμ (hc/λ, where h = Planck’s constant; c = velocity of light; λ = wavelength in cm.). The effective threshold energy of the Onchidoris eye was then found by multiplying the threshold quantum value by the area of the aperture of the eye (diameter 100μ). The absolute threshold of the ‘naked’ Onchidoris eye was then found after allowance had been made for the percentage transmission of the dorsal skin (9·23%; Holmes, 1968).

Spectral sensitivity

For the interference filters used in this work the curves of percentage response to the standard lamp plotted against the intensity of the spectral lamp (Fig. 2) are generally sigmoid in shape, although the slopes vary. The wavelengths closest to 500 mμ require the smallest amounts of energy to complement the flux of the standard lamp. When the 50% response values are plotted against wavelength (Fig. 3) it becomes apparent that Onchidoris is particularly sensitive to blue-green light of 500·505 mμ, and that this sensitivity drops off quickly on either side of the peak, but declines more steeply towards the red end of the spectrum than towards the ultraviolet. The lower curve in Fig. 3 corresponds to the calculated 50% response values of the naked eye of Onchidoris. The brown skin of Onchidoris transmits more red light than blue (Holmes, 1968), and consequently the observed and calculated curves diverge more towards the shorter wavelengths. However, when expressed as percentages of the peak wavelength (501 mμ) the shapes of the two curves differ very little.

The percentage sensitivity curve plotted on a linear scale (Fig. 4) gives a bell-shaped curve, which according to Strange (1961), is typical of a photopigment of the rhodopsin group. If this curve is compared with that of a hypothetical ‘normal’ curve having a λmax at 500 mμ (Dartnell, 1953) it appears too steep and narrow (Fig. 4). However, Dartnell’s ‘Nomogram’ is the curve expected for a pure solution of extracted pigment and does not take pre-retinal absorption into account. It has been shown in man (Ludvigh & McCarthy, 1938), other vertebrates (Kennedy & Milkman, 1956) and cephalopods (Denton, 1960) that the cornea, lens and humour absorb a significant proportion of the light entering the eye, and that the lens in particular may differentially absorb light at the blue end of the spectrum. It is at the blue end of the spectrum that the Onchidoris action spectrum differs most widely from Dartnell’s ‘Nomogram’ (Fig. 4). In addition to pre-retinal absorption within the eye, there is the possible differential absorption of light by the pigmented connective tissue which covers the eyes and brain of many dorids (Holmes, 1968). This possibility has not been taken into account in these calculations. Such extra-optic absorption may account for the differences between the curves of Onchidoris and Hermissenda (Fig. 5); but the fact that the spectral sensitivity curves of the two species are essentially the same, and that they differ from the ‘Nomogram’ to a similar degree, suggests that it is something within nudibranch eyes which is masking the photopigment and causing this disparity (Figs. 4, 5). Hamdorf et al. (1968) measured the spectral sensitivity of Eledone by extraction techniques and by electrophysiology. The differences between the findings of the two methods were attributed to the masking of the deeper tubuli of the retina by rhodopsin and its intermediates situated on the more distal villi.

In contrast to these observations on the eyes of molluscs, the spectral sensitivity curve of Calliphora maggots (Strange, 1961), determined from behaviour experiments, agrees well with the ‘Nomogram’ of the same peak wavelength (Dartnell, 1953). Calliphora maggots have simple eyes with no lens and are covered by thin, opaque, white skin. It is probable that only such simple eyes will give close agreement between the experimental and the hypothetical spectral sensitivity curves.

It may be the case that in species such as Calliphora the retina does receive an uninterrupted spectrum of light. In other animals the situation is not as clear. The retinal characteristics of Aplysia californica (Waser, 1968), for example, appear to be different from those of other simple eyes. This difference may be due to the experimental method used with Aplysia. The parameter measured was the frequency of spikes in the optic nerve in response to different wavelengths of the same energy content. A plateau of peak sensitivity from 400 to 600 m/i was found by this method, but the curve does not fit any known or hypothetical pigment (Dartnell, 1953). The plateau of Aplysia would therefore seem to be masking the true peak, or peaks, of sensitivity.

The spectral sensitivity of two other gastropod eyes, Hermissenda (Dennis, 1967) and Otala (Gilliary & Wolbarsht, 1967), found electrophysiologically by measuring spike height rather than spike frequency, show broad peaks of sensitivity around 490 mμ, which is in general agreement with the peak at 500−505 mμ of Onchidoris. This suggests that gastropods may have a single photopigment with peak efficiency at 490−510 mμ, which may also be the pigment present in cephalopods. The variations in λmax of 485−500 mμ found in cephalopods may be due to differences in the protein ‘opsin’ part of the rhodopsin molecule (Brown & Brown, 1958). A similar argument could also apply to gastropods. However, the broad peak in gastropods may be due to experimental error, in which case the true maximum could only be resolved by extraction of the photopigment.

Pelecypods, on the other hand, may prove to have two or more photopigments, since Mya (Hecht, 1920−1) and Pecten (Cronly-Dillon, 1966) show two unequal peaks of sensitivity. In comparison Pholas (Hecht, 1927) has only a single peak of spectral efficiency. A further step in the study of the spectral sensitivity of the pigments of molluscs (not including cephalopods) would be the devising of a practicable method of measuring the absorption spectrum of very small quantities of photopigment. With present methods it is estimated (H. J. A. Dartnell, personal communication) that at least 500−1000 animals with eyes of about 100 μ in diameter would have to be dissected in order to provide a sufficient quantity of pigment.

Absolute threshold

The movements of Onchidoris within the experimental tank remained significantly different from random at the 5 % level after the addition of neutral density filters of value 6·9 log units, which is equivalent to an intensity of 5·83 × 10−5 ergs/sec./cm.2 (Fig. 6), but became random at an intensity of 4·47 × 10−5 ergs/sec./cm.2 (7·0 log units). After taking into account the transmission of the dorsal skin and the diameter of the aperture of the eye, the smallest number of quanta/sec. necessary to stimulate the eyes of Onchidoris was found to lie between 81 and 106. If 10% of the incident light at 501 mμ is absorbed by the lens and cornea, a conservative estimate in comparison with the eyes of other animals (Pirenne, 1956; Ludvigh & McCarthy, 1938), then only 72−90 quanta/sec. are available to the receptors at threshold intensities. Assuming that there are five receptor cells in each Onchidoris eye (Holmes, 1968 ; Hughes, 1970), then each cell could respond to as few as 12−18 quanta/sec.

The eye of Onchidoris has a similar order of sensitivity to the eye of Xenopus (Denton & Pirenne, 1954b), Dendrocoelum (Marriott, 1958, 1960) and barnacle nauplii (Siddle, 1968). These animals have eyes which are about 20 times less sensitive than the human eye (Denton & Pirenne, 1954a; Marriott, 1960) which can respond to 5–15 quanta/sec. on the retina (Hecht, Shlaer & Pirenne, 1942; Pirenne, 1956). Waser (1968) found that Aplysia califomica gave a measurable electrical response down to an illumination of 0·01 μW./cm.2. Calculations based on his data give a threshold value of approximately 7·8 × 106 quanta/sec./eye. Jacklett (1969) estimates that there are approximately 7000 receptors in the eye of Aplysia calif omica, so that each cell could react to 11·14 × 102 quanta/sec. Assuming the threshold energy to be applicable to the λmax. of the eye, then the absolute sensitivity of the eye of Aplysia californica would be 106 lower than the eye of Onchidoris, and that of the individual cell 100 times lower.

Two other nudibranchs tested in the present series of experiments were found to have much higher effective thresholds than Onchidoris. Coryphella verrusosa, a thinskinned aeolid, became unresponsive at 4·2−6·1 × 10−1 ergs/sec./cm.2, and Goniodoris nodosa, a thin-skinned dorid, at 7·3−12·4 × io-1 ergs/sec./cm.2. These values would bring them closer to the threshold energy of Aplysia californica. However, because of the much lower threshold of Onchidoris (a thick-skinned dorid), and of the common difficulty experienced when animals became unresponsive to light of any intensity while in breeding condition or at the end of their life cycle (Holmes, 1968), it is considered that the true threshold of Coryphella and Goniodoris was not discovered.

If the threshold sensitivity of Onchidoris is representative of the nudibranchs in general then the dense skin, the sunken position and the paucity of sensory cells in their eyes (Willem, 1892; Dennis, 1967; Eakin, Westfall & Dennis, 1967; Holmes, 1968; Hughes, 1970) does not necessarily indicate diminished sensitivity. However, an opaque skin does mean that the effective sensitivity of the eye is reduced. Thus the effective threshold of the Onchidoris eye at 501 mμ is 881–1148 quanta/sec./eye. The lowering of the effective sensitivity of the nudibranch eye by the covering of opaque skin would be of little importance to animals which live intertidally or in the upper regions of the sublittoral zone, where the purpose of photoreceptors would be to detect changes in light intensity well above the effective threshold. Positive phototaxis (Holmes, 1968) would ensure that the animals seldom find themselves in areas where the diurnal light intensity is below their effective threshold.

Brown
,
P. K.
&
Brown
,
P. S.
(
1958
).
Visual pigments of the octopus and cuttlefish
.
Nature, Lond
.
182
,
1288
90
.
Charles
,
G. H.
(
1961
).
The orientation of Littorina species to polarized light
.
J. exp. Biol
.
38
,
189
202
.
Cronly-Dillon
,
J. R.
(
1966
).
Spectral sensitivity of the scallop Pecten maximus
.
Science, N.Y
.
151
,
345
6
.
Dartnell
,
H. J. A.
(
1953
).
The interpretation of spectral sensitivity curves
.
Br. Med. Bull
.
9
,
24
30
.
Defant
,
A
(
1961
).
Phytical Oceanography
. Vol.
1
,
725
pp.
Pergamon Press
.
Dennis
,
M. J.
(
1967
).
Electrophysiology of the visual system in a nudibranch molusc
.
J. Neurophyriol
.
30
,
1439
65
.
Denton
,
E. J.
(
1960
).
The design of fish and cephalopod eyes in relation to their environment
.
Symp. zool. Soc. Lond. No
.
3
,
53
5
.
Denton
,
E. J.
&
Pirennb
,
M. H.
(
1954a
).
The absolute sensitivity and functional stability of the human eye
.
J. Physiol
.
123
,
417
42
.
Denton
,
E. J.
&
Pirenne
,
M. H.
(
1954b
).
The visual sensitivity of the toad Xenoput laevit
.
J. Physiol
.
135
,
181
207
.
Eakin
,
R. M.
,
Westfall
,
J. A.
&
Dennis
,
M. J.
(
1967
).
The fine structure of the eye of nudibranch mollusc Hermittenda crattic omit
.
J. Cell Sci
.
2
,
349
58
.
Gilliary
,
H. L.
&
Wolbarsht
,
M. L.
(
1967
).
Electrical responses from the eye of a land snail Otala lac tea
.
Rev. Can. Biol
.
26
,
135
34
.
Hamdorf
,
K.
,
Schweimer
,
J.
&
Taober
,
U.
(
1968
).
The visual pigment, the absorption of the photoreceptors and the spectral sensitivity of the retina of Eledone motchata
.
Z. vergl. Physiol
.
60
,
375
90
.
Hartline
,
H. K.
(
1938
).
The discharge of impulses in the optic nerve of Pecten in response to illumination of the eye
.
J. Cell. comp. Physiol
.
II
,
465
78
.
Hecht
,
S.
(
1920-1
).
The relation between the wavelength of light and its effect on the photosensory process
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
3
,
375
90
.
Hecht
,
S.
(
1927
).
The relation of time, intensity and wavelength on the photosensory system of Pholas
.
J. Gen. Physiol
.
11
,
657
72
.
Hecht
,
S.
,
Shlaer
,
S.
&
Pirenne
,
M. H.
(
1942
).
Energy, quanta and vision
.
J. Gen. Physiol
.
25
,
819
40
.
Holmes
,
H. P. I.
(
1968
).
Structure of the eye and responses to light of certain nudibranchs
.
Ph.D. Thesis
.
University of Wales
.
Hubbard
,
R.
&
St George
,
R. C. C.
(
1958
).
The rhodopsin system of the squid
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
41
,
501
28
.
Hughes
,
H. P. I.
(
1970
).
A light and electron microscope study of some Opisthobranch eyes
.
Z. Zellfortch. in press
.
Kennedy
,
D.
&
Milkman
,
R. D.
(
1956
).
Selective light absorption by the lenses of lower vertebrates, and its influence on the spectral sensitivity
.
Biol. Bull. mar. biol. Lab., Woodt Hole
.
111
,
375
87
.
Kropf
,
A.
,
Brown
,
P. K.
&
Hubbard
,
R.
(
1959
).
Lumi-rhodopsin and meta-rhodopsin of squid and octopus
.
Nature, Lond
.
183
,
446
8
.
Jacklett
,
J. W.
(
1969
).
Electrophysiological organisation of the eye of Aplysia
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
53
,
21
42
.
Ludvigh
,
E.
&
Mccarthy
,
E. F.
(
1938
).
Absorption of visible light by refractive media of the human eye
.
Arch. Opthal. N.Y
.
20
,
37
51
.
Marriott
,
F. H. C.
(
1958
).
The absolute light sensitivity and spectral threshold curve of the aquatic flatworm Dendrocoelum lacteum
.
J. Physiol
.
143
,
369
79
.
Marriott
,
F. H. C.
(
1960
).
The sensitivity limit of photoreceptors
.
Symp. zool. Soc. Lond
. no.
3
,
67
83
.
Pirenne
,
M. H.
(
1956
).
Physiological mechanisms of vision and the quantum nature of light
.
Biol. Rev
.
31
,
194
241
.
Siddle
,
N.
(
1968
).
The absolute threshold and spectral sensitivity curve of barnacle nauplii
.
M.Sc. Thesis
.
University of Wales
.
Strange
,
P. H.
(
1961
).
The spectral sensitivity of Calliphora maggots
.
J. Exp. Biol
.
38
,
237
48
.
Waser
,
P. M.
(
1968
).
The spectral sensitivity of the eye of Aplytia californica
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol
.
27
,
339
47
.
Willem
,
V.
(
1892
).
Contributions a l’étude physiologique des organes des sens chez les molluscques. III. Observations sur la vision et les organes visuels de quelques molluscques Prosobranches et Opisthobranches
.
Arch Biol
.,
13
,
123
49
.