Fast escape responses to a predator threat are fundamental to the survival of mobile marine organisms. However, elasmobranchs are often underrepresented in such studies. Here, we measured the escape latency (time interval between the stimulus and first visible reaction) of mechanically induced escape responses in the Pacific spiny dogfish, Squalus suckleyi, and in two teleosts from the same region, the great sculpin, Myoxocephalus polyacanthocephalus, and the pile perch, Rhacochilus vacca. We found that the dogfish had a longer minimum latency (66.7 ms) compared with that for the great sculpin (20.8 ms) and pile perch (16.7 ms). Furthermore, the dogfish had a longer latency than that of 48 different teleosts identified from 35 different studies. We suggest such long latencies in dogfish may be due to the absence of Mauthner cells, the giant neurons that control fast escape responses in fishes.

The ability of fishes to perform escape responses plays a vital role in avoiding predation and has been investigated to a great extent (Eaton et al., 1977; Eaton and Emberley, 1991; Hale, 2000; Domenici et al., 2004; Walker et al., 2005; Fuiman et al., 2006; Domenici, 2010; Marras et al., 2011; Ramasamy et al., 2015; McCormick et al., 2018). Many behavioral and kinematic variables have been studied, such as turning speed and angle, acceleration, the trajectory of escape, and escape latency (Webb, 1982; Eaton and Emberley, 1991; Domenici and Blake, 1997; Domenici, 2001; Walker et al., 2005; Fuiman et al., 2006; Domenici, 2010; McCormick et al., 2018). These escape variables have an ecological relevance in predator avoidance and hence survival. Escape latency, defined as the time interval between the onset of the threatening stimuli and the first visible reaction of the fish (Domenici and Hale, 2019), has been shown to predict survival with high accuracy (Katzir and Camhi, 1993; McCormick et al., 2018). A recent study by McCormick et al. (2018) investigated 13 performance variables of the white-tailed damselfish, Pomacentrus chrysurus, and found that latency was the best predictor of survival.

The initiation of the escape response in teleost fishes is typically controlled by the Mauthner cells (Eaton et al., 2001), which are a pair of large reticulospinal interneurons located in the hindbrain that receive sensory inputs from visual, auditory and mechanosensory cells (Eaton et al., 1977; Korn and Faber, 2005). Action potentials from Mauthner cells are very short (∼1 ms) (Hale, 2000; Eaton et al., 2001) and lead to contralateral muscle contractions that result in a body bend away from the stimulus, which takes the general form of a C, i.e. a C-start escape response (Yasargil and Diamond, 1968; Zottoli, 1977; Hale, 2002). The C-start is typically followed by a return flip of the tail, accelerating the fish forward and away from the predator (Domenici and Blake, 1997). Although escape responses can be generated with or without Mauthner cell activity (Liu and Fetcho, 1999; Kohashi and Oda, 2008; Bhattacharyya et al., 2017), short-latency fast escape responses are typically initiated by Mauthner cells while non-Mauthner cell escape responses have longer latencies (Hale, 2000; Zottoli and Faber, 2000; Greenwood et al., 2010; Bhattacharyya et al., 2017; Hecker et al., 2020). Furthermore, ablation (Eaton et al., 1982; DiDomenico et al., 1988; Liu and Fetcho, 1999; Zottoli et al., 1999; Issa et al., 2011) or evolutionary loss of Mauthner cells (Greenwood et al., 2010) is known to result in longer latencies. In addition, recent work based on cell ablation showed that Mauthner axons are fundamental for rapid escapes and that the absence of Mauthner cells increases the vulnerability to natural predators (Hecker et al., 2020).

Latency varies amongst fishes and is affected by temperature, distance to and strength of the stimuli, and foraging and schooling behavior (Webb, 1978a; Eaton and Hackett, 1984; Batty and Blaxter, 1992; Domenici and Batty, 1997; Domenici, 2010; Bohórquez-Herrera et al., 2013; Ramasamy et al., 2015). Latency tends to decrease with increasing temperature, increasing stimuli strength and decreasing distance to stimuli (Webb, 1978b; Domenici and Batty, 1997; Preuss and Faber, 2003). Furthermore, mechanical stimuli typically result in shorter latencies than visual stimuli (Eaton and Hackett, 1984; Batty, 1989). For teleosts, mean latency values generally lie within a range of 10 to 40 ms and minimum latencies within 5 to 20 ms (Eaton and Hackett, 1984; Domenici, 2010). Arguably, the minimum latency is a better representation of the physiological limitations of a fish's ability to react fast to a predator attack, i.e. the maximum performance allowed by the neural command system. The effect of the presence or absence of Mauthner cells is therefore best described by taking into account minimum latency.

While Mauthner cells have been described in many teleosts (Eaton et al., 1977; Zottoli, 1977; Eaton and Emberley, 1991; Eaton et al., 2001), these cells are absent or highly reduced in elasmobranchs, and may represent the vestige of the apparatus in an ancestral group (Bone, 1977). In particular, Mauthner cells have not been found in adult elasmobranch species, including Mustelus vulgaris, Scyllium stellare, Scyllium canicola, Raja punctate, Torpedo ocellata (Stefanelli, 1980), Scyliorhinus canicula and Squalus achiantias (Bone, 1977). Escape responses are particularly understudied in elasmobranchs, probably because of their large size and difficulty in handling under experimental settings (Seamone et al., 2014). However, the majority of elasmobranchs are mesopredators, such as the Pacific spiny dogfish, Squalus suckleyi (Girard 1855), which are vulnerable to predation from larger sharks and marine mammals (Vaughn et al., 2007; Ford et al., 2011). Some studies have observed escape responses in spiny dogfish when startled by thrusting of a pole or by an approaching predator model (Domenici et al., 2004; Seamone et al., 2014). However, these approaches were unable to accurately determine the time between the onset of the stimuli and the reaction of the sharks (i.e. the latency). While Mauthner cells have been related to teleost escape performance and latency in particular (DiDomenico et al., 1988; Kohashi and Oda, 2008; Greenwood et al., 2010), it is unknown how the timing of the escape response of adult elasmobranchs compares with that of teleosts, given that adult elasmobranchs lack Mauthner cells (Bone, 1977; Stefanelli, 1980). To our knowledge, no studies have measured the escape latency of this species or any other elasmobranchs.

This study aimed to (1) measure the escape latency of a model elasmobranch, the Pacific spiny dogfish, S. suckleyi, when startled by a mechanical stimulus; (2) compare the results with the latencies of four teleost species from the same geographical area, the great sculpin, Myoxocephalus polyacanthocephalus (Pallas 1814), the pile perch, Rhacochilus vacca (Girard 1855) (both measured in this study), the shiner perch, Cymatogaster aggregata, and the silver-spotted sculpin, Blepsias cirrhosus (data for both from previous studies), all of which were assumed to possess Mauthner cells (Stefanelli, 1980); and (3) compare the findings with a literature search of published escape latencies of 45 other teleosts from a range of environmental conditions. We hypothesized that the latency of the Pacific spiny dogfish is considerably longer than that of teleosts, because of the absence of Mauthner cells.

Animals

Pacific spiny dogfish, S. suckleyi (n=11; total length 76.25±6.05 cm, mean±s.d.; Table 1), were caught off Pier H in Friday Harbor, San Juan Island, WA, USA, in July 2019 with hook and line. The sharks were then transported in an aerated cooler (106×48×50 cm) and kept in three separate circular flow-through seawater tanks (224×100 cm, 12.0–14.4°C, 12 h:12 h light:dark photoperiod) at Friday Harbor Laboratories, University of Washington. The sharks were fed to satiation once per day, with locally caught fish from beach seining.

Two teleost species, the great sculpin, M. polyacanthocephalus (n=7; total length 16.30±1.30 cm, mass 44.90±8.90 g, means±s.d.), and the pile perch, R. vacca (n=10; total length 15.85±1.76 cm, mass 54.17±20.30 g, means±s.d.) were used to determine escape latency in teleosts living in the same area (Table 1). Both species were caught by beach seining at Jackson Beach, San Juan Island, WA, USA, in July 2019. All fish were separated by species and kept in several flow-through seawater tanks (90×60×15 cm, 12.0–14.4°C, 12 h:12 h light:dark photoperiod) at Friday Harbor Laboratories, University of Washington. Fish were fed every other day with pieces of shrimp and fasted for 2 days before the experiment commenced. All experiments were performed under IACUC protocol number 4238-03.

Experimental setup

Experimental trials inducing escape responses in the Pacific spiny dogfish, S. suckleyi, were performed in a 3870 l circular flow-through experimental tank with a diameter of 3.60 m and a water height of 45 cm. A cylindrical PVC tube with a diameter of 17 cm and a height of 1.30 m was hung 3 cm above the water surface and 10 cm from the wall of the tank. Inside the tube was a 75 cl plastic bottle, filled with sand, hung from a string and dropped from a height of 1.33 m. The stimulus was dropped from inside the tube to ensure that the sharks would not be able to detect the object falling (i.e. no visual stimulation) and the time of mechanical stimulation was considered to correspond to the frame in which the object broke the surface of the water (t0) (Fig. 1A). The stimulus was not initiated if the shark was between the stimulus and the closest wall and as fish escape responses typically result in a bend away from the stimulus (Domenici, 2010) the sharks always had plenty of space between them and the opposite wall towards which they escaped. A GoPro (GoPro Hero5 Black) was placed 3.45 m above the water surface and 35 cm from the tank wall, next to the tube, recording the escape response at a frame rate of 240 frames s−1. A mirror was attached to the side of the tank 15 cm from the tube and placed right at the water surface at a 45 deg angle. This enabled the detection of stimulus onset, i.e. when the stimulus broke the water surface (t0).

The experimental setup for the great sculpin consisted of a flow-through tank (130×110×74 cm) and smaller flow-through tank (130×60×40 cm) for the pile perch with a water height of 21 cm for both. The test area for the pile perches was smaller to allow fish to remain within the camera's field of view. A cylindrical PVC tube with a diameter of 10 cm and a height of 54 cm was placed 6 cm off center from the long axis, with its lower edge 2 cm above the water. A 50 ml conical test tube, filled with sand, was hung on a string with an electromagnet, and dropped from a height of 56 cm inside the tube, to avoid visual stimulation while the stimulus fell from above the water. An Olympus Tough TG-870 camera was mounted 77 cm above the water, recording at a frame rate of 240 frames s−1. A mirror was mounted on the side of the tank at a 45 deg angle so the camera could detect the moment when the object broke the water surface (t0).

Protocol

Preliminary trials showed that sharks that had been fasted were more likely to actively swim around the tank in contrast to fed sharks that tended to rest at the bottom. As mostly only the active hungry sharks would repeatedly swim close to the mechanical stimuli, the sharks were starved for 5 days before the experiment commenced (Bangley and Rulifson, 2014). Furthermore, preliminary trials also showed that calm sharks typically swam slowly around the tank close to the edge. Once startled, however, they would immediately increase their swimming speed and frequently cross the center section of the test tank. Following each startle trial, it took 5–15 min for the sharks to calm down and resume their calm pre-stimulus swimming behavior.

The experimental protocol was designed to trigger a maximum of five successful escape responses in each individual with a 30 min re-acclimation period between each response (i.e. double the time it took them to resume calm swimming behavior). Before escape response trials commenced, each individual of the Pacific spiny dogfish was acclimated to the experimental tank for 1 h, until they exhibited calm swimming behavior. The first stimulus was then initiated. If the shark did not react to a stimulus, a second attempt was performed within 60 s. This procedure was followed by a waiting period of 30 min to let the sharks resume calm swimming behavior before the next stimulus onset. Trials were continuously run until five escape responses had been collected for each individual. If an individual showed no response in three consecutive trials, the experiment was terminated after the third trial. As a result, the experiments yielded the following combinations of individual sharks (S) and their responses (R): 3S with 5R, 3S with 4R, 2S with 3R, 1S with 2R, 2S with 1R (i.e. a total of 11 individuals with 37 trials).

Preliminary trials in the two teleosts (great sculpin and pile perch) showed that they resumed pre-startle behavior within 2 min of each startle, defined as resting at the bottom of the tank for the great sculpin and slowly swimming around the tank with no burst swimming and with no frequent stops for the pile perch. Thus, a waiting period of 5 min between each escape response trial was implemented (i.e. double the time it took them to resume calm pre-stimulus behavior). Similar to the shark trials, the teleosts were allowed to acclimate to the test tank for 1 h before escape response trials started, and trials were run continuously until five escape responses per individual were induced, unless an individual showed no response in three consecutive trials. As a result, the trials yielded the following combinations of individual sculpin (Sc) and their responses (R): 4Sc with 5R, 2Sc with 4R, 1Sc with 3R (i.e. a total of 7 individuals with 31 trials). Pile perch (P) and their responses (R) were 6P with 5R, 2P with 4R, 1P with 3R and 1P with 2R (i.e. a total of 10 individuals with 43 trials).

Literature search

A literature search on Google Scholar was performed using a combination the following keywords: fish, teleost, latency, latencies, fast-start, escape response, escape kinematics, Mauthner cells. Studies that had reported the latency ranges, averages and/or minimums of adult or juvenile teleosts were included and resulted in 33 articles reporting latencies of 45 teleost (see Table S1 for the full list from the literature search).

Measurements and statistical analysis

The latency of the escape responses of all three species was measured as the time interval between the mechanical stimulus, i.e. the object, breaking the surface of the water (t0), and the first detectable movement of the test subject initiating an escape response (t1).

To test for differences in latency between the three species, the Pacific spiny dogfish, the great sculpin and the pile perch, both the average latency and the minimum latency were used. The average latency was taken as an average of all successful trials for each individual. The minimum latency was defined as the shortest latency an individual achieved across all its successful trials. Hence, only one data point (for both minimum and average latency) was used per individual in the statistical analysis and the number of fish (n) was 11, 7 and 10 for the dogfish, great sculpin and pile perch, respectively. Both datasets were tested for normality with a Kolmogorov–Smirnov and Lillifors test and for homogeneity with a Bartlett chi-squared test. A one-way ANOVA was then run followed by a Tukey HSD for multiple comparisons. Lastly, a linear regression analysis was performed on the distance between the stimulus and the shark (using the point on the shark that was closest to the stimulus) and the latency of the first escape response of each individual to test whether distance to the stimulus had an effect on latency.

Latency was measured using the video-analysis program Kinovea (version 0.8.15), statistical analysis was performed in Statistica (version 13.3.704) and graphic illustrations were made in Graphpad Prism 8 (version 8.2.1. 2019).

The latency of escape responses (based on n=37 trials) of the Pacific spiny dogfish showed a clear peak in the frequency distribution, with 56.8% being between 70 and 80 ms (Fig. 2). The shortest latency was recorded at 66.67 ms and the mean latency was 97.75±18.17 ms (mean±s.d., n=11; Table 1). We found no effect of the distance between the shark and the stimulus on the latency to response for this species (F1,9=0.9183, P=0.36; R2=0.0927).

Among the teleosts, the mean latency was 44.15±5.39 ms (mean±s.d., n=7) for the great sculpin and 64.33±23.42 ms (mean±s.d., n=10) for the pile perch (Table 1). A one-way ANOVA found significant differences for both the minimum (F2,25=46.2, P<0.0001) and average latency (F2,25=17.8, P<0.0001). Specifically, 90.3% of latencies for the great sculpin and 67.4% for the pile perch were shorter than the shortest dogfish latency (66.7 ms). A post hoc test (Tukey HSD) showed that both the great sculpin (P<0.001) and the pile perch (P<0.001) had significantly shorter minimum and average latencies than the Pacific spiny dogfish whereas neither differed between the two teleost (P=0.624 and P=0.106, respectively) (Fig. 1B,C). Both the great sculpin and pile perch exhibited a clear peak in frequency at 30 ms (Fig. 2) and a minimum latency more than 3 times shorter than that of the Pacific spiny dogfish (Table 1). The shiner perch from Dadda et al. (2010) had a peak between 20 and 30 ms and the silver-spotted sculpin from Bohórquez-Herrera et al. (2013) had a peak at 10 ms (Fig. 2). Both had about 5.5 times lower minimum latency than that of the Pacific spiny dogfish (Table 1). The Pacific spiny dogfish also had between 2.2 and 33.3 times longer minimum latency than the 18 different teleosts species tested at similar temperatures (12.5–16°C) (Table 1). While statistics cannot be run because of the lack of raw data from many previous studies, the relative difference appears considerable. Similarly, the Pacific spiny dogfish had between 2.2 and 33.3 times longer minimum latency than the 48 different teleosts identified across 35 different studies with various environmental and experimental conditions (see Table S1).

It is evident from both the minimum and average latency that the Pacific spiny dogfish reacts slower to a predatory stimulus than all other examined teleosts, probably because of the absence of Mauthner cells (Bone, 1977). Previous studies of fishes with and without these interneurons have shown longer latencies in species lacking them (DiDomenico et al., 1988; Nissanov et al., 1990; Greenwood et al., 2010). For instance, Greenwood et al. (2010) found Tetraodon nigroviridis, which possesses Mauthner cells, to have less than half the response latency than that of Diodon holocanthus (11.2±1.3 and 27.3±1.2 ms, respectively), which lack Mauthner cells. Because Mauthner cell are absent in most adult stages of elasmobranchs (Bone, 1977; Stefanelli, 1980), long escape latencies may be a common feature of this taxon.

Fish size has been related to differences in performance variables in escape responses (Domenici, 2001). Size could also cause longer latencies because of the increased length of axons of the Mauthner cells with increased body size and the possible lack of compensation in larger fish (Funch et al., 1981). However, results from Turesson and Domenici (2007) showed that no relationship could be found between total body length and minimum latency of gray mullets ranging from 6.1 to 28.5 cm. The size range used in this study was larger (76.25±6.05 cm for the Pacific spiny dogfish and 15.85±1.76 cm for the pile perch, means±s.d.) (Table 1). Therefore, we cannot exclude that size may have played a role in the long latencies of the Pacific spiny dogfish, although it is unlikely to have caused such a large difference in latency given that a 22 cm range did not generate any effect (Turesson and Domenici, 2007).

Increasing distance from the fish to where the object breaks the surface of the water (the stimulus) has been shown to increase the latency and slow turning rate during an escape response (Domenici and Batty, 1994; Domenici and Batty, 1997). Here, we found no relationship between the distance to the stimulus and the latency of the Pacific spiny dogfish, possibly because we used a smaller range of stimulus distances (1.1–15.9 cm used here as opposed to 25–55 cm in Domenici and Batty, 1997). Because of the difference in water height between the experimental tank for the teleost and sharks (21 and 45 cm, respectively), we cannot exclude that stimulus distance was consistently longer for the sharks than for the teleost, causing the sharks to have higher latencies. However, we kept the stimulus distance short which is likely to be the reason why we found no relationship between stimulus distance and latency, in line with Domenici and Batty (1997), who found that almost 100% of the escape responses were of short latency for herring below a stimulus distance of 35 cm. In addition, other previous work on teleosts used a longer stimulus range (e.g. 8.3–24.8 cm in Bohórquez-Herrera et al., 2013, and 11.8–37.6 cm in Dadda et al., 2010) than we used (1.1–15.9 cm), yet the latencies found in such previous work were much shorter than those found for the dogfish. Therefore, it is unlikely that an increase in vertical distance of only a few centimeters was the cause of the longer latencies of the Pacific spiny dogfish.

The disadvantage the Pacific spiny dogfish might suffer in terms of predator avoidance by having longer response latencies (Katzir and Camhi, 1993; McCormick et al., 2018; Hecker et al., 2020) could be counteracted by other types of performance in a predator–prey situation. Domenici et al. (2004), for example, found that the Pacific spiny dogfish had a small relative turning radius (in body lengths) compared with most other teleosts, which is probably due to its morphology exhibiting comparatively high maneuverability and flexibility (Webb, 1978a, 1984; Domenici et al., 2004). A tight turning radius can be important during predator–prey interactions (Howland, 1974; Webb, 1976), as prey usually have a much tighter turning radius than their predators, which can be used to their advantage (Weihs and Webb, 1984; Domenici, 2001). Webb (1976) suggested that it is a relevant trait for survival and a key variable when evaluating performance in a predator–prey interaction. Hence, the dogfish species might be able to make up for the longer latency by turning at the same rate as teleosts but with a smaller radius (Domenici et al., 2004), thereby regaining some advantage in maneuverability both as predator and as prey.

Our study shows that the shortest latency of escape responses that the Pacific spiny dogfish was able to achieve was at least 3 times longer than that of teleosts from the same environment. Taken together with results from previous studies, our findings support the hypothesis that the absence of Mauthner cells in the Pacific spiny dogfish and other elasmobranchs may be associated with longer latencies when escaping from a threat.

We thank the University of Washington's Friday Harbor Laboratories, San Juan Islands, Washington, USA, for financial and logistic support as well as the Solar foundation of 1978, the Elisabeth and Knud Petersen foundation and the William Demant foundation for providing us with individual funds to cover travel and tuition costs.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: M. Schakmann, V.B., M. Sogaard, J.J., J.S., P.D.; Methodology: M. Schakmann, V.B., M. Sogaard, J.J., J.S., P.D.; Validation: M. Schakmann, V.B., M. Sogaard, J.J., J.S., P.D.; Formal analysis: M. Schakmann, V.B., J.J., J.S., P.D.; Investigation: M. Schakmann, V.B., M. Sogaard, J.J., J.S., P.D.; Resources: J.J., J.S., P.D.; Data curation: M. Schakmann, V.B., M. Sogaard, J.J., J.S., P.D.; Writing - original draft: M. Schakmann; Writing - review & editing: M. Schakmann, V.B., M. Sogaard, J.J., J.S., P.D.; Visualization: M. Schakmann, V.B., M. Sogaard, J.J., J.S., P.D.; Supervision: J.J., J.S., P.D.; Project administration: V.B., J.J., J.S., P.D.; Funding acquisition: M. Schakmann, V.B., M. Sogaard.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Bangley
,
C. W.
and
Rulifson
,
R. A.
(
2014
).
Feeding habits, daily ration, and potential predatory impact of mature female Spiny Dogfish in North Carolina coastal waters
.
N. Am. J. Fisheries Manag.
34
,
668
-
677
.
Batty
,
R.
(
1989
).
Escape responses of herring larvae to visual stimuli
.
J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. U. K.
69
,
647
-
654
.
Batty
,
R.
and
Blaxter
,
J.
(
1992
).
The effect of temperature on the burst swimming performance of fish larvae
.
J. Exp. Biol.
170
,
187
-
201
.
Bhattacharyya
,
K.
,
McLean
,
D. L.
and
MacIver
,
M. A.
(
2017
).
Visual threat assessment and reticulospinal encoding of calibrated responses in larval zebrafish
.
Curr. Biol.
27
,
2751
-
2762.e6
.
Bohórquez-Herrera
,
J.
,
Kawano
,
S. M.
and
Domenici
,
P.
(
2013
).
Foraging behavior delays mechanically-stimulated escape responses in fish
.
Integr. Comp. Biol.
53
,
780
-
786
.
Bone
,
Q.
(
1977
).
Mauthner neurons in elasmobranchs
.
J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. U. K.
57
,
253
-
259
.
Dadda
,
M.
,
Koolhaas
,
W. H.
and
Domenici
,
P.
(
2010
).
Behavioural asymmetry affects escape performance in a teleost fish
.
Biol. Lett.
6
,
414
-
417
.
DiDomenico
,
R.
,
Nissanov
,
J.
and
Eaton
,
R. C.
(
1988
).
Lateralization and adaptation of a continuously variable behavior following lesions of a reticulospinal command neuron
.
Brain Res.
473
,
15
-
28
.
Domenici
,
P.
(
2001
).
The scaling of locomotor performance in predator–prey encounters: from fish to killer whales
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Part A: Mol. Integr. Physiol.
131
,
169
-
182
.
Domenici
,
P.
(
2010
).
Context–dependent variability in the components of fish escape response: integrating locomotor performance and behavior
.
J. Exp. Zoolog. Part A: Ecol. Genet. Physiol.
313A
,
59
-
79
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Batty
,
R. S.
(
1994
).
Escape manoeuvres of schooling Clupea harengus
.
J. Fish Biol.
45
,
97
-
110
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Batty
,
R. S.
(
1997
).
Escape behaviour of solitary herring (Clupea harengus) and comparisons with schooling individuals
.
Mar. Biol.
128
,
29
-
38
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Blake
,
R.
(
1997
).
The kinematics and performance of fish fast-start swimming
.
J. Exp. Biol.
200
,
1165
-
1178
.
Domenici
,
P.
and
Hale
,
M. E.
(
2019
).
Escape responses of fish: a review of the diversity in motor control, kinematics and behaviour
.
J. Exp. Biol.
222
,
jeb166009
.
Domenici
,
P.
,
Standen
,
E. M.
and
Levine
,
R. P.
(
2004
).
Escape manoeuvres in the spiny dogfish (Squalus acanthias)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
207
,
2339
-
2349
.
Domenici
,
P.
,
Turesson
,
H.
,
Brodersen
,
J.
and
Brönmark
,
C.
(
2008
).
Predator-induced morphology enhances escape locomotion in crucian carp
.
Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
275
,
195
-
201
.
Eaton
,
R. C.
and
Emberley
,
D. S.
(
1991
).
How stimulus direction determines the trajectory of the Mauthner-initiated escape response in a teleost fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
161
,
469
-
487
.
Eaton
,
R. C.
and
Hackett
,
J. T.
(
1984
).
The role of the Mauthner cell in fast-starts involving escape in teleost fishes
. In
Neural Mechanisms of Startle Behavior
(ed. R. C. Eaton), pp.
213
-
266
.
Springer
.
Eaton
,
R. C.
,
Bombardieri
,
R. A.
and
Meyer
,
D. L.
(
1977
).
The Mauthner-initiated startle response in teleost fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
66
,
65
-
81
.
Eaton
,
R. C.
,
Lavender
,
W. A.
and
Wieland
,
C. M.
(
1982
).
Alternative neural pathways initiate fast-start responses following lesions of the Mauthner neuron in goldfish
.
J. Comp. Physiol.
145
,
485
-
496
.
Eaton
,
R. C.
,
Lee
,
R. K. K.
and
Foreman
,
M. B.
(
2001
).
The Mauthner cell and other identified neurons of the brainstem escape network of fish
.
Prog. Neurobiol.
63
,
467
-
485
.
Ford
,
J. K. B.
,
Ellis
,
G. M.
,
Matkin
,
C. O.
,
Wetklo
,
M. H.
,
Barrett-Lennard
,
L. G.
and
Withler
,
R. E.
(
2011
).
Shark predation and tooth wear in a population of northeastern Pacific killer whales
.
Aquatic Biol.
11
,
213
-
224
.
Fuiman
,
L. A.
,
Rose
,
K. A.
,
Cowan
,
J. H.
, Jr.
and
Smith
,
E. P.
(
2006
).
Survival skills required for predator evasion by fish larvae and their relation to laboratory measures of performance
.
Anim. Behav.
71
,
1389
-
1399
.
Funch
,
P. G.
,
Kinsman
,
S. L.
,
Faber
,
D. S.
,
Koenig
,
E.
and
Zottoli
,
S. J.
(
1981
).
Mauthner axon diameter and impulse conduction velocity decrease with growth of goldfish
.
Neurosci. Lett.
27
,
159
-
164
.
Greenwood
,
A. K.
,
Peichel
,
C. L.
and
Zottoli
,
S. J.
(
2010
).
Distinct startle responses are associated with neuroanatomical differences in pufferfishes
.
J. Exp. Biol.
213
,
613
-
620
.
Hale
,
M. E.
(
2000
).
Startle responses of fish without Mauthner neurons: escape behavior of the lumpfish (Cyclopterus lumpus)
.
Biol. Bull.
199
,
180
-
182
.
Hale
,
M. E.
(
2002
).
S-and C-start escape responses of the muskellunge (Esox masquinongy) require alternative neuromotor mechanisms
.
J. Exp. Biol.
205
,
2005
-
2016
.
Hecker
,
A.
,
Schulze
,
W.
,
Oster
,
J.
,
Richter
,
D. O.
and
Schuster
,
S.
(
2020
).
Removing a single neuron in a vertebrate brain forever abolishes an essential behavior
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
117
,
3254
-
3260
.
Howland
,
H. C.
(
1974
).
Optimal strategies for predator avoidance: the relative importance of speed and manoeuvrability
.
J. Theor. Biol.
47
,
333
-
350
.
Issa
,
F. A.
,
O'Brien
,
G.
,
Kettunen
,
P.
,
Sagasti
,
A.
,
Glanzman
,
D. L.
and
Papazian
,
D. M.
(
2011
).
Neural circuit activity in freely behaving zebrafish (Danio rerio)
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
1028
-
1038
.
Katzir
,
G.
and
Camhi
,
J. M.
(
1993
).
Escape response of black mollies (Poecilia sphenops) to predatory dives of a pied kingfisher (Ceryle rudis)
.
Copeia
1993
,
549
-
553
.
Kohashi
,
T.
and
Oda
,
Y.
(
2008
).
Initiation of Mauthner- or non-Mauthner-mediated fast escape evoked by different modes of sensory input
.
J. Neurosci.
28
,
10641
-
10653
.
Korn
,
H.
and
Faber
,
D. S.
(
2005
).
The Mauthner cell half a century later: a neurobiological model for decision-making?
Neuron
47
,
13
-
28
.
Liu
,
K. S.
and
Fetcho
,
J. R.
(
1999
).
Laser ablations reveal functional relationships of segmental hindbrain neurons in zebrafish
.
Neuron
23
,
325
-
335
.
Marras
,
S.
,
Killen
,
S. S.
,
Claireaux
,
G.
,
Domenici
,
P.
and
McKenzie
,
D. J.
(
2011
).
Behavioural and kinematic components of the fast-start escape response in fish: individual variation and temporal repeatability
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
3102
-
3110
.
McCormick
,
M. I.
,
Fakan
,
E.
and
Allan
,
B. J. M.
(
2018
).
Behavioural measures determine survivorship within the hierarchy of whole-organism phenotypic traits
.
Funct. Ecol.
32
,
958
-
969
.
Nasuchon
,
N.
,
Yagi
,
M.
,
Kawabata
,
Y.
,
Gao
,
K.
and
Ishimatsu
,
A.
(
2016
).
Escape responses of the Japanese anchovy Engraulis japonicus under elevated temperature and CO2 conditions
.
Fisheries Sci.
82
,
435
-
444
.
Nissanov
,
J.
,
Eaton
,
R. C.
and
DiDomenico
,
R.
(
1990
).
The motor output of the Mauthner cell, a reticulospinal command neuron
.
Brain Res.
517
,
88
-
98
.
Peng
,
J.
,
Cao
,
Z.-D.
and
Fu
,
S.-J.
(
2014
).
The effects of constant and diel-fluctuating temperature acclimation on the thermal tolerance, swimming capacity, specific dynamic action and growth performance of juvenile Chinese bream
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Part A: Mol. Integr. Physiol.
176
,
32
-
40
.
Penghan
,
L.-Y.
,
Pang
,
X.
and
Fu
,
S.-J.
(
2016
).
The effects of starvation on fast-start escape and constant acceleration swimming performance in rose bitterling (Rhodeus ocellatus) at two acclimation temperatures
.
Fish Physiol. Biochem.
42
,
909
-
918
.
Preuss
,
T.
and
Faber
,
D. S.
(
2003
).
Central cellular mechanisms underlying temperature-dependent changes in the goldfish startle-escape behavior
.
J. Neurosci.
23
,
5617
-
5626
.
Ramasamy
,
R. A.
,
Allan
,
B. J. M.
and
McCormick
,
M. I.
(
2015
).
Plasticity of escape responses: prior predator experience enhances escape performance in a coral reef fish
.
PLoS ONE
10
,
e0132790
.
Seamone
,
S.
,
Blaine
,
T.
and
Higham
,
T. E.
(
2014
).
Sharks modulate their escape behavior in response to predator size, speed and approach orientation
.
Zoology
117
,
377
-
382
.
Stefanelli
,
A.
(
1980
).
I neuroni di Mauthner degli ittiopsidi: valutazioni comparative morfologiche e funzionali
.
Accademia Nazionale dei Lincei
.
Szabo
,
T. M.
,
Brookings
,
T.
,
Preuss
,
T.
and
Faber
,
D. S.
(
2008
).
Effects of temperature acclimation on a central neural circuit and its behavioral output
.
J. Neurophysiol.
100
,
2997
-
3008
.
Turesson
,
H.
and
Domenici
,
P.
(
2007
).
Escape latency is size independent in grey mullet
.
J. Fish Biol.
71
,
253
-
259
.
Vaughn
,
R. L.
,
Shelton
,
D. E.
,
Timm
,
L. L.
,
Watson
,
L. A.
and
Würsig
,
B.
(
2007
).
Dusky dolphin (Lagenorhynchus obscurus) feeding tactics and multi-species associations
.
N. Z. J. Mar. Freshw. Res.
41
,
391
-
400
.
Walker
,
J. A.
,
Ghalambor
,
C. K.
,
Griset
,
O. L.
,
McKenney
,
D.
and
Reznick
,
D. N.
(
2005
).
Do faster starts increase the probability of evading predators?
Funct. Ecol.
19
,
808
-
815
.
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1976
).
The effect of size on the fast-start performance of rainbow trout Salmo gairdneri, and a consideration of piscivorous predator-prey interactions
.
J. Exp. Biol.
65
,
157
-
177
.
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1978a
).
Fast-start performance and body form in seven species of teleost fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
74
,
211
-
226
.
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1978b
).
Temperature effects on acceleration of rainbow trout, Salmo gairdneri
.
J. Fisheries Board Can.
35
,
1417
-
1422
.
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1980
).
Does schooling reduce fast-start response latencies in teleosts?
Comp. Biochem. Physiol.
65A
,
231
-
234
.
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1982
).
Avoidance responses of fathead minnow to strikes by four teleost predators
.
J. Comp. Physiol.
147
,
371
-
378
.
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1984
).
Body form, locomotion and foraging in aquatic vertebrates
.
Am. Zool.
24
,
107
-
120
.
Weihs
,
D.
and
Webb
,
P. W.
(
1984
).
Optimal avoidance and evasion tactics in predator-prey interactions
.
J. Theoretical Biol.
106
,
189
-
206
.
Yasargil
,
G. M.
and
Diamond
,
J.
(
1968
).
Startle-response in teleost fish: an elementary circuit for neural discrimination
.
Nature
220
,
241
-
243
.
Zottoli
,
S. J.
(
1977
).
Correlation of the startle reflex and Mauthner cell auditory responses in unrestrained goldfish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
66
,
243
.
Zottoli
,
S. J.
and
Faber
,
D. S.
(
2000
).
Review: the mauthner cell: what has it taught us?
Neuroscientist
6
,
26
-
38
.
Zottoli
,
S. J.
,
Newman
,
B. C.
,
Rieff
,
H. I.
and
Winters
,
D. C.
(
1999
).
Decrease in occurrence of fast startle responses after selective Mauthner cell ablation in goldfish (Carassius auratus)
.
J. Comp. Physiol. A
184
,
207
-
218
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information