Pronounced dive responses through peripheral vasoconstriction and bradycardia enable prolonged apnoea in marine mammals. For most vertebrates, the dive response is initiated upon face immersion, but little is known about the physical drivers of diving and surfacing heart rate in cetaceans whose faces are always mostly submerged. Using two trained harbour porpoises instrumented with an ECG-measuring sound-and-movement tag (DTAG-3), we investigated the initiation and progression of bradycardia and tachycardia during apnoea and eupnoea for varying levels of immersion. We show that paranasal wetting drives bradycardia initiation and progression, whereas apnoea leads to dive-level bradycardia eventually, but not instantly. At the end of dives, heart rate accelerates independently of lung expansion, perhaps in anticipation of surfacing; however, full tachycardia is only engaged upon inhalation. We conclude that breathing drives surface tachycardia, whereas blowhole wetting is an important driver of bradycardia; however, anticipatory/volitional modulation can overrule such responses to sensory inputs.

The dive response is a critical enabler of extended underwater foraging in marine mammals by prolonging both aerobic and anaerobic dive times. It is, to some degree, found in all air-breathing vertebrates as a response to submersion and apnoea. The peripheral vasoconstriction and concurrent bradycardia conserve blood oxygen for the hypoxia-sensitive brain and heart (Dykes, 1974a; Irving et al., 1942; Scholander, 1940; Zapol et al., 1979), making myoglobin-bound oxygen stores available for local use (Kooyman, 1985).

Although the dive response in marine mammals has been broadly considered a strict reflex (Scholander, 1940, 1963), studies from the past half­-century have demonstrated that the response is highly dynamic (Harrison et al., 1972; Jones et al., 1973; Kooyman and Campbell, 1972), depending on dive depth, duration, exercise (Andrews et al., 1997; Boyd et al., 1999; Hill et al., 1987; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; McDonald et al., 2018; Noren et al., 2012; Williams et al., 2015) and volitional control (Elmegaard et al., 2016; Elsner et al., 1966; Noren et al., 2012). For most vertebrates, the dive response is triggered by apnoea and stimulation of thermoreceptors and mechanoreceptors in the paranasal area (Dykes, 1974a,b; Panneton, 2013), but pinnipeds can initiate a strong dive response-like bradycardia in air, during trained breath holding (Kaczmarek et al., 2018; Ridgway et al., 1975) as well as naturally occurring sleep apnoea (Andrews et al., 1997; Castellini et al., 1994). A facial bradycardia trigger has never been tested for in cetaceans, for which the trait might be unfavourable given that most of their face is submerged, even when breathing.

Marine mammals maximize the time they spend submerged by efficient respiratory gas exchange at the surface, resulting from a greatly accelerated heart rate (fH) during the inspiratory phase of respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) (Hayano et al., 1996). The gas exchange is further augmented by a pre-surfacing tachycardia to facilitate the use of remaining lung and blood oxygen stores at the end of a dive, while re-perfused tissues release carbon dioxide and nitrogen into the blood and lungs (Irving et al., 1941; Kooyman and Campbell, 1972; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; Noren et al., 2012; Thompson and Fedak, 1993). Pre-surfacing tachycardia is often ascribed to the anticipation of surfacing or breathing (Kooyman and Campbell, 1972; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014), and has been shown to be initiated during depressurisation in seals (Kooyman and Campbell, 1972).

Here, we investigated the physical stimulators of diving bradycardia and surfacing tachycardia, whether through autonomic or volitional routes, to better understand the drivers of the cetacean diving physiology. By comparing (1) how the onset of bradycardia relates temporally to breaths and submergence, (2) the progression of bradycardia between trained tasks and (3) the fH acceleration when ending these tasks with or without lung ventilation, we tested the hypotheses that submersion is a primary driver of diving bradycardia, as predicted from other vertebrates, or alternatively that RSA alone drives diving bradycardia and surfacing tachycardia, as predicted from convergently evolved pinnipeds.

Animals and equipment

ECG data were collected from June to October 2015 from two captive female harbour porpoises, Phocoena phocoena (Linnaeus 1758), Freja and Sif, at the Fjord & Belt Centre in Kerteminde, Denmark. At the time of data collection, Freja and Sif had been in captivity for 18 and 11 years, respectively, and weighed 54 and 50 kg in August 2015. Both porpoises were by-caught as yearlings in pound nets in Danish waters, and were subsequently housed and trained at the Fjord & Belt Centre under permits SN 343/FY-0014 and 1996-3446-0021 from the Ministry of Environment and Food of Denmark, with approval from the IACUC committee of Aarhus University, and in accordance with recommendations of the Danish Council for Experiments on Animals.

fH was measured with an animal-borne sound-and-movement tag, ECG-DTAG-3, with two external chlorinated silver electrodes embedded in silicone suction cups for easy non-invasive attachment to the animals. Electrodes were attached on each side of the animal, rostral and caudal to the heart, approximately along the axis of the ventricle contraction for an optimal QRS-complex signal (Fig. 1A). The potential difference between the two electrodes was sampled at 10 kHz with 16-bit resolution, and a 4 kHz, 2-pole anti-alias filter. A small amount of conductive paste (Ten20, Weaver and Company, CO, USA) was applied on the silver electrodes to improve skin contact. The tag also recorded sound and pressure, which were used to identify breaths and dive times. Stereo sound was sampled at 500 kHz with 16-bit resolution and a 0.5–150 kHz bandwidth; pressure was sampled at 2 kHz with 16-bit resolution (see McDonald et al., 2018, for details).

Breath-holding tasks

To investigate the initiation and progress of bradycardia, the two porpoises were trained to wear the ECG-DTAG-3 during a suite of trained behaviours of similar low-activity exercise levels and of approximately 20 s duration, during which they would breath hold (Fig. 1B–G). The task duration was defined from the last breath before the task until the first breath when task was completed. One task was performed with the blowhole mostly in air (dorsal swimming, DSw); three were performed with the blowhole right below the surface (ventral swimming, VSw; dorsal station, DSt; ventral station, VSt); one was performed at 1 m depth (station at 1 m depth, ST); and one was performed on land (beaching, B). This allowed examination of the influence of submergence level on fH. When the animals completed a task correctly, the trainer used a bridge signal (i.e. blew a whistle, ∼6 kHz) to get them to return for a fish reward. In between tasks, the animals spent a minimum of 1 min in low activity at the surface with respiratory frequencies of 6.7 breaths min−1 (Sif) and 9.7 breaths min−1 (Freja). This ensured CO2 elimination and full recovery from the preceding task, which is expected after 6–8 breaths for these short dive times (Boutilier et al., 2001).

Dorsal swimming (DSw)

The porpoises swam calmly at the surface with the blowhole mostly in air (Fig. 1B). When breath-hold time reached 15–20 s, the bridge signal was sounded and the animals returned to the trainer, which usually was at a depth of a few metres. Sometimes, they took a breath when they heard the bridge signal, and sometimes they waited until they were back with the trainer; therefore, the duration of this task was defined from the last breath until the bridge signal. By closely inspecting video recordings of tasks, we found average lag times of 2 s (Freja) and close to 0 s (Sif) from the last breath until initiating the dorsal swim.

Ventral swimming (VSw)

This task proceeded as for DSw above, except the porpoises swam with the ventral-side up (Fig. 1C). While the ventral orientation is mostly known from foraging or mating manoeuvres in freely swimming porpoises (Akamatsu et al., 2010; Keener et al., 2018), it here serves to compare fH in a task with the blowhole submerged with that in a task where the blowhole is in air, while the same approximate proportion of the body is in the water for comparable swimming resistances and body submergence sensory input. The average lag time from the last breath until submergence of the blowhole was 2 s for both animals.

Dorsal station (DSt)

The porpoises stationed at the trainer's hand, approximately 10 cm below the surface (Fig. 1D). The average lag time from the last breath until blowhole submergence was 2 s (Freja) and close to 0 s (Sif).

Ventral station (VSt)

The porpoises stationed at the trainer's hand in a belly-up position (Fig. 1E). Comparing the fH between this task and DSt informed us about the potential cost of the ventral position. In both tasks, the porpoise was at the surface with the blowhole submerged. The average lag time from the last breath until blowhole submergence was 2 s (Freja) and 3 s (Sif).

Station at 1 m depth (ST)

The porpoises swam to a biteplate station at 1 m depth (Fig. 1F) where they remained for approximately 10 s before the bridge signal was sounded, resulting in a 15–20 s breath hold. Both porpoises took their last breath just as they dived, meaning no lag time from the last breath until blowhole submergence.

Beaching (B)

To investigate the effect of apnoea alone on breath-holding bradycardia, fH was measured when Freja beached herself on a platform for 15 s (Fig. 1G). After the last breath, approximately 6 s were spent swimming for momentum to jump onto the platform. After 15 s on the platform, the bridge signal was sounded and Freja slid back into the water to swim to the trainer for a reward, resulting in a total breath hold of ∼40 s. fH parameters were calculated for the beaching period only so, unlike the other tasks, the breath hold had already been ongoing for ∼6 s. This behaviour was only repeated 5 times to prevent loss of motivation to perform an important behaviour for health monitoring that is normally just a few seconds long. Therefore, we used it for qualitative comparison only.

Data processing and analysis

Data were processed and prepared using custom-written scripts in MatLab (The MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA). R-peaks of the ECG data were identified with a peak detector and corrected by visual inspection. The instantaneous fH (beats min−1) for each heartbeat was then calculated from inter-peak intervals (fH=60/inter-peak interval). Respirations were detected by aural and visual audit of sound files and spectrograms.

Does bradycardia initiate with apnoea or submergence?

In order to test whether blowhole submergence and bradycardia initiation were tightly linked, as predicted from the classical dive reflex, we examined fH specifically at the points of blowhole submergence, identified by synchronised video recordings. Thus, we could determine whether the initiation related temporally to the respiration or to the time of submergence. For graphical presentation of data, fH was binned in 1 s bins for each trial. Then, for each task, mean and s.e.m. were calculated for each bin. For this question, we focused solely on the breaking point of when the fH started to decline after task initiation.

Is fH influenced by task and submergence level?

To examine the impact of task and submergence level on fH, we compared the initial 15 s of the tasks as the exact duration varied. Residuals were normally distributed, but heteroscedastic, so we performed Welch's ANOVA and Games–Howell post hoc analyses of the lower quartiles (LQ) of the instantaneous fH (i.e. 25th percentile, the median of the lower half of instantaneous fH) between the tasks described (DSw, VSw, DSt, VSt, ST) using the R package userfriendlyscience (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=userfriendlyscience; R v.3.5.2, R Foundation for Statistical Computing, http://www.R-project.org/). In the analysis of variance between tasks, the null hypotheses were rejected at P<0.05. The effect of exercise (using acceleration data as an index for activity) on LQ fH was examined with a generalised least squares model using the R package nlme (https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme) and found to be insignificant between the tasks performed, which were all of low activity. In Table S1, a table of fH parameters can be found.

Is surface tachycardia driven by anticipation or breathing?

To investigate whether surface tachycardia depends on lung expansion, we examined fH in the 3 s before and after the breath ending each breath-hold task. In most cases, the porpoises took a full breath after completing a task, but sometimes they only exhaled (probably a training artefact). These differing breaths allowed us to examine whether, and to what extent, surface tachycardia was due to anticipation of surfacing or lung ventilation. Some tasks (B, DSw, VSw – see above description) were excluded because the task-ending breath did not directly follow the shift in behaviour from performing the task to surfacing. In these tasks, porpoises swam back to the trainer in a variety of activity levels, which was reflected in their presumably exercise-modulated fH, potentially masking the initial surface tachycardia. The instantaneous fH around the breaths was plotted for respiration types and animals, and smooth trend lines were added using a local polynomial regression fitting algorithm using the ggplot2 R package (Wickham, 2016).

Like many marine mammals, harbour porpoises have a bimodal fH, with low fH during dives, ensuring conservation of oxygen, and high fH at the surface, for fast and efficient gas exchange (Eskesen et al., 2009; Kastelein and Meijler, 1989; McDonald et al., 2018; Reed et al., 2000). Accordingly, the porpoises in this study displayed a breath-holding (apnoeic) submergence mode of 76 beats min−1 (Freja) and 77 beats min−1 (Sif) and a breathing (eupnoeic) mode of 143 beats min−1 (Freja) and 162 beats min−1 (Sif) during surface intervals (Fig. S1). Here, we investigated the drivers of these modes, which are ascribed to both the dive response and RSA (Fahlman et al., 2019).

Does bradycardia initiate with apnoea or submergence?

We hypothesized that apnoea is the primary driver of diving bradycardia in harbour porpoises, as found for pinnipeds (Kaczmarek et al., 2018), as the facial triggers of the classic dive response might be counterproductive, given their constant facial submersion, where only the blowhole area is exposed during ventilations (Dykes, 1974b; Kaczmarek et al., 2018; Panneton, 2013). From the first seconds of fH during the tasks (Fig. 2A,B), it was apparent that breath holding is not the primary trigger of bradycardia; rather, bradycardia is initiated at the time of blowhole submergence. Without blowhole submersion, bradycardia developed more gradually during a breath hold (Fig. 2A,B: DSw). Thus, for harbour porpoises, apnoea is not an instant trigger, in contrast to recent findings in some pinnipeds (Kaczmarek et al., 2018). Instead, the correlation with submergence indicates that the nasal sensory mechanism responsible for initiating the dive response migrated with the nares from the tip of the snout to the top of the cranium during the protocetid evolution, although we cannot account for the conservation or loss of additional contributing facial sensory afferents. Blowhole submersion was associated with immediate bradycardia in dorsally oriented tasks (Fig. 2A,B: DSt, ST) and during the time intervals between tasks (Fig. 2C). In contrast, bradycardia onset was delayed several seconds into submergence during 52–74% of ventrally oriented tasks (example in Fig. 2C). This indicates that higher level anticipatory control can modify the onset of bradycardia, consistent with the anticipatory control of diving fH that was recently demonstrated in porpoises (Elmegaard et al., 2016). It could be that cetaceans can override or evoke bradycardia and peripheral vasoconstriction by volition, akin to their completely voluntary breathing (Lilly, 1958).

Is bradycardia influenced by task and submergence level?

We hypothesised that the progression and severity of diving bradycardia, once initiated, depends on the level of submergence, within the limit of 1 m depth. A relationship between dive depth and degree of diving bradycardia has been documented in several marine mammal species (McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; Williams et al., 2015). In support of this hypothesis, both porpoises exhibited the lowest LQ fH at 1 m depth (Freja: 58.8±1.5 beats min−1; Sif: 63.5±0.7 beats min−1) and the highest LQ fH with the blowhole in air (Freja: 108.8±3.3 beats min−1; Sif: 151.8±3.5 beats min−1) (Fig. 2A,B; Table S1). However, when investigating the relationship between submergence level and fH for all of the behaviours, we found it was not consistent between the two porpoises. For Freja, there was a statistically significant difference of LQ fH between some of the tasks (Welch's one-way ANOVA: F4,59.7=68.5, P<0.0001), where LQ fH grouped according to a gradual response to submergence (see Games–Howell post hoc analysis results in Table S2). The lowest breath-hold fH was at 1 m depth (ST), there was an intermediate breath-hold fH category just below the surface (DSt, VSt, VSw) and the highest breath-hold fH was found when the blowhole was out of the water (DSw). This is similar to what was observed in trained bottlenose dolphins and killer whales, which displayed a more profound bradycardia at 2 and 5 m depth, respectively, compared with that at the surface (Bickett et al., 2019; Elsner et al., 1966). In contrast, Sif did not display as clear a relationship between submergence level and fH; rather, each task had a distinctive bradycardia progression (Welch's one-way ANOVA: F4,36.6=265.3, P<0.0001) in spite of similar levels and durations of exercise and submergence (Fig. 2A,B; Table S2). The only LQ fH that did not differ from each other were for DSt and ST (Games–Howell post hoc analysis: P=0.999), showing that the level of bradycardia observed at 1 m depth can also be obtained just below the surface. The inconsistencies between the porpoises may be partly due to the limited range (1 m) of submergence depth in this study. Although depth, duration and exercise are known to affect diving fH (Andrews et al., 1997; Boyd et al., 1999; Hill et al., 1987; McDonald et al., 2018; McDonald and Ponganis, 2014; Noren et al., 2012; Williams et al., 2015), so does anticipatory fH regulation (Elmegaard et al., 2016; Elsner et al., 1966; Noren et al., 2012), which may also offer an explanation for this variation. Such a strong anticipatory influence on fH entails concerns as to how cetaceans could respond to some anthropogenic noises while at depth. For example, stranding of several cetacean species has been linked to naval sonar exercises (Frantzis, 1998; Wright et al., 2013), and some of these beached animals had severe gas emboli, suggesting problems with gas management (Jepson et al., 2003; Rommel et al., 2006). Altered diving behaviour as well as altered cardiovascular regulation by diversion of attention away from aspects of the diving response that are under volitional control could explain such fatal outcomes (Fahlman et al., 2006).

To investigate whether the dive response would be absent without the sensory input associated with submergence, we recorded fH when Freja was out of the water (beaching behaviour: B; Fig. 2A). At the end of the beaching behaviour, fH was strikingly similar to the bradycardia obtained when Freja was submerged near the surface (DSt, VSt and VSw; Fig. 2A). This indicates that bradycardia is not contingent on immersion, although the data suggest that blowhole wetting and complete submergence are important drivers of the immediate initiation and severity of bradycardia.

Is surface tachycardia driven by anticipation or breathing?

Marine mammals maximise submergence time by exhibiting both a diving bradycardia and a pronounced surface and pre-surface tachycardia. Pre-surfacing fH acceleration was initiated by depressurisation during a forced dive of an elephant seal in a pressure chamber, suggesting that baroreceptors or lung expansion may trigger the tachycardia (Kooyman and Campbell, 1972). Pre-surfacing acceleration of heart rate has also been noted in trained tursiops when surfacing from 2 to 15 m depth (Elsner et al., 1966; Irving et al., 1941; Noren et al., 2012). We investigated pre- and post-surfacing fH, independent of substantial pressure changes and pre-surfacing lung expansion (all depths ≤1 m) by comparing fH changes before and after the first breath at the end of each DSt, VSt and ST task (Fig. 3). We found pre-surfacing fH acceleration without depressurisation and also without a subsequent lung ventilation, leading us to posit that fH increases in anticipation of surfacing, rather than in anticipation of breathing per se. The supposed tachycardia-facilitated wash out of carbon dioxide and nitrogen from tissues to the blood and lungs as well as residual oxygen uptake from the blood may be beneficial whether the breath is a few seconds or a few tens of seconds away (Thompson and Fedak, 1993). Following an exhalation only, fH decreased again, after reaching maximum values of 91.4±3.4 beats min−1 (Freja) and 94.9±4.1 beats min−1 (Sif), whereas full breaths with lung ventilation led to full tachycardia of 115.2±2.1 beats min−1 (Freja) and 146.0±1.5 beats min−1 (Sif). It may be noted that these investigations, as well as prior investigations of similar topics, were performed with trained animals. A controlled study with specified behaviours would be very hard to pursue in animals in the wild, and such studies thus emphasise the value of healthy trained captive animals for advancing knowledge of animals in the wild.

Conclusion

We conclude that harbour porpoises exhibit dynamic diving fH during short breath-hold submergence, and that diving fH is highly context dependent, highlighting that the cardiovascular adjustments to diving are a regulated response rather than a reflex. We found that blowhole submersion, rather than apnoea alone, drives the initiation and rate of bradycardia; however, breath holding in air also leads to submergence-level bradycardia, albeit with slower progression. We further show that anticipatory fH acceleration occurs independent of depressurisation and lung expansion, although it only leads to full tachycardia upon lung ventilation. Thus, the pronounced fH dynamics of harbour porpoises may be categorised as respiration-driven tachycardia and submergence-driven bradycardia, and not just an extreme sinus arrhythmia. We conclude that blowhole wetting is an important sensory input for fH regulation in cetaceans; however, higher-level control may define both the timing and progression of fH dynamics.

M. Johnson's creation of the ECG-DTAG-3 as well as custom analysis software critically enabled this study. R. Swift helped prepare the tag and electrodes. Furthermore, the study depended on the skilled and dedicated effort of the animal trainers at the Fjord & Belt Centre: J. H. Kristensen, J. Larsson, C. Eriksson and F. Johansson. We finally wish to thank M. Johnson, A. Fahlman and M. Bayley as well as the two reviewers for valuable feedback and critique to improve the final manuscript.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: S.L.E., P.T.M.; Methodology: S.L.E., B.I.M., P.T.M.; Software: S.L.E., B.I.M.; Validation: S.L.E., P.T.M.; Formal analysis: S.L.E.; Investigation: S.L.E.; Resources: S.L.E., B.I.M., P.T.M.; Data curation: S.L.E.; Writing - original draft: S.L.E.; Writing - review & editing: S.L.E., B.I.M., P.T.M.; Visualization: S.L.E.; Supervision: P.T.M.; Project administration: P.T.M.; Funding acquisition: B.I.M., P.T.M.

Funding

The Office of Naval Research supported this research through grant N000141210633 and N000141512852 to P.T.M. and B.I.M. The Danish Research Council (Det Frie Forskningsråd | Natur og Univers) funded training time and equipment through grants to P.T.M. B.I.M. was supported by a National Science Foundation International Research Postdoctoral Fellowship (OISE - 1159123).

Akamatsu
,
T.
,
Wang
,
D.
,
Wang
,
K.
,
Li
,
S.
and
Dong
,
S.
(
2010
).
Scanning sonar of rolling porpoises during prey capture dives
.
J. Exp. Biol.
213
,
146
-
152
.
Andrews
,
R. D.
,
Jones
,
D. R.
,
Williams
,
J. D.
,
Thorson
,
P. H.
,
Oliver
,
G. W.
,
Costa
,
D. P.
and
Le Boeuf
,
B. J.
(
1997
).
Heart rates of northern elephant seals diving at sea and resting on the beach
.
J. Exp. Biol.
200
,
2083
-
2095
.
Bickett
,
N. J.
,
Tift
,
M. S.
,
St. Leger
,
J.
and
Ponganis
,
P. J.
(
2019
).
Heart rates, heart rate profiles, and electrocardiograms in three killer whales, a beluga, and a pilot whale: An exploratory investigation
.
Mar. Mammal Sci.
35
,
1112
-
1132
.
Boutilier
,
R. G.
,
Reed
,
J. Z.
and
Fedak
,
M. A.
(
2001
).
Unsteady-state gas exchange and storage in diving marine mammals: the harbor porpoise and gray seal
.
Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol.
281
,
R490
-
R494
.
Boyd
,
I. L.
,
Bevan
,
R. M.
,
Woakes
,
A. J.
and
Butler
,
P. J.
(
1999
).
Heart rate and behavior of fur seals: implications for measurement of field energetics
.
Am. J. Physiol.
276
,
H844
-
H857
.
Castellini
,
M. A.
,
Milsom
,
W. K.
,
Berger
,
R. J.
,
Costa
,
D. P.
,
Jones
,
D. R.
,
Castellini
,
J. M.
,
Rea
,
L. D.
,
Bharma
,
S.
and
Harris
,
M.
(
1994
).
Patterns of respiration and heart rate during wakefulness and sleep in elephant seal pups
.
Am. J. Physiol.
266
,
R863
-
R869
.
Dykes
,
R. W.
(
1974a
).
Factors related to the dive reflex in harbor seals: respiration, immersion bradycardia, and lability of the heart rate
.
Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol.
52
,
248
-
258
.
Dykes
,
R. W.
(
1974b
).
Factors related to the dive reflex in harbor seals: sensory contributions from the trigeminal region
.
Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol.
52
,
259
-
265
.
Elmegaard
,
S. L.
,
Johnson
,
M.
,
Madsen
,
P. T.
and
McDonald
,
B. I.
(
2016
).
Cognitive control of heart rate in diving harbor porpoises
.
Curr. Biol.
26
,
R1175
-
R1176
.
Elsner
,
R.
,
Kenney
,
D. W.
and
Burgess
,
K.
(
1966
).
Diving bradycardia in the trained dolphin
.
Nature
212
,
407
-
408
.
Eskesen
,
I. G.
,
Teilmann
,
J.
,
Geertsen
,
B. M.
,
Desportes
,
G.
,
Riget
,
F.
,
Dietz
,
R.
,
Larsen
,
F.
and
Siebert
,
U.
(
2009
).
Stress level in wild harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) during satellite tagging measured by respiration, heart rate and cortisol
.
J. Mar. Biol. Assoc. United Kingdom
89
,
885
-
892
.
Fahlman
,
A.
,
Olszowka
,
A.
,
Bostrom
,
B.
and
Jones
,
D. R.
(
2006
).
Deep diving mammals: Dive behavior and circulatory adjustments contribute to bends avoidance
.
Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol.
153
,
66
-
77
.
Fahlman
,
A.
,
Miedler
,
S.
,
Rocho-Levine
,
J.
,
Jabois
,
A.
,
Arenarez
,
J.
,
Marti-Bonmati
,
L.
,
García-Párraga
,
D.
and
Cauture
,
F.
(
2019
).
Re-evaluating the significance of the dive response during voluntary surface apneas in the bottlenose dolphin, Tursiops truncatus
.
Sci. Rep.
9
,
1
-
10
.
Frantzis
,
A.
(
1998
).
Does acoustic testing strand whales?
Nature
392
,
29
.
Harrison
,
R. J.
,
Ridgway
,
S. H.
and
Joyce
,
P. L.
(
1972
).
Telemetry of heart rate in diving seals
.
Nature
238
,
280
.
Hayano
,
J.
,
Yasuma
,
F.
,
Okada
,
A.
,
Mukai
,
S.
and
Fujinami
,
T.
(
1996
).
Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: a phenomenon improving pulmonary gas exchange and circulatory efficiency
.
Circulation
94
,
842
-
847
.
Hill
,
R. D.
,
Schneider
,
R. C.
,
Liggins
,
G. C.
,
Schuette
,
A. H.
,
Elliott
,
R. L.
,
Guppy
,
M.
,
Hochachka
,
P. W.
,
Qvist
,
J.
,
Falke
,
K. J.
and
Zapol
,
W. M.
(
1987
).
Heart rate and body temperature during free diving of Weddell seals
.
Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol.
253
,
R344
-
R351
.
Irving
,
L.
,
Scholander
,
P. F.
and
Grinnell
,
S. W.
(
1941
).
The respiration of the porpoise, Tursiops truncatus
.
J. Cell. Comp. Physiol.
17
,
145
-
168
.
Irving
,
L.
,
Scholander
,
P. F.
and
Grinnell
,
S. W.
(
1942
).
The regulation of arterial blood pressure in the seal during diving
.
Am. J. Physiol.
135
,
557
-
566
.
Jepson
,
P. D.
,
Arbelo
,
M.
,
Deaville
,
R.
,
Patterson
,
I. A. P.
,
Castro
,
P.
,
Baker
,
J. R.
,
Degollada
,
E.
,
Ross
,
H. M.
,
Herráez
,
P.
,
Pocknell
,
A. M.
, et al. 
(
2003
).
Gas-bubble lesions in stranded cetaceans
.
Nature
425
,
575
-
576
.
Jones
,
D. R.
,
Fisher
,
H. D.
,
McTaggart
,
S.
and
West
,
N. H.
(
1973
).
Heart rate during breath-holding and diving in the unrestrained harbor seal (Phoca vitulina richardi)
.
Can. J. Zool.
51
,
671
-
680
.
Kaczmarek
,
J.
,
Reichmuth
,
C.
,
McDonald
,
B. I.
,
Kristensen
,
J. H.
,
Larson
,
J.
,
Johansson
,
F.
,
Sullivan
,
J. L.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2018
).
Drivers of the dive response in pinnipeds; apnea, submergence or temperature?
J. Exp. Biol.
221
,
jeb176545
.
Kastelein
,
R. A.
and
Meijler
,
F. L.
(
1989
).
Respiratory arrhythmia in the hearts of harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena)
.
Aquat. Mamm.
15
,
57
-
63
.
Keener
,
W.
,
Webber
,
M. A.
,
Szczepaniak
,
I. D.
,
Markowitz
,
T. M.
and
Orbach
,
D. N.
(
2018
).
The sex life of harbor porpoises (Phocoena phocoena): lateralized and aerial behavior
.
Aquat. Mam
44
,
620
-
632
.
Kooyman
,
G. L.
(
1985
).
Physiology without restraint in diving mammals
.
Mar. Mammal Sci.
1
,
166
-
178
.
Kooyman
,
G. L.
and
Campbell
,
W. B.
(
1972
).
Heart rates in freely diving Weddell seals, Leptonychotes weddelli
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol.
43A
,
31
-
36
.
Lilly
,
J. C.
(
1958
).
Some considerations regarding basic mechanisms of positive and negative types of motivations
.
Am. J. Psychiatry
115
,
498
-
504
.
McDonald
,
B. I.
and
Ponganis
,
P. J.
(
2014
).
Deep-diving sea lions exhibit extreme bradycardia in long-duration dives
.
J. Exp. Biol.
217
,
1525
-
1534
.
McDonald
,
B. I.
,
Johnson
,
M.
and
Madsen
,
P. T.
(
2018
).
Dive heart rate in harbour porpoises is influenced by exercise and expectations
.
J. Exp. Biol.
221
,
jeb168740
.
Noren
,
S. R.
,
Kendall
,
T.
,
Cuccurullo
,
V.
and
Williams
,
T. M.
(
2012
).
The dive response redefined: underwater behavior influences cardiac variability in freely diving dolphins
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
2735
-
2741
.
Panneton
,
W. M.
(
2013
).
The mammalian diving response: an enigmatic reflex to preserve life?
Physiology
28
,
284
-
297
.
Reed
,
J. Z.
,
Chambers
,
C.
,
Hunter
,
C. J.
,
Lockyer
,
C.
,
Kastelein
,
R.
,
Fedak
,
M. A.
and
Boutilier
,
R. G.
(
2000
).
Gas exchange and heart rate in the harbour porpoise, Phocoena phocoena
.
J. Comp. Physiol. B Biochem. Syst. Environ. Physiol.
170
,
1
-
10
.
Ridgway
,
S. H.
,
Carder
,
D. A.
and
Clark
,
W.
(
1975
).
Conditioned bradycardia in the sea lion Zalophus californianus
.
Nature
256
,
1
-
2
.
Rommel
,
S. A.
,
Costidis
,
A. M.
,
Fernandez
,
A.
,
Jepson
,
P. D.
,
Pabst
,
D. A.
,
McLellan
,
W. A.
,
Houser
,
D. S.
,
Cranford
,
T. W.
,
Van Helden
,
A. L.
,
Allen
,
D. M.
, et al. 
(
2006
).
Elements of beaked whale anatomy and diving physiology and some hypothetical causes of sonar-related stranding
.
J. Cetacean Res. Manag.
7
,
189
-
209
.
Scholander
,
P. F.
(
1940
).
Experimental investigations on the respiratory function in diving mammals and birds
. In
Hvalradets Skrifter
, pp.
1
-
131
.
Hvalradets Skrifter
.
Scholander
,
P. F.
(
1963
).
The master switch of life
.
Sci. Am.
209
,
92
-
106
.
Thompson
,
D.
and
Fedak
,
M. A.
(
1993
).
Cardiac responses of grey seals during diving at sea
.
J. Exp. Biol.
174
,
139
-
154
.
Wickham
,
H.
(
2016
).
ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis
.
New York
:
Springer-Verlag
.
Williams
,
T. M.
,
Fuiman
,
L. A.
,
Kendall
,
T.
,
Berry
,
P.
,
Richter
,
B.
,
Noren
,
S. R.
,
Thometz
,
N.
,
Shattock
,
M. J.
,
Farrell
,
E.
,
Stamper
,
A. M.
, et al. 
(
2015
).
Exercise at depth alters bradycardia and incidence of cardiac anomalies in deep-diving marine mammals
.
Nat. Commun.
6
,
6055
.
Wright
,
A. J.
,
Maar
,
M.
,
Mohn
,
C.
,
Nabe-nielsen
,
J.
,
Siebert
,
U.
,
Jensen
,
L. F.
,
Baagøe
,
H. J.
and
Teilmann
,
J.
(
2013
).
Possible causes of a harbour porpoise mass stranding in Danish waters in 2005
.
PLoS ONE
8
,
e55553
.
Zapol
,
W. M.
,
Liggins
,
G. C.
,
Schneider
,
R. C.
,
Qvist
,
J.
,
Snider
,
M. T.
,
Creasy
,
R. K.
and
Hochachka
,
P. W.
(
1979
).
Regional blood flow during simulated diving in the conscious Weddell seal
.
J. Appl. Physiol.
47
,
968
-
973
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information