Successful navigation depends on an animal's ability to perceive its spatial orientation relative to visual surroundings. Heading direction in insects is represented in the central complex (CX), a navigation center in the brain, to generate steering commands. In insects that navigate relative to sky compass signals, CX neurons are tuned to celestial cues indicating the location of the sun. The desert locust CX contains a compass-like representation of two related celestial cues: the direction of unpolarized direct sunlight and the pattern of polarized light, which depends on the sun position. Whether congruent tuning to these two compass cues emerges within the CX network or is inherited from CX input neurons is unclear. To address this question, we intracellularly recorded from GABA-immunoreactive TL neurons, which are input elements to the locust CX (corresponding to R neurons in Drosophila), while applying visual stimuli simulating unpolarized sunlight and polarized light across the hemisphere above the animal. We show that TL neurons have large receptive fields for both types of stimuli. However, faithful integration of polarization angles across the dorsal hemisphere, or matched-filter ability to encode particular sun positions, was found in only two out of 22 recordings. Those two neurons also showed a good match in sun position coding through polarized and unpolarized light signaling, whereas 20 neurons showed substantial mismatch in signaling of the two compass cues. The data, therefore, suggest that considerable refinement of azimuth coding based on sky compass signals occurs at the synapses from TL neurons to postsynaptic CX compass neurons.

Spatial orientation relative to visual surroundings is a crucial ability for successful navigation. Neurons representing an animal's orientation, such as head direction cells in the rat (Taube et al., 1990a,b), have been intensely studied (Cullen and Taube, 2017). Theoretical studies have proposed recurrent network models called ring attractors to explain neuronal population dynamics of heading representation (Knierim and Zhang, 2012; Skaggs et al., 1995). Theoretical and experimental data suggested that heading-direction systems are driven by internally generated self-motion cues, but most networks also use external sensory cues, such as visual landmarks, for feedback control.

Insects also show physiological signatures of heading-direction coding to visual references, enabling the characterization of ring attractor elements in biological circuits that consist of a much smaller number of neurons than mammalian systems (Green and Maimon, 2018; Turner-Evans et al., 2020). Heading direction is represented in the central complex (CX), a navigation center of the insect brain, to generate steering commands to a navigational goal. The CX is a group of midline-spanning neuropils consisting of the protocerebral bridge (PB), the upper and the lower divisions of the central body (CBU and CBL, also termed fan-shaped body and ellipsoid body), and the paired noduli. These neuropils are subdivided into vertical slices and horizontal layers by neuronal projection patterns (Heinze and Homberg, 2008; Hulse et al., 2021; Wolff et al., 2015). In Drosophila, heading direction is represented as a localized bump of population activity in the so-called E-PG neurons of a ring attractor (Green et al., 2017; Seelig and Jayaraman, 2015). E-PG neurons are topographically arranged in the slices of the CBL and PB. The activity bump moves to neighboring slices when the fly turns clockwise or counterclockwise, and optogenetic manipulation of the bump position leads to flight orientation shifts following the bump (Kim et al., 2019). This heading representation works in darkness but more reliably when a visual cue is available (Turner-Evans et al., 2020).

In several insects, many neurons of the CX are tuned to visual stimuli simulating celestial cues (el Jundi et al., 2015; Hardcastle et al., 2021; Heinze and Homberg, 2007; Heinze and Reppert, 2011). Celestial cues are related to the location of the sun and include the direction of unpolarized direct sunlight as well as the products of sunlight scattering in the atmosphere, such as a coherent polarization pattern and chromatic gradient across the sky (Fig. 1A). Therefore, these neurons are suitable for heading-direction coding relative to the sun and considered a basis of orientation behavior dependent on a sky compass (Heinze, 2017; Honkanen et al., 2019).

In desert locusts, different celestial cues complement each other for robust head-direction coding. The locust CX contains a topographic arrangement of neurons tuned to the azimuth of bright light spots, simulating direct sunlight, across the vertical slices of the PB (Pegel et al., 2019). The neurons are also tuned to the angle of polarization (AoP) of light across the entire hemisphere above the animal (Bech et al., 2014; Zittrell et al., 2020). The AoP tunings of individual neurons are coherently arranged across the dorsal hemisphere and match the sky polarization pattern produced by a particular position of the sun (Bech et al., 2014; Zittrell et al., 2020). This polarization-based solar azimuth is topographically arranged consistent with the direct sunlight compass (Zittrell et al., 2020).

The polarization-vision pathway is largely conserved across insects (el Jundi et al., 2014; Hardcastle et al., 2021; Homberg et al., 2011). It originates from the dorsal rim area of the compound eye, where specialized, homochromatic photoreceptors detect polarized light, and runs through the optic lobe, anterior optic tubercle and bulb to finally enter the CBL via tangential neurons (TL neurons, corresponding to R neurons in Drosophila). Tuning of TL neurons to the AoP in the zenith above the animal and to the azimuth of light spots shows a 90 deg angular difference (Pegel et al., 2018) corresponding to the natural relationship between the zenithal AoP and the solar azimuth in the sky (Fig. 1A). To elucidate whether the matching AoP and direct sunlight signaling in postsynaptic CX compass neurons studied by Zittrell et al. (2020) is inherited from TL neurons, or emerges through integration of TL inputs to postsynaptic CX compass neurons, we studied the receptive field structures of TL neurons by applying light stimuli simulating polarization and direct sunlight across the sky.

Animals and preparation

Adult male and female desert locusts (Schistocerca gregaria Forsskål 1775) were reared under crowded conditions at 28°C in a 12 h light:12 h dark cycle. After removing legs and wings, animals were mounted on a metal holder with dental wax. The head capsule was opened frontally; ocelli and antennae were removed. Fat, tracheal tissues and muscles were partially removed to expose the brain. We also removed the esophagus and gut through the abdomen to reduce peristaltic movements. A twisted metal wire was placed under the brain to stabilize it. A small part of the neural sheath was removed with fine tweezers to allow brain penetration by the recording electrode. During dissection and intracellular recording, the brain was immersed in locust saline (Clements and May, 1974).

Intracellular recording

Sharp glass microelectrodes were drawn from borosilicate capillaries (Hilgenberg, Malsfeld, Germany) by a Flaming/Brown horizontal puller (P-97, Sutter Instruments, Novato, CA, USA). We filled electrode tips with 4% Neurobiotin (Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA, USA) in 1 mol l−1 KCl and shanks with 1 mol l−1 KCl. Neural signals were amplified (×10) and filtered (20 kHz low-pass) by an amplifier (SEC 05 L, NPI Electronic, Tamm, Germany). The signals were digitized at 20 kHz and stored on a PC by an A/D converter and associated software (Power1401-mkII and Spike2 version 7.06, Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK).

Visual stimulation

We used three types of light for visual stimulation: linearly polarized blue light, unpolarized green light, and unpolarized blue light. They were switchable during recording. Polarized blue light was used to test AoP sensitivity of single cells. Blue LED light (ELJ-465-627, Roithner LaserTechnik, Vienna, Austria) was passed through a diffuser and a polarizer (HN38S, Polaroid, Cambridge, MA, USA). The light covered a visual angle of 5.2 deg, and the light intensity was 8.4×1013 photons cm−2 s−1 with a peak at 461 nm. A single AoP stimulus was a full rotation of the polarizer at an angular velocity of 40 deg s−1 clockwise or counterclockwise. We started rotating the polarizer several seconds after the light was turned on to exclude phasic responses to lights on. The initial orientation of the polarizer was always 0 deg, which is parallel to the animal's anterior–posterior body axis when the light was positioned at the animal's zenith. An unpolarized green light was used to test sensitivity to direct sunlight, reported previously in TL neurons by Pegel et al. (2018, 2019). An unpolarized blue light was used for comparison. The light sources were green and blue LEDs (green: Nichia NCSE119A, Lumitronix, Hechingen, Germany; blue: OSLON SSL 80 LD CQ7P, OSRAM Opto Semiconductors, Regensburg, Germany). Both lights covered a visual angle of 1.05 deg. The green light intensity was 1.7×1014 photons cm−2 s−1 with a peak at 518 nm, and the blue one was 1.2×1015 photons cm−2 s−1 with a peak at 440 nm.

The stimulus setup was modified from that described by Bech et al. (2014) and Zittrell et al. (2020). The animal was positioned in the center of the setup with its anterior–posterior body axis oriented vertically (Fig. 1B). A stimulation device containing the three lights was mounted on a perimeter apparatus. After stimulating the animal from the zenith, we shifted the light device in left–right directions along the perimeter and tilted the whole perimeter in anterior–posterior directions (arrows in Fig. 1B). This allowed stimulation of the animal from various positions in its dorsal visual field.

Histology

We injected Neurobiotin into the recorded cell by applying a positive current of up to 1 nA for 0.5–4 min. The brains were dissected out and submerged overnight at 4°C in fixative containing 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA), 0.25% glutaraldehyde, and 0.2% saturated picric acid in 0.1 mol l−1 phosphate buffered saline (PBS, 0.15 mol l−1 NaCl in 0.1 mol l−1 sodium phosphate buffer, pH 7.4). Optionally, the fixed brains were stored at 4°C in sodium phosphate buffer until further processing. After the fixation, the brains were rinsed in PBS and then incubated in Cy3-conjugated streptavidin (Dianova, Hamburg, Germany; 1:1000 in PBS with 0.3% Triton X-100) for 3 days at 4°C. The incubated brains were rinsed in PBS with 0.3% Triton X-100 followed by PBS, dehydrated in an ascending ethanol series (30%, 50%, 70%, 90%, 95%, 2×100%; 15 min each), and cleared in a 1:1 mixture of 100% ethanol and methyl salicylate for 20 min, followed by pure methyl salicylate for 35 min. Finally, we embedded the brains between two coverslips in Permount (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA, USA).

For double labeling of the recorded cells combined with GABA immunostaining, the staining method was modified from a previous study (Takahashi et al., 2017). Neurobiotin-injected brains were submerged overnight at 4°C in 4% PFA in 0.1 mol l−1 sodium phosphate buffer (pH 7.4). Immediately after fixation, the brains were rinsed in PBS, embedded in albumin-gelatin (4.8% gelatin and 12% ovalbumin in demineralized water) and fixed overnight at 4°C in 8% formaldehyde diluted in 0.1 mol l−1 sodium phosphate buffer. The brains in the gelatin block were cut into 80–130 µm sections by a vibrating-blade microtome (VT 1000S, Leica Microsystems, Wetzlar, Germany). Brain sections were rinsed in PBS with 1% Triton X-100 (PBST) and then blocked for 1 h at room temperature in 2% normal goat serum diluted in PBST with 0.25% bovine serum albumin (PBST-BSA). Afterward, the sections were incubated for 5 days at 4°C in a mixture of anti-synapsin monoclonal antibody (RRID: AB_2315425; provided by Erich Buchner and Christian Wegener, University of Würzburg, Germany; 1:50) and anti-GABA polyclonal antibody generated in rabbit (A2052, RRID: AB_477652, Sigma, Steinheim, Germany; 1:1000) diluted in PBST-BSA. Following the incubation, the sections were rinsed in PBST-BSA and incubated for 5 days at 4°C in a mixture of goat-anti-mouse-Cy5 (Dianova, 1:300), goat-anti-rabbit-Cy2 (Dianova, 1:300), and streptavidin-Cy3 (1:1000) in PBST-BSA. After incubation, the sections were rinsed, dehydrated, cleared, and embedded in Permount.

Image acquisition and processing

We scanned preparations with a confocal laser scanning microscope (TCS SP5, Leica Microsystems). Cy3 signals were detected with a diode-pumped solid-state laser (561 nm). In GABA- and synapsin-labeled sections, Cy2 and Cy5 signals were detected with an argon laser (458 nm) and a helium-neon laser (633 nm), respectively. Spatial resolution (pixel size) in the x, y-plane was about 0.51 µm×0.51 µm for the morphology of whole neurons and approximately 0.13 µm×0.13 µm for magnified cell bodies. Step size was 1.5–3.0 µm along the z-axis. Scanned images were stacked two-dimensionally in an image-processing software (ImageJ v. 1.52a, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA; Schneider et al., 2012). Input levels of the image stacks were uniformly adjusted in photo-editing software (GNU Image Manipulation Program version 2.10.22, GIMP Development Team). We deduced innervation layers of neurons from the position of their arborizations within neuropils, identifiable through tissue autofluorescence or visualized synapsin.

Data pre-processing

Physiological data were analyzed when the recorded neuron was successfully labeled. More than one neuron was stained in some preparations, probably because of leakage of Neurobiotin into neighboring cells. We included these cases in the analyses if we identified the recorded neuron based on Neurobiotin (Cy3) signal intensity or if all stained cells belonged to the same cell type and had cell bodies in the same brain hemisphere.

For pre-processing of recorded data, action potentials were detected in Spike2 by a threshold-based feature detection script (FeatureDetect.s2s, downloaded from the CED website). Detection quality was verified by visual inspection. We performed all subsequent analyses in MATLAB v. 2021b (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) and R v. 4.1.1 (https://www.r-project.org/). The significance level for statistical tests was α=0.05.

Data plots

Data were plotted as boxplots in the following way. Boxes range from the 25th (Q1) to 75th (Q3) percentile of the data. Horizontal lines in the boxes indicate the median. Whiskers extend to the adjacent value that is the most extreme data point, which is not less than Q3−1.5×(Q3−Q1) and greater than Q3+1.5×(Q3−Q1). Numerals of x-axis labels represent sample numbers.

Spherical coordinates of the dorsal visual field (Fig. 1B,C) were transformed on a polar-coordinate grid to show the data on a flattened hemisphere from above (Fig. 1D) following Zittrell et al. (2020). The center (pole) of the grid corresponds to the zenith (elevation=90 deg). The radius from the pole (ρ) is defined as 1 – elevation/90 deg (0≤ρ≤1) and the angle (θ) equals the spherical azimuth (0≤θ<360 deg). Elevation and azimuth are indicated relative to the animal's head.

Response to AoP: sensitivity

Spike times during stimulation were transformed into the orientation of the polarizer (spike angles) based on angular velocity (40 deg s−1) and direction (clockwise or counterclockwise) of polarizer rotation. Spike angles were used to calculate spike rates per 10 deg bin (36 bins from 0 to 360 deg). For spike rate calculations, we pooled spike activities to equal numbers of clockwise and counterclockwise rotations to avoid spike angle shifts due to rotation direction.

To judge neural responses to the AoP orientation, we calculated the square of the circular-linear correlation coefficient (0≤rcl2≤1) between bin center angles and spike rates per bin using the function ‘circ_corrcl’ in the Circular Statistics Toolbox of MATLAB (Berens, 2009) (see Supplementary Materials and Methods). Bin center angles were doubled for the calculation because the AoP is axial data: 0 deg=180 deg (Batschelet, 1981). Spike activities were considered an AoP response when the P-value of rcl2<0.05. Because the P-value of rcl2 depends on the sample number used for the calculation (Berens, 2009), and we always used spike rates of 36 bins, P<0.05 is equivalent to rcl2>0.1664.

Response to AoP: tuning properties

To yield a tuning curve of AoP responses, we fitted von Mises distributions to spike angle data. The von Mises distribution is known as a circular normal distribution, commonly used for circular data analysis. In a von Mises distribution, the probability of angles θ depends on two parameters: peak position μ and concentration κ. Increasing values of κ represent increased probability of angles around μ, whereas κ=0 results in the uniform distribution.

To describe our bimodal AoP response data, we mixed two von Mises distributions as described in Fitak and Johnsen (2017) and Schnute and Groot (1992):
formula
(1)
where M(θ | μ, κ) denotes a von Mises distribution with peak μ and concentration κ. The mixed von Mises distribution represented by Eqn 1 possesses bimodal peaks of the same height (λ) and width (κ) in symmetric positions (μ and μ+π). The best-fit parameters λ, μ and κ for a tuning curve of the data were found by the maximum likelihood method; the likelihood was calculated by the function ‘circ_mle’ in the CircMLE package of R (Fitak and Johnsen, 2017). The best-fit peak position μ (0≤μ<180 deg) was termed Φmax (preferred angle) and Φmin (anti-preferred angle) was defined as 90 deg distant from Φmax. The half width at the half amplitude of the peak was used as tuning width. Tuning width was measured with increments of 1 deg to simplify the calculation.

Further, we quantified excitatory and inhibitory modulations of spike rate caused by AoP presentation based on the tuning curve and the background activity (BA). The BA level for this analysis was the averaged firing rate (spikes s−1) calculated from one or several 1 s bins before the polarized light was turned on. We defined Φmax/min activity as the difference between the spike rate at Φmax/min and the BA. These values indicate excitatory (upward arrow) or inhibitory (downward arrow) modulation from the BA. In most cases, Φmax and Φmin activities were excitatory and inhibitory, respectively. However, both Φmax and Φmin activities can be excitatory or inhibitory, depending on the tuning curve position relative to the BA. To compare modulation strengths and direction between individuals, we scaled Φmax activities by the amplitudes of the individual tuning curves (Φmax activity/amplitude). The amplitude of a tuning curve is defined as the difference of Φmax and Φmin activities. Here, we did not use the BA for scaling because some individuals had no background spiking before the stimulus.

Response to AoP: receptive fields

To visualize neural receptive fields to AoP, we plotted surface heatmaps based on AoP sensitivity (rcl2). The rcl2 values of tested positions were linearly interpolated in between on the flattened hemispheres (Fig. 1D) using the function ‘scatteredInterpolant’ in MATLAB.

Further, we determined highly AoP sensitive parts of the receptive fields in each cell for comparison with downstream CX neurons analyzed in Zittrell et al. (2020), in which AoP receptive fields were defined as the regions with 75% or more rcl2 value to the response maxima of individual recordings. To find the corresponding regions, we followed the procedures of Zittrell et al. (2020). Normalized rcl2 values of tested positions were linearly interpolated to the points distributed over the surface of the dorsal visual field, and boundary lines were drawn to enclose all points with normalized rcl2≥0.75.

A detailed description is provided by Zittrell et al. (2020). Briefly, first, we distributed 2500 points evenly over the surface of a spherical dorsal visual field hemisphere using a hemispherical Fibonacci grid proposed by Swinbank and Purser (2006). The spherical coordinates for the 2500 points were transformed to two-dimensional coordinates on a flattened hemisphere (Fig. 1D) to simplify the following calculations. Out of the 2500 points, we only used the points inside the convex hull of the positions tested in individual recordings; the convex hull was built by the Delaunay triangulation method in the same way as surface heatmaps.

Next, the rcl2 values at the tested positions were normalized to the response maxima of individual recordings and linearly interpolated over the generated points using the function ‘scatteredInterpolant’ in MATLAB. We picked up the points with normalized rcl2≥0.75, which resulted in one or more clouds of data points. To categorize those clouds into individual fields, we used an agglomerative hierarchical clustering approach. This clustering method successfully merges pairs of the closest or most similar data point sets (clusters) into single clusters. In the first step, each data point was considered its own cluster. The distances or similarities between pairs of clusters are defined by a linkage criterion. In the current study, the linkage criterion ‘minimum distance (single-linkage) method’ was applied to all recordings, defining the clusters' distance as the minimum distance between a data point in one cluster and a data point in the other cluster. The great-circle distance was adopted as a distance measure, and the original spherical coordinates of data points were used to calculate the distances here (see Supplementary Materials and Methods). The clusters were split (not merged) when their distance was greater than the visual angle covered by the polarized light of the stimulus setup (5.2 deg×π/180). Finally, we drew a boundary line to enclose all data points of each cluster to mark the higher AoP sensitivity regions corresponding to the study by Zittrell et al. (2020).

Response to AoP: best-matching polarization patterns

Many neurons of the locust CX show coherent arrangements of preferred AoP orientations across the dorsal visual field (Bech et al., 2014; Zittrell et al., 2020). These tuning arrangements act as filters that match the sky polarization pattern generated by particular solar coordinates relative to the animal (Bech et al., 2014; Zittrell et al., 2020). We defined the best-matching polarization pattern as the sky polarization pattern that would evoke the highest neuronal activity in a cell. We also defined the preferred sun encoded by AoP responses as the solar coordinates that generate the best-matching polarization pattern.

To find the preferred sun encoded by AoP responses, we used the procedure described in Zittrell et al. (2020), which was adapted from the original of Bech et al. (2014). This procedure calculates deviations between a neural response pattern (Φmax angles) and various model sky polarization patterns. The model pattern with the minimum deviation from the neural responses is considered the best-matching pattern, and the corresponding solar coordinates are the position of the preferred sun.

A detailed description of the procedures is provided in Zittrell et al. (2020). Briefly, first, we generated sky polarization patterns (angles and degrees of polarization) based on the single-scattering Rayleigh model (Strutt, 1871) (Fig. 1A and see Supplementary Materials and Methods). We prepared 32,760 model patterns from equally spaced solar positions (azimuth 360 ways×elevation 91 ways). Next, for each model pattern, we calculated the absolute angular differences (from 0 deg to 90 deg) between the Φmax angles of AoP responses (rcl2>0.1664) and the model angles of polarization. Finally, we averaged the absolute angular differences to yield the deviation of the model pattern.

Before the averaging process, the absolute angular differences were weighted in each position; weighting factors were (1) response rcl2 value, (2) model degree of polarization, and (3) the normalized sum of the great-circle distances to the nearest 22% of tested positions. The third weighting factor was introduced to counterbalance the overrepresentation of values from densely sampled areas. The value of 22% was chosen in accordance with the original procedure (Bech et al., 2014), where every data set contained AoP responses from 37 positions, and the nearest eight positions were used to calculate the weighting factor. In the current study and the study of Zittrell et al. (2020), the total number of tested positions varied due to the instability of intracellular recordings. Hence, we chose 8/37≈22% of the total number of tested positions of each data set as the number of nearest positions to calculate the weighting factor.

Response to AoP: evaluation of pattern matching results

Neural AoP responses can encode unambiguous solar coordinates when the minimum deviation between the response pattern and the best-matching polarization pattern is small enough. To evaluate the minimum deviation and, thus, the matched-filter quality of a neuron, Zittrell et al. (2020) performed a bootstrap procedure. In this procedure, the P-value of the minimum deviation of the best matching pattern is calculated as the probability that a lower value is observed in a population of the minimum deviations of randomized response patterns. When P<0.05, the neuron is considered as a reliable matched filter encoding unambiguous solar coordinates based on the sky polarization pattern.

A detailed description of the procedures is provided in Zittrell et al. (2020). Briefly, first, we generated 5000 randomized response patterns from an actual neural response pattern; a randomized pattern was generated by randomly drawing (with replacement) AoP responses from the pool of the actual neural responses (rcl2>0.1664) and distributing them on all neural response positions. Next, for each randomized response pattern, we calculated the best-matching polarization pattern and its deviation in the same way as described in the previous section, yielding a bootstrap population of the minimum deviations. Finally, the P-value of the minimum deviation for the actual neural response pattern was calculated as follows:
formula
(2)
where D is the minimum deviation of the actual data, k is the bootstrap sample index, b is the number of randomized samples (5000) and dk is the minimum deviation of the kth sample. Recordings were excluded from the analysis if there were only one or two AoP responses because the number of possible randomized sample variations was too small (≤22).

In this procedure, the P-value tends to be large when the preferred sun encoded by the actual AoP responses is near the elevation of 0 deg in anterior (azimuth 0 deg), left (90 deg), posterior (180 deg) and right (270 deg) directions from the animal (Fig. 1D). That is because all Φmax angles are approximately 0 deg or 90 deg in these cases, resulting in a small difference between the actual neural response pattern and the randomized patterns (e.g. TL2a_18 in Fig. S3). However, for comparison to the results of downstream CX neurons analyzed in Zittrell et al. (2020), we did not add any modification to the procedure.

Response to stationary light spots

To analyze neural responses caused by light spots, we counted spikes during 1 s intervals before and after the light was turned on (control and post-ON) and estimated a 68% confidence interval (CI) of the mean of post-ON spike counts. A 68% CI equivalents to mean±s.d. of normal distribution data. Spike activities during a post-ON interval were considered an inhibitory response when the CI was on the left to the mean of control spike counts, while they were considered an excitatory response when the CI was on the right to the control mean. When the CI contained the control mean, spike activities were considered no response.

We estimated a 68% CI of spike counts by the χ2 distribution method (Sahai and Khurshid, 1993), because the simple mean±s.d. method is inappropriate to estimate a CI of count data when the mean value is small. The χ2 distribution method estimates lower and upper 68% confidence limits of mean λ separately as follows:
formula
(3)
where χ2(k) (α) denotes the 100α percentile of the χ2 distribution with k degrees of freedom, n is the sample number and k is the sample sum (Sahai and Khurshid, 1993).

To visualize neural receptive fields to light spots, we plotted surface heatmaps based on spike count modulation (Δspikes s−1) in the same way as the AoP receptive fields. Spike count modulation was defined as the difference from the mean of control spike counts to the nearest 68% confidence limit of the mean of post-ON spike counts: the upper limit in inhibitory responses and the lower limit in excitatory responses. Spike count modulation of no response was defined as 0 independent of the control and post-ON spike count means.

Morphology of AoP-sensitive TL neurons

We recorded intracellularly from 59 tangential neurons of the CBL in the locust CX (Fig. 2A). All neurons were sensitive to the AoP. First, we determined their cell types. The locust CBL consists of six horizontal layers (Fig. 2B,C) (Müller et al., 1997) and six types of tangential neurons termed TL1–TL5 (Müller et al., 1997; von Hadeln et al., 2020) and TL7 (Hensgen et al., 2021) have been distinguished based on the location of their input arborizations and cell bodies. All of our recordings were from TL2 and TL3 neurons (N=38 and 21, respectively).

Each of TL2 and TL3 populations was estimated to consist of up to 40 individuals per brain hemisphere (Homberg et al., 1999). TL2 neurons are defined by their cell body along the ventro-medial face of the lateral complex and ramifications in small areas of the lateral bulb (Müller et al., 1997; von Hadeln et al., 2020). Two subtypes of TL2 neurons have been distinguished: TL2a and TL2b (Pegel et al., 2019). TL2a neurons arborize in dorsal parts of the lateral bulb and invade layer 2 of the CBL, while TL2b neurons arborize in ventral parts of the lateral bulb and invade layer 3 (orange neuron in Fig. 2A). Based on these criteria, we recorded from 34 TL2a neurons and four TL2b neurons (Fig. 2D).

TL3 neurons share a common cell body position with TL2 neurons, but their dendrites ramify in the medial bulb (Müller et al., 1997; von Hadeln et al., 2020). Three subtypes of TL3 neurons have been distinguished, termed TL3a, TL3b (von Hadeln et al., 2020) and TL3c (Hensgen et al., 2021). We recorded from four TL3a and 17 TL3b neurons but no TL3c neurons (e.g. Fig. 2D). TL3a neurons exclusively ramify in the medial bulb, while TL3b neurons have additional ramifications in the lateral bulb or along the isthmus tract (blue neuron in Fig. 2A). All TL3a and TL3b neurons recorded in this study innervated layer 5. Pegel et al. (2019) reported three AoP-sensitive TL3 neurons innervating layers 4 and 5 of the CBL. We reanalyzed those neurons and concluded that their innervation was confined to layer 5 as in all TL3 neurons studied here. We found symmetric and asymmetric branching patterns (von Hadeln et al., 2020) in both TL3a and TL3b neurons.

Based on morphological criteria, TL2 neurons likely correspond to R2 cells and TL3 neurons to R3 cells in Drosophila (Omoto et al., 2017). R2 and R3 cells are thought to be GABAergic (Hanesch et al., 1989). Homberg et al. (1999) reported GABA immunoreactivity of single TL2 and TL3 neurons but did not distinguish between the different subtypes. Therefore, we tested GABA immunoreactivity of 22 TL neurons (Fig. 2E). We found that the cell bodies of all examined TL2 and TL3 neurons were GABA-immunoreactive: TL2a [N=10 (recorded) and N=6 (staining only)], TL2b (N=1 and N=2) and TL3b (N=2 and N=1) neurons. Unfortunately, we could not perform double labeling of TL3a neurons, but our data support similar polarization-sensitive input architectures to the ring attractor networks in the locust and the fly.

Response to zenithal AoP

TL neuron data presented previously (Pegel et al., 2018, 2019) were included in our analysis hereafter: seven recordings in which the polarizer was rotated at the same angular velocity (40 deg s−1) as in our stimulus setup. First, we investigated neural sensitivity and tuning properties to zenithal AoP (Fig. 3A) tested at the beginning of the recordings (∼100 s) to exclude effects of stimulus position and fluctuations of background activity (BA) (Supplementary Materials and Methods, Fig. S1). To judge neural responses to the AoP orientation, we used the square of circular–linear correlation coefficient (rcl2) between spike rate and the polarizer orientation (Fig. 3B,C). Out of 66 neurons, five TL2a and two TL2b neurons were not sensitive to the zenithal AoP (Fig. 3D, rcl2<0.1664), although they responded to the AoP at other stimulus positions. All TL3a and TL3b neurons were sensitive to the zenithal AoP. In the TL2a population, individual rcl2 values were widely distributed, ranging from 0.00935 to 0.930 with the median 0.611, while the TL2b population showed lower AoP sensitivity (median=0.341) than the other cell types. The rcl2 values of TL3a and TL3b neurons were higher (medians=0.847 and 0.7888, respectively) than those of TL2 neurons (Fig. 3D).

We analyzed tuning properties of AoP responses by fitting mixed von Mises distributions to spike activities (Fig. 3C). Tuning width (horizontal line segment in Fig. 3C) was largest in TL2b neurons and smallest in TL3a neurons (Fig. 3E). TL2a and TL3b neurons shared similar intermediate tuning widths (Fig. 3E).

Φmax (min) activity (left and center arrows in Fig. 3C) indicates spike rate modulations from the BA (horizontal line in Fig. 3C) at Φmax (min). Positive (upward arrow) and negative (downward arrow) Φmax/min activities are excitatory and inhibitory modulations, respectively. Fig. 3F shows Φmax activities scaled by the amplitudes of individual tuning curves (Φmax activity/amplitude); here, the amplitude of a tuning curve is the difference of Φmax and Φmin activities (right arrow in Fig. 3C). Φmax activity/amplitude between 0 and 1 means excitatory modulation at Φmax and inhibitory modulation at Φmin. When the tuning curve is below the BA, inhibitory modulations occur both at Φmax and Φmin, resulting in Φmax activity/amplitude values <0. When the tuning curve is above the BA, the neuron was excited both at Φmax and Φmin, resulting in values >1 because, in this case, Φmax activity is larger than the amplitude of the tuning curve. In our data set, Φmax activity/amplitude of most individuals was between 0 and 1 independent of cell type, which means that the zenithal AoP usually induced excitation at Φmax and inhibition at Φmin in AoP-sensitive TL neurons.

Finally, Fig. 3G shows Φmax histograms for each cell type. The Φmax distribution of TL2a neurons was uniform (Rayleigh test of uniformity to doubled Φmax, Z=0.0804, P=0.784), while that of TL3b neurons had a gap around 90 deg (Z=0.487, P=0.00925). We did not test the distribution of TL2b and TL3a neurons owing to the small sample size. In summary, TL2a neurons cover the full range of zenithal AoP orientations (from 0 deg to 180 deg) with various sensitivity levels (rcl2). On the other hand, TL3 neurons have higher AoP sensitivity, but their activities do not code zenithal AoP orientation around 90 deg.

Receptive fields of AoP responses

Next, we investigated the receptive fields of the neurons to the AoP (Fig. 4). Following zenithal AoP stimulation, we stimulated from various positions within the dorsal visual hemisphere of the animal, which yielded an AoP sensitivity (rcl2) map for each neuron (examples in Fig. 4A). Recordings from 27 individual neurons were used for the analysis in which the AoP sensitivity was measured at least at five positions: at the zenith and at elevations of 30 deg in anterior (azimuth 0 deg), left (90 deg), posterior (180 deg), and right (270 deg) directions from the animal (inset in Fig. 4C). No TL3a neuron was available for this analysis.

To compare the receptive field organizations for AoP sensitivity of TL neurons with those in downstream neurons of the CX (Zittrell et al., 2020), we defined the receptive fields as those areas that had rcl2 values of at least 75% of the response maxima of each cell (inside black boundary line in Fig. 4A; see Figs S2–S4 for all 27 individuals) as done by Zittrell et al. (2020). Similar to the results of the downstream CX neurons, the receptive fields for AoP sensitivity generally varied in size, shape and position in individuals, but with some cell type-specific trends. In TL2a/2b neurons, the rcl2 values tended to be highly affected by stimulus position, resulting in relatively small susceptible parts (Fig. 4A,B, Figs S2 and S3). In contrast, the rcl2 values of most TL3b neurons were high across the dorsal visual field, resulting in larger sizes of receptive fields (Fig. 4A,B, Fig. S4).

We superimposed the boundaries of the 75% rcl2 fields of all 27 neurons (Fig. 4C) in the same way as done by Zittrell et al. (2020). Similar to the downstream CX neurons, the overlap of the receptive fields was nearly bilaterally symmetrical to the midline and maximal around the zenith (Fig. 4C), suggesting that the AoP receptive field structures are conveyed from TL populations to the downstream CX network.

Matched-filter properties of AoP sensitivity

The preferred AoP orientations of TL neurons were coherently arranged across the dorsal visual field (Fig. 5, Figs S2–S4), similar to those of CX neurons investigated in previous studies (Bech et al., 2014; Zittrell et al., 2020). In postsynaptic CX compass neurons, these tuning arrangements likely act as filters matched to the sky polarization pattern generated by particular solar coordinates. We estimated the sun positions encoded by AoP responses by calculating the best-matching sky polarization pattern with the minimum deviation from the neural response pattern (pattern matching procedure; Bech et al., 2014; Zittrell et al., 2020) and quantitatively assessed the matched filter qualities of the neurons by calculating the P-value of the minimum deviation using a bootstrap procedure (Zittrell et al., 2020).

Fig. 5A shows examples of the AoP response patterns measured in single neurons (orange and gray bars). On each response pattern, we superimposed the best-matching sky polarization pattern (black bars) calculated by the single-scattering Rayleigh model and its corresponding sun position (crossed yellow circle). We estimated best matching sun coordinates in 22 individuals out of 27 used in the receptive field analysis in the previous section; five recordings were excluded because of low number of AoP responses (≤2).

To assess the matched filter quality of individual neurons, we applied a bootstrap procedure that evaluates the minimum deviation of the best matching polarization pattern. Fig. 5B shows the distributions of the sun positions encoded by AoP responses for all analyzed neurons. Data points are color-coded by the P-value of the minimum deviation of the best-matching pattern with increments of 0.05. Only two neurons passed the criterion P<0.05 and thus are considered a reliable matched filter of the sky polarization patterns (yellow circles, arrows in Fig. 5B; TL2a_05: minimum pattern deviation=1.84 deg, P=0.0192; TL2b_01: minimum pattern deviation=7.63 deg, P=0.0296). Therefore, the proportion of reliable matched filter coding in TL neurons (2 out of 22 recordings) is considerably lower than in downstream CX neurons (17 of 23 neurons; Zittrell et al., 2020). This proportion was not affected (proportion: 1 out of 10 recordings) by including only recordings with at least nine tested stimulus positions, the requirements equal to those of Zittrell et al. (2020). The morphology of the two TL2 neurons exhibiting good matched-filter properties was not distinct from that of other TL2 cells.

Receptive fields for stationary light spots

Besides sensitivity to AoP, TL2 and TL3 neurons are sensitive to the azimuth of an unpolarized green light spot rotating around the animal's head, suited to code for solar azimuth (Pegel et al., 2018, 2019). Our data, based on stationary green and blue light spots presented at different positions across the dorsal visual field, confirm these results.

In addition, we investigated whether the preferred azimuth of the light spot corresponded to the preferred sun encoded by AoP responses in individual TL neurons. To judge neural responses to light spots, we counted spikes during 1 s intervals before and after the light was turned on (control and post-ON in Fig. 6A). When the mean of the control spike counts was outside of the 68% CI of the mean of the post-ON spike counts, post-ON spike activities were considered inhibitory or excitatory responses to a light spot presentation (Fig. 6B). Otherwise, spike activities were considered no response. To plot surface heatmaps of receptive fields, we then calculated spike count modulation values (Δspikes s−1) as the difference from the mean of control spike counts to the nearest 68% confidence limit of the mean of post-ON spike counts (horizontal arrows in Fig. 6B). In no response spike activities, Δspikes s−1 value was defined as 0 independent of the control and post-ON spike count means.

In most neurons (13 recordings), the receptive fields for stationary unpolarized green light spots comprised spatially distinct excitatory and inhibitory subfields (Fig. 6C, Figs S2–S4). Six recordings, however, showed only excitatory, five only inhibitory responses, and one TL2 neuron was completely unresponsive (Figs S2–S4). Overall, the receptive field organization for blue light spots was similar to that for green light stimuli (Figs S2–S4), but when presenting blue spots, purely inhibitory responses (6 recordings) occurred more frequently than mixed excitatory/inhibitory fields (5 recordings) and purely excitatory responses (3 recordings). TL2b neurons (2 recordings) were only excited and TL3b neurons (3 recordings) were only inhibited to blue light throughout their receptive fields.

Fig. 6C shows examples of receptive fields for unpolarized green light stimulation together with the sun positions encoded by AoP responses estimated in the previous section. These examples were chosen because their P-values of the minimum deviations from a particular sky polarization pattern (see in the previous section) were smaller than 0.2 (see Figs S2–S4 for all individuals). In the two TL2 neurons with a faithful polarization-matched filter quality (TL2a_05 and TL2b_01, P<0.05), the preferred sun encoded by AoP responses and the excitatory fields defined by green light responses were located in close proximity, suggesting integration of the two related celestial cues that match the situation in the sky. However, in three other TL2a neurons and a TL3b neuron (TL2a_07, TL2a_08, TL2a_09, and TL3b_02), the sun coordinates encoded by AoP responses were near the inhibitory fields. In the remaining cases shown in Fig. 6C, the preferred sun encoded by AoP responses was far away from the receptive fields for the green light responses. These examples illustrate striking mismatches in coding of the two celestial cues in neurons at the input stage of the CX.

Receptive fields defined by unpolarized blue light are shown in Figs S2–S4. The excitatory subfield structures differ from those of green light. As a result, the preferred sun encoded by AoP responses was far away from the excitatory fields in all recordings.

Changes in response properties

Finally, we investigated changes in response properties that occurred in some neurons during the recording. Particularly prominent changes, including a response reversal, were observed in a TL2a neuron (TL2a_09 in Fig. 7 and Table S1). Responses to zenithal visual stimuli were tested twice, at the beginning of the recording and roughly 30 min later. The neuron showed an AoP response during the first test period, while no AoP response was observed during the second testing (top traces in Fig. 7, rcl2=0.791 in test 1 and rcl2=0.133 in test 2). The lack of AoP response during the second test was likely caused by the disappearance of inhibition at Φmin (Table S1, the first Φmax activity/amplitude=0.973 and the second Φmax activity/amplitude=1.45). Similarly, the inhibitory response to unpolarized blue light spots changed to an excitatory response (bottom in Fig. 7, Δspikes s−1=−4.90 in test 1 and Δspikes s−1=2.29 in test 2). No response was observed during both test periods to unpolarized green light spot presentation (middle in Fig. 7).

In the recording of another TL2a neuron (TL2a_31), we tested zenithal AoP responses seven times (results of first, fourth and last testing in Table S1). AoP responses were stable during the first three test periods (∼10 min) but were not present during tests 4 and 5. AoP response returned in tests 6 and 7, roughly 5 min after the no response activities. Similarly to the TL2a_09 neuron, inhibition at Φmin angle was not observed during tests 4 and 5 (Φmax activity/amplitude=3.53 and 2.28). Responses to stationary light spots were not tested in this neuron.

Zenithal responses were, likewise, tested twice with a time interval >10 min in six other TL2a neurons and two TL3b neurons. AoP response properties of one TL2a neuron (TL2a_38) and the two TL3b neurons were stable over time, while those of five TL2a neurons differed between the first and the second test of stimuli (summarized in Table S1). Especially in the second test of a TL2a neuron (TL2a_20), we observed no AoP response due to the disappearance of inhibition at Φmin, similarly to the examples described above. Some unknown factors may have modulated the balance of excitation and inhibition in these neurons during the recording.

In the insect brain, TL/R neurons constitute the input to a ring attractor network in the CX, resulting in heading direction coding and steering commands transmitted to thoracic motor centers (Green et al., 2019; Heinze and Homberg, 2007, 2009; Nguyen et al., 2021; Omoto et al., 2017; Rayshubskiy et al., 2020 preprint; Seelig and Jayaraman, 2013, 2015; Shiozaki and Kazama, 2017; Sun et al., 2017; Vitzthum et al., 2002). We have characterized receptive field properties of two subtypes of these neurons, TL2 and TL3, in the locust by using visual stimuli simulating celestial compass cues across the sky. These neurons have large receptive fields for skylight polarization. However, unlike postsynaptic compass neurons of the CX (Zittrell et al., 2020), only a minority of the recorded neurons showed coding for polarization angles that match sky polarization patterns for particular sun positions. TL2 and TL3 neurons, in addition, have complex, spatially partitioned excitatory and inhibitory subfields for small light spots. In most cases, these subfields were not located in line with the polarization tuning, revealing substantial mismatch in compass coding through both sky compass cues.

Input neurons to the CX network

TL/R neurons convey multiple cues to different layers of the CBL. These cues help to establish the animal's spatial orientation, including sky compass signals, object and visual panorama information, and wind direction (el Jundi et al., 2015; Hardcastle et al., 2021; Heinze and Reppert, 2011; Kim et al., 2019; Okubo et al., 2020; Pegel et al., 2018). A connectome analysis of the Drosophila CX suggests hierarchical competition between R neuron subtypes, in which different cues influence heading direction coding to various degrees (Hulse et al., 2021).

Our recordings were confined to TL2a, TL2b and TL3 neurons innervating layers 2, 3 and 5 of the locust CBL, respectively (Fig. 2). We consider these cell types and CBL layers as the main polarization inputs to the locust CX. However, our sampling may be unintentionally biased toward specific cell types owing to limitations of intracellular recordings, such as a preference for larger diameter neurons. Previous studies showed that TL1 and TL4 neurons are, likewise, sensitive to celestial cues, although their responses are much less pronounced than those of TL2 and TL3 neurons (Pegel et al., 2018; Vitzthum et al., 2002). They innervate layer 1 (TL4) or all layers of the CBL (TL1) and, therefore, complement the main polarization inputs by TL2 and TL3. Based on morphological criteria, TL2 neurons likely correspond to R2 cells and TL3 neurons to R3 cells in Drosophila (Omoto et al., 2017). Like R2 and R3 cells, all TL2 and TL3 neurons appear to be GABAergic, further supporting similar polarization-sensitive input architectures to the ring attractor networks in both species. However, in contrast to TL3 neurons, R3 cells in the fly are not sensitive to the orientation of polarized light and thus might have lost sensitivity to the polarization pattern in the sky (Hardcastle et al., 2021).

Cell type-specific responses to sky compass signals

All TL subtypes were responsive to light stimuli simulating polarization and direct sunlight across the sky but differed in physiological properties (Figs 35, Figs S1–S4). Similar cell type-specific trends were reported previously (Bockhorst and Homberg, 2015; Heinze et al., 2009; Pegel et al., 2018, 2019; Vitzthum et al., 2002) but were not systematically analyzed. The different inputs to the different CBL layers may allow for dynamic head direction coding depending on sky conditions. The AoP sensitivity of TL2 neurons was highly dependent on stimulus position, which corresponds to the highly varying degree of polarization in the sky and should result in relatively good performance in matching sky polarization patterns (Fig. 5). Therefore, signals from TL2 neurons might be particularly useful under clear sky conditions. In contrast, TL3 neurons do not cover the full range of AoP orientations at the zenith (Fig. 3G), which is disadvantageous to matching sky polarization patterns. However, most TL3 neurons showed uniform high AoP sensitivity across large parts of the dorsal visual field, and thus, their signals may be robust even under cloudy or hazy sky conditions.

Comparison to the postsynaptic network

The large size and position of receptive fields for AoP sensitivity of TL neurons (Fig. 4, Figs S2–S4) were similar to those of downstream neurons studied in Zittrell et al. (2020). Heinze et al. (2009) reported that TL2 neurons have medium-sized, ipsilaterally-biased receptive fields relative to other CX neurons, but this conclusion is based on only a few tested stimulus positions. Vitzthum et al. (2002) and Heinze et al. (2009) showed that AoP responses in TL3 neurons are mediated by the ipsilateral eye only (monocular input) but did not distinguish between TL3a and TL3b subtypes. We reanalyzed the morphology of those neurons and found that at least two of their recordings were from TL3b neurons. As shown here, most TL3b neurons were equally sensitive to AoP from ipsi- and contralateral directions, suggesting that their monocular input source did not limit their receptive field sizes within the range tested.

The preferred AoP of TL neurons changed gradually within the 120 deg range around the zenith (Fig. 5, Figs S2–S4), again similarly to downstream neurons of the CX (Zittrell et al., 2020). Pattern matching between the AoP responses and sky polarization model yielded best matches to particular sun positions. However, judged by P-values obtained from bootstrapping procedure, the quality of the best match was good in only 10% of the cells compared to 74% in downstream neurons (Zittrell et al., 2020). Therefore, the matched filter properties in postsynaptic columnar neurons, such as CL1 or CPU types of the CX, are likely considerably refined by convergence and integration of synaptic input from appropriate TL neurons. In fact, E-PG neurons in Drosophila (equivalents to CL1 neurons) receive synaptic input from nearly all visually tuned R neurons (Hulse et al., 2021).

The two TL2 neurons with faithful polarization-matched filter quality also showed a good match in sun position coding through polarized and unpolarized light signaling. Although we did not find a distinct morphological difference between those two cells and the other TL2 neurons, there may be further subdivisions of TL2a and TL2b cell types based on physiological properties. Alternatively, response property changes observed in some recordings (Fig. 7 and Table S1) may contribute to differences in compass coding within the same cell type.

Responsiveness to unpolarized light spots

The receptive fields for stationary unpolarized light spots comprised spatially distinct excitatory and inhibitory subfields in most neurons (Fig. 6, Figs S2–S4). This receptive field organization for visual stimuli is similar to that of R2 and R4d ring neurons in Drosophila (Seelig and Jayaraman, 2013). In the fly, inhibitory subfields are usually in close proximity to an excitatory area and partly surround an excitatory center, suggesting contrast enhancement for object detection similar to mechanisms in the mammalian visual cortex (Bonin et al., 2011). In the locust, however, both subfields were often spatially far apart and suggest spatial excitatory–inhibitory opponency across the sky, likely used to evaluate brightness contrast (Pegel et al., 2018; Pegel et al., 2019).

Some TL neurons possessed only excitatory or inhibitory fields for stationary light spots, and one TL2 neuron was completely unresponsive. In contrast, Pegel et al. (2018) found pronounced spatial opponency responses to rotating light spots in all TL2 neurons (7 recordings) compared to weaker responses in two TL3 neurons. A primary reason for this discrepancy may be the coarse grid of tested stimulus positions in our study. In addition, we used unpolarized light spots of 1.05 deg visual angle, which is closer to the apparent size of the sun (about 0.5 deg) but smaller than 16.3 deg light spots used by Pegel et al. (2018).

We found some differences between receptive field structures for unpolarized green and blue light. This difference may be critical for compass integration because the excitatory fields for blue light, in contrast to those for green light, were often far from the sun position estimated from AoP responses (Figs S2–S4). The response to green light originates from the main retina, while unpolarized blue light is detected by both the main retina and the dorsal rim area. In the main retina most photoreceptors co-express two types of opsins, a long wavelength (green)-absorbing type and a blue-absorbing type, while in the dorsal rim area, all photoreceptors express only a blue-absorbing opsin (Schmeling et al., 2014). It remains an open question how this receptive field difference affects solar azimuth detection.

Changes in response properties

We observed response property changes in seven recordings out of 10 in which the response to zenithal stimuli was tested repeatedly over the course of the recording (Fig. 7 and Table S1). In three individuals, the AoP responses during the initial test were comprised of excitation at Φmax and inhibition at Φmin, but inhibition at Φmin was no longer detected during the following test. In one TL neuron, even a reverse response to unpolarized light spots was found. The balance of excitation and inhibition may have been modulated in these neurons by changes in the internal state of the animal, suggesting state-dependent processing of visual information in these neurons. Similar activity changes were found in Drosophila R neurons as epochs of elevated calcium activity visualized in glomeruli of the bulb, which were restricted to neurons in a specific glomerulus but not correlated with the activities of the upstream neuron in the same glomerulus (Sun et al., 2017). However, fluctuation in calcium activity was not reported when the activities to AoP stimuli were recorded as ensemble responses of R neuron populations (Hardcastle et al., 2021). Therefore, activity fluctuations may affect the dominance of TL/R neurons relative to others in the same layer and thereby control the output of the CBL/ellipsoid body to select visual features in a specific location.

The animal's behavioral state (rest, feeding, flying, etc.) may strongly affect the physiological properties of neurons, especially at higher level processing areas such as the CX. In this study and the work by Zittrell et al. (2020), all intracellular recordings were made from restrained, immobile animals. In monarch butterflies, extracellular recordings from neurons in tethered, flying animals demonstrated flight-induced changes in angular sensitivity of sun-compass neurons of the CX, likely induced by octopamine (Beetz et al., 2022). Therefore, in the locust, the neural activities of TL inputs and downstream cells may, likewise, change as the animal starts flying, and thus, the matched filter qualities of these cell types may improve or change in other ways to be explored.

Visual features of the sky are reliable compass cues owing to their persistent presence during navigation. Parallel channels for celestial cues as inputs to the CX are likely combined and refined by the compass network in the CX to yield a robust heading signal based on a combination of sky compass cues that eventually leads to accurate spatial orientation.

We thank Dr Uta Pegel for providing data for reanalysis, Drs Christian Wegener and Erich Buchner for supplying anti-synapsin antibodies and Martina Kern for maintaining locust cultures.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: N.T., U.H.; Methodology: N.T., F.Z.; Software: N.T., F.Z.; Validation: F.Z., U.H.; Investigation: N.T., R.H.; Resources: F.Z.; Data curation: N.T., U.H.; Writing - original draft: N.T.; Writing - review & editing: N.T., F.Z., R.H., U.H.; Visualization: N.T., R.H.; Supervision: U.H.; Project administration: U.H.; Funding acquisition: N.T., U.H.

Funding

This work was supported by JSPS Overseas Challenge Program for Young Researchers and JSPS Overseas Research Fellowships from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (201780130 and 201960238 to N.T.) and the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (HO 950/28-1 to U.H.). Open access funding provided by Philipps University of Marburg. Deposited in PMC for immediate release.

Batschelet
,
E.
(
1981
).
Circular Statistics in Biology
.
London, NY
,
USA
:
Academic Press
.
Bech
,
M.
,
Homberg
,
U.
and
Pfeiffer
,
K.
(
2014
).
Receptive fields of locust brain neurons are matched to polarization patterns of the sky
.
Curr. Biol.
24
,
2124
-
2129
.
Beetz
,
M. J.
,
Kraus
,
C.
,
Franzke
,
M.
,
Dreyer
,
D.
,
Strube-Bloss
,
M. F.
,
Rössler
,
W.
,
Warrant
,
E. J.
,
Merlin
,
C.
and
el Jundi
,
B.
(
2022
).
Flight-induced compass representation in the monarch butterfly heading network
.
Curr. Biol.
32
,
1
-
12
.
Berens
,
P.
(
2009
).
CircStat: a MATLAB toolbox for circular statistics
.
J. Stat. Softw.
31
,
1
-
21
.
Bockhorst
,
T.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2015
).
Amplitude and dynamics of polarization-plane signaling in the central complex of the locust brain
.
J. Neurophysiol.
113
,
3291
-
3311
.
Bonin
,
V.
,
Histed
,
M. H.
,
Yurgenson
,
S.
and
Reid
,
R. C.
(
2011
).
Local diversity and fine-scale organization of receptive fields in mouse visual cortex
.
J. Neurosci.
31
,
18506
-
18521
.
Clements
,
A. N.
and
May
,
T. E.
(
1974
).
Studies on locust neuromuscular physiology in relation to glutamic acid
.
J. Exp. Biol.
60
,
673
-
705
.
Cullen
,
K. E.
and
Taube
,
J. S.
(
2017
).
Our sense of direction: progress, controversies and challenges
.
Nat. Neurosci.
20
,
1465
-
1473
.
el Jundi
,
B.
,
Pfeiffer
,
K.
,
Heinze
,
S.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2014
).
Integration of polarization and chromatic cues in the insect sky compass
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
200
,
575
-
589
.
el Jundi
,
B.
,
Warrant
,
E. J.
,
Byrne
,
M. J.
,
Khaldy
,
L.
,
Baird
,
E.
,
Smolka
,
J.
and
Dacke
,
M.
(
2015
).
Neural coding underlying the cue preference for celestial orientation
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
112
,
11395
-
11400
.
Fitak
,
R. R.
and
Johnsen
,
S.
(
2017
).
Bringing the analysis of animal orientation data full circle: model-based approaches with maximum likelihood
.
J. Exp. Biol.
220
,
3878
-
3882
.
Green
,
J.
and
Maimon
,
G.
(
2018
).
Building a heading signal from anatomically defined neuron types in the Drosophila central complex
.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.
52
,
156
-
164
.
Green
,
J.
,
Adachi
,
A.
,
Shah
,
K. K.
,
Hirokawa
,
J. D.
,
Magani
,
P. S.
and
Maimon
,
G.
(
2017
).
A neural circuit architecture for angular integration in Drosophila
.
Nature
546
,
101
-
106
.
Green
,
J.
,
Vijayan
,
V.
,
Mussells Pires
,
P.
,
Adachi
,
A.
and
Maimon
,
G.
(
2019
).
A neural heading estimate is compared with an internal goal to guide oriented navigation
.
Nat. Neurosci.
22
,
1460
-
1468
.
Hanesch
,
U.
,
Fischbach
,
K. F.
and
Heisenberg
,
M.
(
1989
).
Neuronal architecture of the central complex in Drosophila melanogaster
.
Cell Tissue Res.
257
,
343
-
366
.
Hardcastle
,
B. J.
,
Omoto
,
J. J.
,
Kandimalla
,
P.
,
Nguyen
,
B.-C. M.
,
Keleş
,
M. F.
,
Boyd
,
N. K.
,
Hartenstein
,
V.
and
Frye
,
M. A.
(
2021
).
A visual pathway for skylight polarization processing in Drosophila
.
eLife
10
,
e63225
.
Heinze
,
S.
(
2017
).
Unraveling the neural basis of insect navigation
.
Curr. Opin. Insect Sci.
24
,
58
-
67
.
Heinze
,
S.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2007
).
Maplike representation of celestial E-vector orientations in the brain of an insect
.
Science
315
,
995
-
997
.
Heinze
,
S.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2008
).
Neuroarchitecture of the central complex of the desert locust: intrinsic and columnar neurons
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
511
,
454
-
478
.
Heinze
,
S.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2009
).
Linking the input to the output: new sets of neurons complement the polarization vision network in the locust central complex
.
J. Neurosci.
29
,
4911
-
4921
.
Heinze
,
S.
and
Reppert
,
S. M.
(
2011
).
Sun compass integration of skylight cues in migratory monarch butterflies
.
Neuron
69
,
345
-
358
.
Heinze
,
S.
,
Gotthardt
,
S.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2009
).
Transformation of polarized light information in the central complex of the locust
.
J. Neurosci.
29
,
11783
-
11793
.
Hensgen
,
R.
,
Göthe
,
J.
,
Jahn
,
S.
,
Hümmert
,
S.
,
Schneider
,
K. L.
,
Takahashi
,
N.
,
Pegel
,
U.
,
Gotthardt
,
S.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2021
).
Organization and neural connections of the lateral complex in the brain of the desert locust
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
529
,
3533
-
3560
.
Homberg
,
U.
,
Vitzthum
,
H.
,
Müller
,
M.
and
Binkle
,
U.
(
1999
).
Immunocytochemistry of GABA in the central complex of the locust Schistocerca gregaria: identification of immunoreactive neurons and colocalization with neuropeptides
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
409
,
495
-
507
.
Homberg
,
U.
,
Heinze
,
S.
,
Pfeiffer
,
K.
,
Kinoshita
,
M.
and
el Jundi
,
B.
(
2011
).
Central neural coding of sky polarization in insects
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B
366
,
680
-
687
.
Honkanen
,
A.
,
Adden
,
A.
,
da Silva Freitas
,
J.
and
Heinze
,
S.
(
2019
).
The insect central complex and the neural basis of navigational strategies
.
J. Exp. Biol.
222
,
jeb188854
.
Hulse
,
B. K.
,
Haberkern
,
H.
,
Franconville
,
R.
,
Turner-Evans
,
D. B.
,
Takemura
,
S.
,
Wolff
,
T.
,
Noorman
,
M.
,
Dreher
,
M.
,
Dan
,
C.
,
Parekh
,
R.
et al. 
(
2021
).
A connectome of the Drosophila central complex reveals network motifs suitable for flexible navigation and context-dependent action selection
.
eLife
10
,
e66039
.
Kim
,
S. S.
,
Hermundstad
,
A. M.
,
Romani
,
S.
,
Abbott
,
L. F.
,
Jayaraman
,
V.
(
2019
).
Generation of stable heading representations in diverse visual scenes
.
Nature
576
,
126
-
131
.
Knierim
,
J. J.
and
Zhang
,
K.
(
2012
).
Attractor dynamics of spatially correlated neural activity in the limbic system
.
Annu. Rev. Neurosci.
35
,
267
-
285
.
Müller
,
M.
,
Homberg
,
U.
and
Kühn
,
A.
(
1997
).
Neuroarchitecture of the lower division of the central body in the brain of the locust (Schistocerca gregaria)
.
Cell Tissue Res.
288
,
159
-
176
.
Nguyen
,
T. A. T.
,
Beetz
,
M. J.
,
Merlin
,
C.
and
el Jundi
,
B.
(
2021
).
Sun compass neurons are tuned to migratory orientation in monarch butterflies
.
Proc. R. Soc. B
288
,
20202988
.
Okubo
,
T. S.
,
Patella
,
P.
,
D'Alessandro
,
I.
and
Wilson
,
R. I.
(
2020
).
A neural network for wind-guided compass navigation
.
Neuron
107
,
924
-
940
.
Omoto
,
J. J.
,
Keleş
,
M. F.
,
Nguyen
,
B. C. M.
,
Bolanos
,
C.
,
Lovick
,
J. K.
,
Frye
,
M. A.
and
Hartenstein
,
V.
(
2017
).
Visual input to the Drosophila central complex by developmentally and functionally distinct neuronal populations
.
Curr. Biol.
27
,
1098
-
1110
.
Pegel
,
U.
,
Pfeiffer
,
K.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2018
).
Integration of celestial compass cues in the central complex of the locust brain
.
J. Exp. Biol.
221
,
jeb171207
.
Pegel
,
U.
,
Pfeiffer
,
K.
,
Zittrell
,
F.
,
Scholtyssek
,
C.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2019
).
Two compasses in the central complex of the locust brain
.
J. Neurosci.
39
,
3070
-
3080
.
Rayshubskiy
,
A.
,
Holtz
,
S. L.
,
D'Alessandro
,
I.
,
Li
,
A. A.
,
Vanderbeck
,
Q. X.
,
Haber
,
I. S.
,
Gibb
,
P. W.
and
Wilson
,
R. I.
(
2020
).
Neural circuit mechanisms for steering control in walking Drosophila
.
bioRxiv
Sahai
,
H.
and
Khurshid
,
A.
(
1993
).
Confidence intervals for the mean of a Poisson distribution: a review
.
Biometrical J.
35
,
857
-
867
.
Schmeling
,
F.
,
Wakakuwa
,
M.
,
Tegtmeier
,
J.
,
Kinoshita
,
M.
,
Bockhorst
,
T.
,
Arikawa
,
K.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2014
).
Opsin expression, physiological characterization and identification of photoreceptor cells in the dorsal rim area and main retina of the desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria
.
J. Exp. Biol.
217
,
3557
-
3568
.
Schneider
,
C. A.
,
Rasband
,
W. S.
and
Eliceiri
,
K. W.
(
2012
).
NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis
.
Nat. Methods
9
,
671
-
675
.
Schnute
,
J. T.
and
Groot
,
K.
(
1992
).
Statistical analysis of animal orientation data
.
Anim. Behav.
43
,
15
-
33
.
Seelig
,
J. D.
and
Jayaraman
,
V.
(
2013
).
Feature detection and orientation tuning in the Drosophila central complex
.
Nature
503
,
262
-
266
.
Seelig
,
J. D.
and
Jayaraman
,
V.
(
2015
).
Neural dynamics for landmark orientation and angular path integration
.
Nature
521
,
186
-
191
.
Shiozaki
,
H. M.
and
Kazama
,
H.
(
2017
).
Parallel encoding of recent visual experience and self-motion during navigation in Drosophila
.
Nat. Neurosci.
20
,
1395
-
1403
.
Skaggs
,
W. E.
,
Knierim
,
J. J.
,
Kudrimoti
,
H. S.
and
McNaughton
,
B. L.
(
1995
).
A model of the neural basis of the rat's sense of direction
.
Adv. Neural Inf. Process. Syst.
7
,
173
-
180
.
Strutt
,
H. J. W.
(
1871
).
XV. On the light from the sky, its polarization and colour
.
London, Edinburgh, Dublin Philos. Mag. J. Sci.
41
,
107
-
120
.
Sun
,
Y.
,
Nern
,
A.
,
Franconville
,
R.
,
Dana
,
H.
,
Schreiter
,
E. R.
,
Looger
,
L. L.
,
Svoboda
,
K.
,
Kim
,
D. S.
,
Hermundstad
,
A. M.
and
Jayaraman
,
V.
(
2017
).
Neural signatures of dynamic stimulus selection in Drosophila
.
Nat. Neurosci.
20
,
1104
-
1113
.
Swinbank
,
R.
and
Purser
,
R. J.
(
2006
).
Fibonacci grids: a novel approach to global modelling
.
Q. J. R. Meteorol. Soc.
132
,
1769
-
1793
.
Takahashi
,
N.
,
Katoh
,
K.
,
Watanabe
,
H.
,
Nakayama
,
Y.
,
Iwasaki
,
M.
,
Mizunami
,
M.
and
Nishino
,
H.
(
2017
).
Complete identification of four giant interneurons supplying mushroom body calyces in the cockroach Periplaneta americana
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
525
,
204
-
230
.
Taube
,
J. S.
,
Muller
,
R. U.
and
Ranck
,
J. B.
Jr
(
1990a
).
Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. I. Description and quantitative analysis
.
J. Neurosci.
10
,
420
-
435
.
Taube
,
J. S.
,
Muller
,
R. U.
and
Ranck
,
J. B.
Jr
(
1990b
).
Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations
.
J. Neurosci.
10
,
436
-
447
.
Turner-Evans
,
D. B.
,
Jensen
,
K. T.
,
Ali
,
S.
,
Paterson
,
T.
,
Sheridan
,
A.
,
Ray
,
R. P.
,
Wolff
,
T.
,
Lauritzen
,
J. S.
,
Rubin
,
G. M.
,
Bock
,
D. D.
et al. 
(
2020
).
The neuroanatomical ultrastructure and function of a biological ring attractor
.
Neuron
108
,
145
-
163
.
Vitzthum
,
H.
,
Müller
,
M.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2002
).
Neurons of the central complex of the locust Schistocerca gregaria are sensitive to polarized light
.
J. Neurosci.
22
,
1114
-
1125
.
von Hadeln
,
J.
,
Hensgen
,
R.
,
Bockhorst
,
T.
,
Rosner
,
R.
,
Heidasch
,
R.
,
Pegel
,
U.
,
Quintero Pérez
,
M.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2020
).
Neuroarchitecture of the central complex of the desert locust: tangential neurons
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
528
,
906
-
934
.
Wolff
,
T.
,
Iyer
,
N. A.
and
Rubin
,
G. M.
(
2015
).
Neuroarchitecture and neuroanatomy of the Drosophila central complex: A GAL4-based dissection of protocerebral bridge neurons and circuits
.
J. Comp. Neurol.
523
,
997
-
1037
.
Zittrell
,
F.
,
Pfeiffer
,
K.
and
Homberg
,
U.
(
2020
).
Matched-filter coding of sky polarization results in an internal sun compass in the brain of the desert locust
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
117
,
25810
-
25817
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed.

Supplementary information