Recent work has shown that the behaviour of marine organisms can be affected by elevated PCO2, although little is known about the effect of multiple stressors. We therefore investigated the effect of elevated PCO2 and temperature on locomotion and behaviour during prey searching in the marine gastropod Concholepas concholepas, a predator characteristic of the southeastern Pacific coast. Movement duration, decision time, route finding and lateralization were measured using a T-maze tank with a prey positioned behind a barrier. Four treatments, representing present day and near-future scenarios of ocean acidification and warming were used in rearing the individuals for 6 months. Regardless of the treatment, no significant differences were found in relative and absolute lateralization before and after exposure for 6 months. However, relative lateralization was not repeatable for animals tested after 6 months at elevated PCO2 at both experimental temperatures, whereas it was repeatable in individuals kept at the present day level of PCO2. We suggest that these effects may be related to a behavioural malfunction caused by elevated PCO2. Movement duration, decision time and route finding were not repeatable. However, movement duration and decision time increased and route finding decreased in elevated PCO2 (at 15°C), suggesting that elevated PCO2 has negative effects on the locomotor and sensory performance of C. concholepas in the presence of a prey odour, thereby decreasing their ability to forage efficiently.

Ocean acidification (OA) and global warming (GW) are issues of increasing concern (Doney, 2010) owing to their key role in global change. Although early studies have focused on calcifying organisms as the main species that may be affected by ocean acidification (Hofmann et al., 2010; Orr et al., 2005), more recent work has shown that the physiological and behavioural performances of marine fish and invertebrates may also be directly affected (Briffa et al., 2012; Byrne, 2011). The exposure to elevated PCO2 associated with OA could impair sensory performance, anti-predator responses, decision-making, foraging, learning and homing of many marine species (Briffa et al., 2012; Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016). In fish, the malfunctioning of behaviours resulting from high PCO2 exposure has been linked to a disruption of the function of the neurotransmitter GABA-A, (Nilsson et al., 2012). Other suggested mechanisms underlying the impact of acidification are a significant decrease in brain aerobic potential, as observed in sharks (Rosa et al., 2016). Moreover, the increase in seawater temperatures associated with GW is expected to impact many physiological processes and ecological interactions (Pörtner and Farrell, 2008), and to affect a number of behavioural traits (Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016). This suggests that the evaluation of the impact of global changes on the behaviour and physiology of marine organisms needs to consider multiple stressors (e.g. GW and OA). Understanding multiple stressor interactions is a key step for a better assessment of the ecological repercussions of climate change (Byrne and Przeslawski, 2013; Hale et al., 2011; Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016).

Elevated PCO2 and temperature have been shown to affect the movement behaviour and activity of various marine species (Briffa et al., 2012; Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016). Although marine fishes can be considered as a model group for OA because much of the research on the behavioural effects of OA has been carried out on them (Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016), some recent evidence has shown that elevated PCO2 levels can also affect the behaviour of some invertebrate species. For example, OA was shown to alter the antipredator behaviour of jumping snails (Watson et al., 2014), king scallops (Schalkhausser et al., 2013) and squid (Spady et al., 2014), the righting time and predator perception of the muricid gastropod Concholepas concholepas (Manríquez et al., 2013, 2014, 2016), the decision-making, locomotor activity and ability to locate an odour source in hermit crabs (de la Haye et al., 2011, 2012), and the swimming activity of the decapod Metapenaeus joyneri (Dissanayake and Ishimatsu, 2011). These behavioural changes may have far-reaching ecological consequences in a rapidly changing environment (Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016). Therefore, further studies on the effect of elevated PCO2 levels (along with other stressors such as temperature) on the movement behaviour of key species are fundamental in order to increase our predictive ability on the effect of climate change on marine ecosystems.

Lateralization, defined as the tendency to have a preferred side for turning, limb use or localization of function on one side of the body (Frasnelli et al., 2012; Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005) is one of the behavioural traits that was recently shown to be affected by both elevated PCO2 and temperature levels, in marine fishes (Domenici et al., 2014). Behavioural lateralization has been studied extensively in vertebrates and was shown to provide a number of advantages in cognitive tasks in general and specifically in collective behaviour, escaping and multitasking behaviours (Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005). Lateralization may also be a feature of relatively simple brains and indeed, recent evidence shows that behavioural asymmetries can occur in invertebrates, suggesting that lateralization of the nervous system can confer advantages at the individual level in invertebrates as well (Frasnelli, 2013; Frasnelli et al., 2012). This highlights the notion that important traits such as odour-liking and odour-aversion displacements in marine invertebrates such as mollusc gastropods can reflect the existence of asymmetries in the nervous system (Frasnelli, 2013; Frasnelli et al., 2012). Although recent studies have shown that the lateralization of various species of fish at the individual and population level is disrupted by exposure to elevated PCO2 levels (Domenici et al., 2012, 2014; Jutfelt et al., 2013) as well as by hypoxia and increased temperature (Domenici et al., 2014; Lucon-Xiccato et al., 2014), nothing is known about the effect of these environmental factors on lateralization in invertebrates.

The temporal stability of a phenotypic trait within individuals relative to differences in that trait among individuals is known as trait repeatability and it is an important characteristic that is receiving increasing attention (Bell et al., 2009; Killen et al., 2016). Most previous studies on repeatability have been carried out within the context of evolutionary processes in ecology. However, as argued by Killen et al. (2016), the extent to which environmental variables will affect trait repeatability (i.e. across-context repeatability) will play a fundamental role in the plastic responses of species to climate change. Previous work has shown that environmental factors can affect trait repeatability in both marine (Briffa and Greenaway, 2011) and terrestrial organisms (Brommer, 2013). However, little is known about how trait repeatability is affected by elevated PCO2 levels (Killen et al., 2016).

Here, we investigate the combined effect of long-term exposure to elevated PCO2 and temperature levels on locomotion and behaviour (including lateralization) of a marine gastropod, the Chilean abalone Concholepas concholepas (Bruguière 1789). This species is a keystone rocky shore predator characteristic of the southeastern Pacific Ocean coast (Castilla, 1999; Castilla and Paine, 1987). Individuals present a number of morphological asymmetries that might potentially affect lateralization. These include the position of the siphon that brings seawater with external signals to the osphradium and ctenidium both situated in the anterior left (anteriorly on the left side) (Huaquín, 1966; Huaquín and Garrido, 2000; Maldonado, 1965) and the shape of the shell with the umbo situated on the left. In C. concholepas, as in other gastropods, locomotion associated with predator–prey interactions involves bilateral pedal crawling by muscular contractions waves and the secretion of a mucous sheet (Denny, 1980). Previous studies based on Y-maze experiments have shown that juvenile C. concholepas use chemical cues originated from prey and predator to orientate their displacement (Manríquez et al., 2013). After long-term (5 months) exposure to elevated PCO2 levels, small juveniles of this species lost the ability to avoid a predator cue, but maintained their capacity to perceive cues originated from prey items (Manríquez et al., 2014). Although the sensory response of C. concholepas to a prey is unaffected by elevated PCO2 levels, it is not known if such levels affect the locomotion and behaviour (e.g. time to cover a given distance, route finding) associated with prey detection. Concholepas concholepas is an economically and ecologically important component of the rocky intertidal and subtidal communities along the Chilean coast (Castilla 1988, 1999). As a result, it is likely to be exposed to daily fluctuations of environmental conditions. Although this may make it relatively tolerant to short-term changes, its response to longer term changes would be more relevant for its potential response to climate change.

Ocean temperatures and PCO2 are predicted to increase in most regions of the world by the end of the year 2100 (IPCC, 2104; Meinshausen et al., 2011). The rise of atmospheric CO2 caused by human activity is the main source of the Earth's energy imbalance driving GW (von Schuckmann et al., 2016) and the increment of the CO2 flux from the atmosphere to the ocean driving OA (Doney et al., 2009). Regionally, the subtropical coast of western South America is expected to respond to GW in various ways. While GW may intensify the upwelling winds (Bakun, 1990; Garreaud and Falvey, 2009) and La Niña events (Cai et al., 2015) producing a regional cooling, more frequent El Niño-like conditions as a response to the increased concentration of greenhouse gases (Timmermann et al., 1999) will be characterized by extreme warm periods (Cai et al., 2014). During the strong El Niño event of 1997–1998, the coastal waters off northern Chile were anomalously warm (+3°C anomalies) and supersaturated in CO2 (Torres et al., 2003). We took into account this scenario to determine the treatments to test the potential response of C. concholepas to global change characterized by the intensification of extreme events (Easterling et al., 2000).

Using long-term exposure (6 months) we manipulated water temperature and PCO2 levels to determine their synergistic effects on various locomotor and behavioural traits (i.e. movement duration, decision time, route finding and lateralization) in juvenile C. concholepas in a T-maze tank with a prey item positioned behind a barrier at the end of a runway, as well as on the across-context repeatability of each trait. We hypothesized that the locomotion and behaviour of small juveniles of this species will be negatively affected after exposure to the combination of elevated PCO2 and temperature levels.

Animals and experimental treatments

Small juvenile C. concholepas (∼1.5 cm in length, approximately 3 months of age, benthic life) were collected in rocky intertidal platforms of southern Chile (39°45′51″S; 73°23′54″W) dominated by barnacle stands and the mussels Semimytilus algosus and Perumytilus purpuratus. The individuals were then transported to Coquimbo (northern-central Chile; 29°57′58″S; 71°21′17″W) where all the experiments were conducted. In the laboratory, 40 juveniles were reared in a Plexiglass aquarium (18×30×17 cm, high×length×width) with running seawater (1.5 litre h−1) and fed ad libitum with small live specimens of S. algosus for 1.5 months (i.e. 45 days). During this period, the aquarium was semi-immersed in a sea table with running seawater to maintain the temperature at ∼14.8±0.19°C and CO2 level at 506.68±16.90 µatm PCO2 (mean±s.e.). After this 1.5 month period, locomotor and behavioural traits were evaluated using a two-way T-maze runaway (Fig. S1). All individuals were tested in the same conditions at which they were kept during the initial rearing period [pH 7.79 at 25°C compared with in situ (i.e. actual) pH of 7.93]. Prior to the T-maze experiments, C. concholepas were deprived of food for 2 days in order to standardize hunger levels. After the behavioural observations in the T-maze, 10 individuals per group were haphazardly assigned to one of four treatments (see below). Individuals in each treatment show no significant difference in shell size at the beginning of the treatment period (one-way ANOVA, d.f.=3, F=0.33; P=0.80; mean shell length=1.55±0.042 cm; mean±s.e.).

The measurements and the behavioural observations in the T-maze were repeated in the same individuals after ∼6 months (treatment phase; 178 days) of acclimation at the following four conditions: (1) 15°C and present day 500 µatm PCO2 levels [15P (control), N=9]; (2) 15°C and future 1400 µatm PCO2 levels (15F, N=9); (3) 19°C and present day 500 µatm PCO2 levels (19P, N=8); and (4) 19°C and future 1400 µatm PCO2 (19F, N=10). A total of 36 individuals (9, 9, 10 and 8 per treatment, respectively) out of the initial 40 were tested after the 6 month period; 4 individuals were discarded because they had escaped from the rearing bottles. Treatment levels were nominal. Actual values based on seawater chemistry and temperature records are reported in Table 1. The low PCO2 level was chosen as representative of the current-day level in coastal areas near the collection site (Torres et al., 2013) whereas the high PCO2 level was chosen to represent conditions predicted under worst-case scenarios for the beginning of the next century (Meinshausen et al., 2011). Seawater chemistry analysis (Table 1) gives values of current day PCO2 used here, which are within the range of those found in nature by C. concholepas. Indeed, throughout the year, at the site of collection, C. concholepas experience temperatures in the range of 10–16°C with summer temperature ranging from 14 to16°C, and PCO2 ranging from 200 to 650 µatm (Torres et al., 2013). The control temperatures (15±0.5°C) were chosen to match the average high temperatures near the collection site during the austral summer months (Torres et al., 2013) while the elevated temperature (19°C) was chosen to approximate the temperature increase (+4°C) predicted for the beginning of the next century under scenario RCP8.5 (IPCC, 2014), although this level of warming (+4°C) is likely to occur sooner as a result of extreme events such as El Niño periods (Easterling et al., 2000). During the treatment phase, snails were individually maintained in 2 litre plastic bottles (each snail within its own bottle), sealed with a plastic lid (with a small hole to allow tube access) filled with 1.5 litre of 1 μm filtered seawater (FSW) conditioned at the required PCO2 level. The system in which the experimental individuals were reared was not recirculating. Only the water in the sea-tables in which the rearing bottles were semi-immersed was recirculated, meaning that any chemicals present in the sea-tables were never in contact with the seawater within each bottle. Conditioned FSW from the appropriate header tank (see below) was then used to replace the seawater of each bottle every second day.

Each individual was reared during the 6 months of treatment within its own bottle, thus providing a means of replication. However, the system did not include as many tanks with controlled temperature as the rearing bottles. Based on the terminology suggested by Cornwall and Hurd (2016), the system used had two mixing tanks associated with each experimental sea table (one set at 15°C and the other at 19°C). In these 200 litre tanks, a mix between the seawater and the corresponding gases took place. Every 48 h, the seawater in each individual tank (plastic bottles) was replaced using the seawater taken from the corresponding mixing tank. During the time between seawater exchanges, the required PCO2 in the bottles was maintained by the bubbling of the required gases as for the corresponding mixing tank. This experimental array created the four contrasting PCO2 treatments conditions (Table 1) with independent experimental units that corresponded to the individual plastic bottles. This design is a modified version of the designs described by Cornwall and Hurd (2016); all the bottles share a mixing tank but at the same time each tank has its own and continuous supply of pre-mixed gases. However, our experimental design also included temperature as a second experimental variable. This is a potential limitation of our methods, since it does not allow us to distinguish between the treatment effect (temperature) and a potential tank effect. At the end of the experiments all the surviving individuals were returned to the collection site.

Seawater acidification and carbonate system determination in the equilibrated seawater

Conditioned FSW with the two PCO2 levels was generated in four polyethylene (230 litre) header tanks. Each bottle was filled with conditioned FSW, using a tube inserted within each bottle, fitted with an air stone through which a continuous stream of either air (∼400 µatm CO2) or enriched CO2 air (∼1200 µatm CO2) was bubbled through the water. Enriched CO2 air was produced by blending air and pure CO2 using mass flow controllers (Aalborg, model GFC) (Torres et al., 2013). During the rearing phase, seawater parameters (i.e. pH, temperature, salinity and total alkalinity) were measured twice a week in seawater samples obtained from the rearing aquarium (Table 1). In the treatment phase, the parameters were measured twice a week in water samples taken from the bottles before the water change took place. In the rearing phase, these measurements represent the final carbonate speciation due the continuous stream of air and the cumulative effect on the carbonate system of respiration, calcification and ammonium excretion associated with the metabolism of both the snails and the food (S. algosus) between water samples in a running seawater system. However, in the treatment phase, these measurements represent the final carbonate speciation due the continuous stream of either air or enriched CO2 air and the cumulative effect on the carbonate system of the 48 h of respiration, calcification and ammonium excretion in a still-water system. The pH measurements were made in a closed 25 ml cell, thermostatically controlled at 25.0°C, with a Metrohm 713 pH meter (input resistance >1013 Ohm) and a glass fixed ground-joint diaphragm electrode with Pt1000 (Metrohm 6.0257.000, Aquatrode Plus) calibrated with standard Tris buffer in synthetic sea water.

Tris buffer was prepared at salinity 35, rigorously following the standard operating procedures outlined by Dickson and Goyet (1994). The pH of this buffer was calculated to be 8.089 pH units at 25.0°C and salinity 35 on the total hydrogen ion scale (Dickson and Goyet, 1994). We checked the accuracy of this buffer against pH certified Tris buffers (using hydrogen electrode system at 25°C) obtained from Andrew Dickson's laboratory (Nemzer and Dickson, 2005). We discarded any Tris buffer batch that differed from the Dickson Tris buffer by more than 0.003 pH units.

Total alkalinity (AT) was determined by potentiometric titration in an open cell, according to Haraldsson et al. (1997). The accuracy was controlled against a certified reference material supplied by Andrew Dickson (Scripps Institution of Oceanography). The correction factor was ≈1.002, corresponding to a difference of <5 µmol kg−1. Each sample was analysed using 2 or 3 replicates. Temperature and salinity were measured using a CTD (Ocean Seven 305). The pH, AT and hydrographic data were used to calculate the rest of the carbonate system parameters at in situ temperature (pH, PCO2, [CO3−2] and the saturation state of Ω aragonite and calcite), using CO2SYS software (Lewis et al., 1998) set with Mehrbach solubility constants (Mehrbach et al., 1973) refitted by Dickson and Millero (1987).

Shell length

Shell length was measured in all individuals before and after the 6 month treatment, to test whether PCO2 and/or temperature had an effect on growth, which could potentially modulate any effect on locomotion.

T-maze set-up and lateralization

The experimental apparatus was based on a design used previously to investigate behavioural lateralization in fish (Domenici et al., 2014). This consisted of a two-way T-maze runaway (Fig. S1) which allowed scoring of the turning direction (i.e. left or right) of each individual in 8 consecutive runs. To account for any possible asymmetry in the set-up, tests were carried out alternately on the two ends of the runway. The T-maze tank was made of black Plexiglass (60×29×10 cm, length×width×height) with a runaway in the middle section (25×4 cm, length×width). At both ends of the runaway and 5 cm ahead of the side walls, a black Plexiglass barrier (10×12.5 cm, width×height, 0.3 cm thick) was located. Forty prey items (the mussel Semimytilus algosus) were positioned inside a plastic cylindrical cage behind the barrier to attract C. concholepas (Manríquez et al., 2013), because elevated PCO2 is known to have no effect on prey detection in C. concholepas (Manríquez et al., 2014). The prey items were removed after each run, and a new group of prey items was positioned in the opposite end of the runway. Between consecutive runs (of the same individual) and also between individuals, the seawater in the T-mazes was replaced and the tank was carefully cleaned with tap water to remove any remains of previously formed mucous track left by C. concholepas that potentially could affect turning direction. The T-maze was filled with 5 litres of FSW to obtain a water depth of 3.5 cm. The seawater used during the detour test had the same PCO2 level and temperature as the holding tanks.

Turning preference (i.e. bias in left or right turns) was assessed using the relative lateralization index (LR, ranging from −100 to +100), indicating complete preference for left or right turning, respectively, using: [(turn to the right−turn to the left)/(turn to the right+turn to the left)]×100. The strength of lateralization (irrespective of its direction) was then assessed using the absolute lateralization index (LA), ranging from 0 (an individual that turned in equal proportion to the right and to the left) to 100 (an individual that turned right or left on all 8 trials) (Bisazza et al., 1998; Domenici et al., 2012).

At the beginning of the T-maze measurements, one single individual was positioned in the mid-point of the runaway (i.e. 12.5 cm from the end of the runway) with its head oriented toward the end of the runway bearing the prey for an adjustment period of 10 min using two PVC tube stoppers. After this 10 min period, the PVC tube stoppers were removed and the animals were allowed to move towards the barrier. To prevent disturbances, a semi-transparent piece of Plexiglass (35×14 cm, width×length) was used to cover the entire runaway. At the end of the runway and in front of the barrier, a decision zone was defined (i.e. a 4×5 cm area delimited by the barrier and the exit of the runway, Fig. S1). A mirror positioned above the tank was used to make all the observations. The turnings were considered left or right when the entire snail had crossed the left or right edge of the decision zone (Fig. S1). All the experimental trials in the detour test ended with C. concholepas successfully reaching the prey.

Locomotion and route finding

A set of locomotor and behavioural traits were scored while the snail was crawling through the runway. The total time spent by the snail to cover the distance from the midpoint of the runway (i.e. at the beginning of the trial) to the decision zone in the eight runs summed together, was defined as movement duration (tM), Therefore, tM is an index of the average speed (i.e. a low tM indicates a high speed) at which the snails crawled while covering the distance between the centre of the T-maze and the decision zone (Fig. S1). The total time (i.e. the sum of the time spent in the 8 runs) that each snail spent in the decision zone was defined as decision time (tD). Route finding (RF) was assessed by scoring the number of runs out of the total (i.e. as a proportion out of 8) within which the snail made no contact with the Plexiglass barrier at the end of the runaway before turning, but rather crawled directly around the barrier in order to reach the prey (see Fig. 1A-E). Hence RF ranges from 0 to 1 where 1 implies the highest route finding performance. Examples of route finding with and without contact with the Plexiglass barrier are shown in Fig. 1.

Statistics

Parametric statistics were used unless the distribution was not normal (based on D'Agostino–Pearson test for normality), in which case non-parametric tests or rank-transformations were used. For relative lateralization, a mean LR near zero indicates that a given sample of the population is neither left- nor right-biased in its turning tendency (Bisazza et al., 2000). Significant sample-level departures from unbiased turning (LR=0) were assessed by one-sample two-tailed t-tests performed on the mean values of LR (Bisazza et al., 2000; Zar, 1984).

The strength of lateralization (irrespective of its direction) at the individual level was assessed by comparing each treatment's distribution with the proportions of absolute lateralization expected from random choice based on a binomial distribution [with 8 repetitions and p (probability to turn left)=q (probability to turn right)=0.5] using a G-test.

Repeatability was tested between the trials before (first trial) and after treatment (second trial), using a Pearson's correlation (Spearman in the case of non-normal distribution). Repeatable variables were defined as those with a significant positive correlation between the first and second trial (Killen et al., 2016; Kluen and Brommer, 2013). Lateralization, tD, tM and RF before and after treatment were compared using a paired-t-test (Wilcoxon test for non-normal distribution). Two-way ANOVAs, followed by a post hoc test (Tukey test) were used to compare the variables before and after treatment. A rank-transformation using aligned rank transform (Wobbrock et al., 2011) was used if the distribution was not normal.

Shell length

Mean values of shell length after 6 months of treatment were: 15P (control), 3.55±0.09; 15F, 3.42±0.20; 19P, 3.94±0.05; and 19F, 3.73±0.08 cm. Shell length of all samples was not different from a normal distribution (D'Agostino test, P>0.05 in all cases). Two-way ANOVA comparing individuals after 6 months of treatment, showed that temperature had a positive effect on shell length (d.f.=1, F=7.9, P<0.01), while PCO2 had no effect (d.f.=1, F=1.80, P=0.18) and the interaction was also not significant (d.f.=1, F=0.07, P=0.79). Post hoc comparisons (Tukey test) showed significant differences between 15°C and 19°C groups at present day PCO2 levels (15P vs 19P: P<0.05); however, comparisons between all other variables showed no significant differences.

Relative lateralization (LR)

LR of all samples was not different from a normal distribution (D'Agostino test, P>0.05 in all cases). For each treatment, LR data did not differ from the theoretical mean value of zero (Bisazza et al., 2000) (P>0.05 in all cases), implying that none of the samples showed a turning bias at the population level. No difference between the LR before and after the 6 month treatment was found for 15P (P=0.59, t8=0.55), 15F (P=0.49, t8=0.72), 19P (P=0.10, t7=1.87) or 19F (P=0.24, t9=1.24).

LR was repeatable for 15P (Pearson r=0.98, P<0.0001) and 19P (Pearson's r=0.96, P<0.001), but not for 15F (Pearson's r=0.08, P>0.5) or 19F (Pearson's r=0.11, P>0.5), suggesting that any turning bias at the individual level was maintained in control PCO2 levels regardless of temperature, whereas it was lost after individuals were exposed to elevated PCO2 levels for 6 months (Fig. 2). Two-way ANOVA comparing individuals after 6 months of treatment, showed that relative lateralization was not affected by temperature (d.f.=1, F=0.17, P=0.68) nor by PCO2 level (d.f.=1, F=0.75, P=0.39) and no interaction effect was found (d.f.=1, F=0.002, P=0.96) (Fig. 3A).

Absolute lateralization (LA)

LA of all samples were not different from a normal distribution (D'Agostino test, P>0.05 in all cases). As for LR, no significant differences in the absolute lateralization before and after treatment was found in all cases; for 15P (P=0.59, t8=0.55), 15F (P=0.56, t8=0.61), 19P (P=0.10, t7=1.87) and 19F (P=0.26, t9=1.20). Overall, in 46% of the trails, snails turned in a consistent direction in ≥7 out of 8 runs. Therefore, LA was relatively high in all cases (ranging from 43.7 to 61.1) since individuals had the tendency to turn in the same direction in successive runs. Accordingly, snails of all groups (before and after treatment) exhibited a significantly higher preference for making either left or right turns than expected by chance (G-test: P<0.001 in all cases, d.f.=4), except for 19F before the 6 month treatment (P=0.055).

Absolute lateralization was repeatable for 15P (Pearson's r=0.9058, P<0.001), 15F (Pearson's r=0.69, P=0.04), borderline significant for 19P (Pearson's r=0.70, P=0.05), but not significant for 19F (Pearson's r=−0.2450, P=0.49). The measurements of LA for the present day conditions (15P and 19P) reflect those of LR for the present day PCO2 conditions. For 15F, significant repeatability for LA and not for LR implies that individuals tend to maintain their tendency to turn in a given direction, although the side of the turning bias was not necessarily the same as before the treatment. Two-way ANOVA for absolute lateralization showed no effect of PCO2 (d.f.=1, F=0.003, P=0.96) nor of temperature (d.f.=1, F=0.015, P=0.90), and the interaction effect was not significant (d.f.=1, F=0.12, P=0.73) (Fig. 3B).

Locomotion

Both movement duration (tM) and decision time (tD ) were not different from a normal distribution (D'Agostino test, P>0.05 in all cases). No differences in the tM and tD  before and after treatment were found in 15P (tM, P=0.10, t8=0.45; tD, P=0.10, t8=1.87) and 19P (tM, P=0.06, t7=2.19; tD, P=0.92, t7=0.10). However, both were significantly different before and after treatment in 15F (tM, P=0.013, t8=3.150; tD, P<0.001, t8=5.46) and in 19F (tM, P=0.02, t9=2.809; tD, P=0.027, t9=2.64). Therefore, in the control PCO2 level, locomotor traits did not change at the sample level after a 6 month period, whereas exposure to an elevated PCO2 level increased both movement duration and decision time (i.e. decreased speed of locomotion). None of the locomotor traits were repeatable; therefore, individuals did not retain these performance values in the long term.

Two-way ANOVA showed that PCO2 had a significant effect on tM (d.f.=1, F= 7.57, P=0.01), while temperature had no effect (d.f.=1, F=1.56, P=0.22) and the interaction was also not significant (d.f.=1, F=0.75, P=0.39). Post hoc comparisons for tM (Tukey test) showed that 19P differed from 19F (P=0.016), while all other comparisons were not significant (Fig. 4A). Two-way ANOVA shows that PCO2 had a significant effect on tD (d.f.=1, F=34.32, P<0.001), while temperature had no effect (d.f.=1, F=1.29, P=0.26) and the interaction was also not significant (d.f.=1, F=0.60, P=0.44). Post hoc comparisons for tD (Tukey test) showed significant differences between 15P and 15F (P<0.001), and 19P and 19F (P=0.001), while all other comparisons were not significant (Fig. 4B).

Route finding

Route finding data were not different from a normal distribution in all groups (D'Agostino test, P>0.05 in all cases), except for 15P (after-treatment trial, P=0.002). RF was found to be significantly different before and after treatment in 15F (P<0.001, t8=5.96), while all other comparisons yielded non-significant results: 15P (P=0.59, Wilcoxon W=−7, d.f.=8), 19P (P=0.89, t7=0.14) and 19F (P=0.56, t9=0.60). RF was not repeatable in any of the four treatments (Pearson test in all cases, except 15P, for which the Spearman test was used).

Two-way ANOVA on aligned ranked data (Wobbrock et al., 2011) showed that RF was significantly affected by PCO2 level (d.f.=1, F=4.88, P=0.03) as well as by temperature (d.f.=1, F=5.96, P=0.02), and their interaction was significant (d.f.=1, F=18.38, P<0.001). Post hoc comparisons (Tukey test) within each temperature, showed significant differences between 15P and 15F (P<0.001) but not between 19P and 19F (P=0.54). Within each PCO2 level, post hoc comparisons showed differences between 15F and 19F (P<0.01) but not between 15P and 19P (P=0.2) (Fig. 4C).

The results show that a number of locomotor and behavioural traits of C. concholepas are affected by long-term exposure to elevated PCO2, while temperature has a modulating effect only in some cases. This is in line with previous work on the effect on multiple stressors on the lateralization behaviour of fish (Domenici et al., 2014). C. concholepas is an intertidal and subtidal species and therefore may experience a relatively wide range of temperatures; hence, it may be quite tolerant to elevated temperatures. Repeatability is affected by elevated PCO2 only for relative lateralization (Fig. 2). However, no effects were detected on relative lateralization at the sample level, as mean relative lateralization appears to be unaffected by the treatments (Fig. 3A).

Interestingly, at the sample level, snails do not exhibit a significant turning bias in any of the treatments, although they show a non-significant tendency to turn left (Fig. 3A). Although previous work on the same species has demonstrated a side-bias in righting behaviour associated with morphological asymmetries, i.e. the projection of the umbo to the left in all individuals impedes righting in that direction (Manríquez et al., 2013), C. concholepas in captivity were observed to be able to turn in any direction if a barrier is positioned between them and a prey item (P.H.M., personal observations). In line with these observations, no directional bias in turning was shown by the present results. Long-term exposure to elevated PCO2 did not have an effect on the lack of turning bias at the population level. The absence of a turning bias during prey searching at the population level might be advantageous by randomizing the movements and therefore the distributions of the individuals.

Although individuals of C. concholepas do not show a significant directional bias at the population level, they are significantly lateralized at the individual level: individuals tend to turn either left or right in most runs, more often than would be expected by random choice. This strong individual-level bias is in line with their significant tendency to retain their lateralization level in the long term (6 months) when tested in control water (i.e. significant repeatability, Fig. 2). However, elevated CO2 interferes with this tendency, and snails tested after 6 months in elevated PCO2, lost (not reversed) their individual turning bias (hence individuals that had a high proportion of right turns in the first trial at control levels, lost this patterns after 6 months in elevated PCO2 when they may show higher proportion of left or right turns, see Fig. 2). As in previous works on fish and molluscs, it is possible that this behavioural effect caused by elevated PCO2 levels is related to interference with the neurotransmitter function (Nilsson et al., 2012; Watson et al., 2014). Previous work on fish (Domenici et al., 2012) showed that absolute lateralization was decreased by short-term exposure (4 days) to elevated PCO2 levels and suggested that such loss of lateralization may increase vulnerability, given that lateralization enhances performance in a number of cognitive tasks and anti-predator behaviours (Vallortigara and Rogers, 2005). Here, absolute lateralization was not affected by exposure to elevated PCO2. Therefore, it could be argued that elevated PCO2 levels will not affect the vulnerability of this species through lateralization effects. However, the effect of elevated PCO2 levels on across-context repeatability might have repercussions on the fitness of individuals if the control-level lateralization was optimized for each specific individual.

Neither movement duration nor decision time were repeatable traits. However, PCO2 level (but not temperature) had a significant effect on both traits. Movement duration was affected by elevated PCO2 only at 19°C, which could, in part, be explained by the higher activity (shorter movement duration) likely induced by high temperature at the control PCO2 level, although the effect of temperature on movement duration was not significant. Generally speaking, high PCO2 levels appear to slow down the snails, which could be related to negative effects on both locomotion and sensory performance. Elevated PCO2 levels appear to have an even stronger effect on decision time, since the latter is affected at both temperatures. This may be due to the additive negative effect on various potential components of decision time such as cognitive, sensory and locomotor performance as found in other organisms (Briffa et al., 2012). It is unlikely that the effect of PCO2 on locomotion is related to the effect of treatment on shell length, since the latter is not affected by PCO2. However, despite the fact that temperature affects shell length (with higher temperature resulting in larger shells), temperature did not affect tM or tD. It is therefore possible that the effect of temperature on shell length is not sufficient to cause differences in displacement speed. The larger size of individuals kept at higher temperature suggests that growth in C. concholepas increases even with temperatures (i.e. 19°C) that are higher than the maximum typically recorded in their natural habitat (15–16°C) (Navarro and Torrijos, 1994; Torres et al., 2013), suggesting that C. concholepas has tolerance for a wide range of temperatures. Previous work has shown that the scope for growth and net growth efficiency in C. concholepas were higher during the warm seasons (summer and spring), compared with the winter and the fall (Navarro and Torrijos, 1994). Our results suggest that this trend to increase growth with temperature continues up to 19°C.

All the experimental trials in the detour test ended with C. concholepas reaching the prey successfully. This suggests that the ability to eventually locate the prey was not affected by the experimental conditions as found by a previous study (Manríquez et al., 2014). However, the relatively high proportion of trials in which the snail made contact with the barrier in elevated PCO2 levels at 15°C suggests a negative effect of elevated PCO2 on perception of the barrier, or more simply, a decreased ability to follow the most direct path towards the prey scent coming from either sides of the barrier. Furthermore, although little is known about the visual performance in gastropods, most gastropod species have some capacity for using their eyes to keep on a straight course, and a few have a very basic capacity for pattern recognition and landmark navigation (Land and Nilsson, 2006). The negative effect of elevated PCO2 levels on eyesight through effects on the retinal reaction has been reported in fish (Chung et al., 2014). Thus, it is possible that near-future PCO2 levels might impair the capacity of other marine organisms such as molluscs to respond visually not only to fast events such as predator attacks (Allan et al., 2013), but also to take the most direct route towards a prey without contact with a barrier.

Furthermore, it is possible that other senses (in addition to, or instead of, vision) that may be potentially involved in minimizing the path length to reach the prey, were negatively affected by the elevated PCO2 in our experiments. These may include mechanical reception of the barrier as well as chemoreception of the precise direction of prey odour from either sides of the barrier. Chemoreception in gastropods is mediated by the osphradium, an external sensory organ containing neuro-epithelial cells which monitor the physiochemical properties of the surrounding seawater (Karnik et al., 2012). Although previous work shows that predator, but not prey, odour detection is affected by elevated PCO2 in C. concholepas (Manríquez et al., 2014), it is possible that elevated PCO2 may decrease the chemosensory performance without eliminating it completely. This would explain why C. concholepas does eventually reach the prey in all the treatments used here, although its performance in elevated PCO2 is low in terms of time to reach the prey and route finding.

Temperature modulated the effect of PCO2 levels on route finding, since elevated PCO2 did not affect RF at 19°C. Furthermore, at 19°C, RF was higher at elevated PCO2 than in the control, suggesting that the effects of elevated PCO2 levels on the route finding of C. concholepas are complex and may be compensated by elevated temperatures. Elevated temperatures were found to mitigate the effects of elevated PCO2 in various marine organisms, for example in sea urchins (Byrne et al., 2013) and in fish (Domenici et al., 2014). Route finding was not repeatable, implying that the ability to reach the most direct route towards the prey is not temporally stable in the long-term, unlike lateralization.

Conclusions and perspectives

This study is the first to investigate the potential impacts of ocean acidification on lateralization and route finding in a predatory gastropod and to show that elevated PCO2 can affect the across-context repeatability of behavioural performance in marine organisms. During the early benthic ontogeny, C. concholepas can be found in rocky intertidal habitats (Guisado and Castilla, 1983; Moreno et al., 1993), where it naturally experiences daily fluctuations in pH, PCO2, total alkalinity and temperature (Torres et al., 2013, 1999) and therefore it may have evolved mechanisms to partially cope with these environmental stressors. Although C. concholepas may be quite tolerant to daily fluctuations in environmental factors, our results showed that it is affected by long-term and chronic exposure to increased PCO2 and this effect is partly modulated by temperature. Long-term exposure to high PCO2 and temperature that are in line with those expected at the end of the century is arguably relevant for what might be the response of C. concholepas to climate change, as it has been suggested for other organisms (Melzner et al., 2009). Future work may also consider the additional effect of parental exposure (Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016). Previous work has shown that elevated PCO2 levels negatively affect their capacity to recognize water-borne predator cues (Manríquez et al., 2014), and the present study suggests that near-future PCO2 levels could have further consequences for the locomotion and sensory performance of C. concholepas and other similar species when searching for prey. The direct effect of temperature was not significant, although temperature did show a modulating effect. The present study suggests that increased PCO2 levels rather than temperature maybe the main stressor driving behavioural modifications in C. concholepas along the Chilean coasts and this may therefore, at least potentially, affect species interactions and ecological processes (Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016).

Further studies are required to determine the effects of the decreased locomotion performance and route finding on the success of this keystone species in the field and the impact this has on their interactions with prey and predator populations. The marine gastropod C. concholepas is a keystone predator species characteristic of rocky intertidal and subtidal communities of the southeastern Pacific Ocean coast (Castilla, 1999; Castilla and Paine, 1987). Since in this system the presence or absence of this species drives the community structure (Castilla, 1999), the negative effects of ocean acidification on locomotion traits associated with prey-finding may cause cascading effects beyond those described at the individual level such as predator–prey population dynamics and community structure. Furthermore, our results on the effect of elevated PCO2 levels on the repeatability of relative lateralization open up the question as to whether or not other behavioural traits affected by elevated PCO2 in various marine organisms (Briffa et al., 2012; Leduc et al., 2013; Nagelkerken and Munday, 2016) may also lose their repeatability after exposure to elevated PCO2. As advocated by Killen et al., (2016), our current lack of knowledge regarding how trait repeatability is affected by elevated PCO2 levels is a crucial factor limiting our ability to predict how aquatic organisms will evolve in response to ocean acidification.

María Elisa Jara is thanked for her valuable help during the collection of both the experimental individuals and the prey items. Mylene Seguel and Emilio Alarcon are acknowledged for their assistance during the lateralization trials and determination of the seawater parameters, respectively. Matthew Lee provided precious help in the data analysis.

Author contributions

P.D. and P.H.M conceived and designed the study. P.H.M. performed the behavioural experiments. R.T. analysed the water samples contributed with reagents, materials and analysis equipment. P.D. analysed the data. P.D. drafted the manuscript, P.H.M and R.T. contributed to revisions.

Funding

This work was supported by the Fondo Nacional de Desarrollo Científico y Tecnológico (1130839) to P.H.M and R.T. This grant also supported international collaboration by paying the travel expenses of P.H.M to Torregrande (Oristano), where most of the results were analysed. P.H.M. acknowledges Celulosa Arauco and Constitución S.A. for economic support that allowed equipment acquisition, paid for research space in the Universidad Católica del Norte (UCN, Coquimbo) and supported field work at Valdivia.

Allan
,
B. J. M.
,
Domenici
,
P.
,
McCormick
,
M. I.
,
Watson
,
S. A.
and
Munday
,
P. L.
(
2013
).
Elevated CO2 affects predator-prey interactions through altered performance
.
PLoS ONE
8
,
e58520
.
Bakun
,
A.
(
1990
).
Global climate change and intensification of coastal ocean upwelling
.
Science
247
,
198
.
Bell
,
A. M.
,
Hankison
,
S. J.
and
Laskowski
,
K. L.
(
2009
).
The repeatability of behaviour: a meta-analysis
.
Anim. Behav.
77
,
771
-
783
.
Bisazza
,
A.
,
Facchin
,
L.
,
Pignatti
,
R.
and
Vallortigara
,
G.
(
1998
).
Lateralization of detour behaviour in poeciliid fish: The effect of species, gender and sexual motivation
.
Behav. Brain Res.
91
,
157
-
164
.
Bisazza
,
A.
,
Cantalupo
,
C.
,
Capocchiano
,
M.
and
Vallortigara
,
G.
(
2000
).
Population lateralisation and social behaviour: a study with 16 species of fish
.
Laterality
5
,
269
-
284
.
Briffa
,
M.
and
Greenaway
,
J.
(
2011
).
High in situ repeatability of behaviour indicates animal personality in the beadlet anemone Actinia equina (Cnidaria)
.
PLoS ONE
6
,
e21963
.
Briffa
,
M.
,
de la Haye
,
K.
and
Munday
,
P. L.
(
2012
).
High CO2 and marine animal behaviour: potential mechanisms and ecological consequences
.
Mar. Pollut. Bull.
64
,
1519
-
1528
.
Brommer
,
J. E.
(
2013
).
Variation in plasticity of personality traits implies that the ranking of personality measures changes between environmental contexts: calculating the cross-environmental correlation
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
67
,
1709
-
1718
.
Byrne
,
M.
(
2011
).
Impact of ocean warming and ocean acidification on marine invertebrate life history stages: Vulnerabilities and potential for persistence in a changing ocean
. In
Oceanography and Marine Biology: An Annual Review
, Vol.
49
(ed.
R. N.
Gibson
,
R. J. A.
Atkinson
and
J. D. M.
Gordon
), pp.
1
-
42
.
Boca Raton
:
Crc Press-Taylor & Francis Group
.
Byrne
,
M.
and
Przeslawski
,
R.
(
2013
).
Multistressor impacts of warming and acidification of the ocean on marine invertebrates’ life histories
.
Integr. Comp. Biol.
53
,
582
-
596
.
Byrne
,
M.
,
Foo
,
S.
,
Soars
,
N. A.
,
Wolfe
,
K. D. L.
,
Nguyen
,
H. D.
,
Hardy
,
N.
and
Dworjanyn
,
S. A.
(
2013
).
Ocean warming will mitigate the effects of acidification on calcifying sea urchin larvae (Heliocidaris tuberculata) from the Australian global warming hot spot
.
J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol.
448
,
250
-
257
.
Cai
,
W.
,
Borlace
,
S.
,
Lengaigne
,
M.
,
van Rensch
,
P.
,
Collins
,
M.
,
Vecchi
,
G.
,
Timmermann
,
A.
,
Santoso
,
A.
,
McPhaden
,
M. J.
,
Wu
,
L.
, et al. 
(
2014
).
Increasing frequency of extreme El Niño events due to greenhouse warming
.
Nat. Clim. Change
4
,
111
-
116
.
Cai
,
W.
,
Santoso
,
A.
,
Wang
,
G.
,
Yeh
,
S.-W.
,
An
,
S.-I.
,
Cobb
,
K. M.
,
Collins
,
M.
,
Guilyardi
,
E.
,
Jin
,
F.-F.
,
Kug
,
J.-S.
, et al. 
(
2015
).
ENSO and greenhouse warming
.
Nat. Clim. Change
5
,
849
-
859
.
Castilla
,
J. C.
(
1988
).
Una revisión bibliográfica (1980-1988) sobre Concholepas concholepas (Gastropoda, Muricidae): problemas pesqueros y experiencias de repoblación
.
Biol. Pesq.
17
,
9
-
19
.
Castilla
,
J. C.
(
1999
).
Coastal marine communities: trends and perspectives from human-exclusion experiments
.
Trends Ecol. Evol.
14
,
280
-
283
.
Castilla
,
J. C.
and
Paine
,
R. T.
(
1987
).
Predation and community organization on Eastern Pacific temperate zone rocky intertidal shores
.
Rev. Chil. Hist. Nat.
60
,
131
-
152
.
Chung
,
W.-S.
,
Marshall
,
N. J.
,
Watson
,
S.-A.
,
Munday
,
P. L.
and
Nilsson
,
G. E.
(
2014
).
Ocean acidification slows retinal function in a damselfish through interference with GABA(A) receptors
.
J. Exp. Biol.
217
,
323
-
326
.
Cornwall
,
C. E.
and
Hurd
,
C. L.
(
2016
).
Experimental design in ocean acidification research: problems and solutions
.
ICES J. Mar. Sci.
73
,
572
-
581
.
de la Haye
,
K. L.
,
Spicer
,
J. I.
,
Widdicombe
,
S.
and
Briffa
,
M.
(
2011
).
Reduced sea water pH disrupts resource assessment and decision making in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus
.
Anim. Behav.
82
,
495
-
501
.
de la Haye
,
K. L.
,
Spicer
,
J. I.
,
Widdicombe
,
S.
and
Briffa
,
M.
(
2012
).
Reduced pH sea water disrupts chemo-responsive behaviour in an intertidal crustacean
.
J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol.
412
,
134
-
140
.
Denny
,
M.
(
1980
).
Locomotion: the cost of gastropod crawling
.
Science (Wash. D. C.)
208
,
1288
-
1290
.
Dickson
,
A. G.
and
Goyet
,
C.
(
1994
).
Handbook of Methods for the Analysis of the Various Parameters of the Carbon Dioxide System in Sea Water. Version 2
TN, US
:
Oak Ridge National Lab
.
Dickson
,
A. G.
and
Millero
,
F. J.
(
1987
).
A comparison of the equilibrium-constants for the dissociation of carbonic-acid in seawater media
.
Deep-Sea Res. Part A Oceanogr. Res. Pap.
34
,
1733
-
1743
.
Dissanayake
,
A.
and
Ishimatsu
,
A.
(
2011
).
Synergistic effects of elevated CO2 and temperature on the metabolic scope and activity in a shallow-water coastal decapod (Metapenaeus joyneri; Crustacea: Penaeidae)
.
ICES J. Mar. Sci.
68
,
1147
-
1154
.
Domenici
,
P.
,
Allan
,
B.
,
McCormick
,
M.
and
Munday
,
P. L.
(
2012
).
Elevated carbon dioxide affects behavioural lateralization in a coral reef fish
.
Biol. Lett.
8
,
78
-
81
.
Domenici
,
P.
,
Allan
,
B. J. M.
,
Watson
,
S. A.
,
McCormick
,
M. I.
and
Munday
,
P. L.
(
2014
).
Shifting from right to left: the combined effect of elevated CO2 and temperature on behavioural lateralization in a coral reef fish
.
PLoS ONE
9
,
e87969
.
Doney
,
S. C.
(
2010
).
The growing human footprint on coastal and open-ocean biogeochemistry
.
Science
328
,
1512
-
1516
.
Doney
,
S. C.
,
Fabry
,
V. J.
,
Feely
,
R. A.
and
Kleypas
,
J. A.
(
2009
).
Ocean acidification: the other CO2 problem
.
Ann. Rev. Mar. Sci.
1
,
169
-
192
.
Easterling
,
D. R.
,
Meehl
,
G. A.
,
Parmesan
,
C.
,
Changnon
,
S. A.
,
Karl
,
T. R.
and
Mearns
,
L. O.
(
2000
).
Climate extremes: observations, modeling, and impacts
.
Science
289
,
2068
.
Frasnelli
,
E.
(
2013
).
Brain and behavioral lateralization in invertebrates
.
Front. Psychol
.
4
,
939
.
Frasnelli
,
E.
,
Vallortigara
,
G.
and
Rogers
,
L. J.
(
2012
).
Left-right asymmetries of behaviour and nervous system in invertebrates
.
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.
36
,
1273
-
1291
.
Garreaud
,
R. D.
and
Falvey
,
M.
(
2009
).
The coastal winds off western subtropical South America in future climate scenarios
.
Int. J. Climatol.
29
,
543
-
554
.
Guisado
,
C.
and
Castilla
,
J. C.
(
1983
).
Aspects of the ecology and growth of an intertidal juvenile population of Concholepas concholepas (Mollusca: Gasteropoda: Muricidae) at Las Cruces Chile
.
Mar. Biol. (Berlin)
78
,
99
-
104
.
Hale
,
R.
,
Calosi
,
P.
,
McNeill
,
L.
,
Mieszkowska
,
N.
and
Widdicombe
,
S.
(
2011
).
Predicted levels of future ocean acidification and temperature rise could alter community structure and biodiversity in marine benthic communities
.
Oikos
120
,
661
-
674
.
Haraldsson
,
C.
,
Anderson
,
L. G.
,
Hassellöv
,
M.
,
Hulth
,
S.
and
Olsson
,
K.
(
1997
).
Rapid, high-precision potentiometric titration of alkalinity in ocean and sediment pore waters
.
Deep-Sea Res. Part I-Oceanogr. Res. Pap.
44
,
2031
-
2044
.
Hofmann
,
G. E.
,
Barry
,
J. P.
,
Edmunds
,
P. J.
,
Gates
,
R. D.
,
Hutchins
,
D. A.
,
Klinger
,
T.
and
Sewell
,
M. A.
(
2010
).
The effect of ocean acidification on calcifying organisms in marine ecosystems: an organism-to-ecosystem perspective
. In
Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics
, Vol.
41
(ed.
D. J.
Futuyma
,
H. B.
Shafer
and
D.
Simberloff
), pp.
127
-
147
.
Palo Alto
:
Annual Reviews
.
Huaquín
,
G.
(
1966
).
Anatomía de Concholepas concholepas (Bruguière), 1789 (Gastropoda: Muricidae)
. In
Escuela de Pedagogía
.
Memoria de Título
,
Santiago de Chile
,
Universidad Católica de Chile
.
Huaquín
,
G.
and
Garrido
,
J.
(
2000
).
Morphology and discussion of the possible role of the osphradium in Concholepas concholepas (Bruguière 1789) (Neogastropoda: Muricidae)
.
J. Med. Appl. Malacol.
10
,
145
-
155
.
IPCC
(
2014
).
Climate change 2014: synthesis report
. In
Contribution of Working Groups I, II and III to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change
(ed.
Core Writing Team
,
R.K.
Pachauri
and
L.A.
Meyer
), pp.
151
.
Geneva
,
Switzerland
:
IPCC
.
Jutfelt
,
F.
,
de Souza
,
K. B.
,
Vuylsteke
,
A.
and
Sturve
,
J.
(
2013
).
Behavioural disturbances in a temperate fish exposed to sustained high-CO2 levels
.
PLoS ONE
8
,
e65825
.
Karnik
,
V.
,
Braun
,
M.
,
Dalesman
,
S.
and
Lukowiak
,
K.
(
2012
).
Sensory input from the osphradium modulates the response to memory-enhancing stressors in Lymnaea stagnalis
.
J. Exp. Biol.
215
,
536
-
542
.
Killen
,
S. S.
,
Adriaenssens
,
B.
,
Marras
,
S.
,
Claireaux
,
G.
and
Cooke
,
S. J.
(
2016
).
Context dependency of trait repeatability and its relevance for management and conservation of fish populations
.
Conservation Physiology
4
,
cow007
.
Kluen
,
E.
and
Brommer
,
J. E.
(
2013
).
Context-specific repeatability of personality traits in a wild bird: a reaction-norm perspective
.
Behav. Ecol.
24
,
650
-
658
.
Land
,
M.
and
Nilsson
,
D.-E.
(
2006
).
General purpose and special purpose visual systems
. In
Invertebrate Vision
(ed.
E.
Warrant
and
D.-E.
Nilsson
), pp.
167
-
210
.
Cambridge
:
Cambridge University Press
.
Leduc
,
A. O. H. C.
,
Munday
,
P. L.
,
Brown
,
G. E.
and
Ferrari
,
M. C. O.
(
2013
).
Effects of acidification on olfactory-mediated behaviour in freshwater and marine ecosystems: a synthesis
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
368
,
20120447
.
Lewis
,
E.
,
Wallace
,
D.
and
Allison
,
L. J.
(
1998
).
Program developed for CO2 system calculations: Carbon Dioxide Information Analysis Center, managed by Lockheed Martin Energy Research Corporation for the US Department of Energy Tennessee. http://cdiac.ornl.gov/ftp/co2sys/CO2SYS_calc_DOS_v1.05/cdiac105.pdf
.
Lucon-Xiccato
,
T.
,
Nati
,
J. J. H.
,
Blasco
,
F. R.
,
Johansen
,
J. L.
,
Steffensen
,
J. F.
and
Domenici
,
P.
(
2014
).
Severe hypoxia impairs lateralization in a marine teleost fish
.
J. Exp. Biol.
217
,
4115
-
4118
.
Maldonado
,
R.
(
1965
).
Estudio macroscópico, microscópico e histoquímico de “Concholepas concholepas”
.
Rev. Biol. Mar.
12
,
121
-
127
.
Manríquez
,
P. H.
,
Jara
,
M. E.
,
Mardones
,
M. L.
,
Navarro
,
J. M.
,
Torres
,
R.
,
Lardies
,
M. A.
,
Vargas
,
C. A.
,
Duarte
,
C.
,
Widdicombe
,
S.
,
Salisbury
,
J.
, et al. 
(
2013
).
Ocean acidification disrupts prey responses to predator cues but not net prey shell growth in Concholepas concholepas (loco)
.
PLoS ONE
8
,
e68643
.
Manríquez
,
P. H.
,
Jara
,
M. E.
,
Mardones
,
M. L.
,
Torres
,
R.
,
Navarro
,
J. M.
,
Lardies
,
M. A.
,
Vargas
,
C. A.
,
Duarte
,
C.
and
Lagos
,
N. A.
(
2014
).
Ocean acidification affects predator avoidance behaviour but not prey detection in the early ontogeny of a keystone species
.
Mar. Ecol.-Prog. Ser.
502
,
157
-
167
.
Manríquez
,
P. H.
,
Jara
,
M. E.
,
Seguel
,
M. E.
,
Torres
,
R.
,
Alarcon
,
E.
and
Lee
,
M. R.
(
2016
).
Ocean acidification and increased temperature have both positive and negative effects on early ontogenetic traits of a rocky shore keystone predator species
.
PLoS ONE
11
,
e0151920
.
Mehrbach
,
C.
,
Culberso
,
C. H.
,
Hawley
,
J. E.
and
Pytkowic
,
R. M.
(
1973
).
Measurement of the apparent dissociation-constants of carbonic acid in seawater at atmospheric pressure
.
Limnol. Oceanogr.
18
,
897
-
907
.
Meinshausen
,
M.
,
Smith
,
S. J.
,
Calvin
,
K.
,
Daniel
,
J. S.
,
Kainuma
,
M. L. T.
,
Lamarque
,
J.-F.
,
Matsumoto
,
K.
,
Montzka
,
S. A.
,
Raper
,
S. C. B.
,
Riahi
,
K.
, et al. 
(
2011
).
The RCP greenhouse gas concentrations and their extensions from 1765 to 2300
.
Clim. Change
109
,
213
-
241
.
Melzner
,
F.
,
Gutowska
,
M. A.
,
Langenbuch
,
M.
,
Dupont
,
S.
,
Lucassen
,
M.
,
Thorndyke
,
M. C.
,
Bleich
,
M.
and
Pörtner
,
H.-O.
(
2009
).
Physiological basis for high CO2 tolerance in marine ectothermic animals: pre-adaptation through lifestyle and ontogeny?
.
Biogeosciences
6
,
2313
-
2331
.
Moreno
,
C. A.
,
Reyes
,
A.
and
Asencio
,
G.
(
1993
).
Habitat and movements of the recruits of Concholepas concholepas (Mollusca, Muricidae) in the rocky intertidal of Southern Chile
.
J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol.
171
,
51
-
61
.
Nagelkerken
,
I.
and
Munday
,
P. L.
(
2016
).
Animal behaviour shapes the ecological effects of ocean acidification and warming: moving from individual to community-level responses
.
Glob. Change Biol.
22
,
974
-
989
.
Navarro
,
J. M.
and
Torrijos
,
R.
(
1994
).
Seasonal variation in oxygen uptake and ammonia excretion in the predatory gastropod Concholepas concholepas (Bruguiere, 1789)
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A Physiol.
108
,
39
-
46
.
Nemzer
,
B. V.
and
Dickson
,
A. G.
(
2005
).
The stability and reproducibility of Tris buffers in synthetic seawater
.
Mar. Chem.
96
,
237
-
242
.
Nilsson
,
G. E.
,
Dixson
,
D. L.
,
Domenici
,
P.
,
McCormick
,
M. I.
,
Sørensen
,
C.
,
Watson
,
S.-A.
and
Munday
,
P. L.
(
2012
).
Near-future carbon dioxide levels alter fish behaviour by interfering with neurotransmitter function
.
Nat. Clim. Change
2
,
201
-
204
.
Orr
,
J. C.
,
Fabry
,
V. J.
,
Aumont
,
O.
,
Bopp
,
L.
,
Doney
,
S. C.
,
Feely
,
R. A.
,
Gnanadesikan
,
A.
,
Gruber
,
N.
,
Ishida
,
A.
,
Joos
,
F.
, et al. 
(
2005
).
Anthropogenic ocean acidification over the twenty-first century and its impact on calcifying organisms
.
Nature
437
,
681
-
686
.
Pörtner
,
H. O.
and
Farrell
,
A. P.
(
2008
).
Ecology: physiology and climate change
.
Science
322
,
690
-
692
.
Rosa
,
R.
,
Paula
,
J. R.
,
Sampaio
,
E.
,
Pimentel
,
M.
,
Lopes
,
A. R.
,
Baptista
,
M.
,
Guerreiro
,
M.
,
Santos
,
C.
,
Campos
,
D.
,
Almeida-Val
,
V. M. F.
, et al. 
(
2016
).
Neuro-oxidative damage and aerobic potential loss of sharks under elevated CO2 and warming
.
Mar. Biol.
163
,
1
-
10
.
Schalkhausser
,
B.
,
Bock
,
C.
,
Stemmer
,
K.
,
Brey
,
T.
,
Pörtner
,
H.-O.
and
Lannig
,
G.
(
2013
).
Impact of ocean acidification on escape performance of the king scallop, Pecten maximus, from Norway
.
Mar. Biol.
160
,
1995
-
2006
.
Spady
,
B. L.
,
Watson
,
S.-A.
,
Chase
,
T. J.
and
Munday
,
P. L.
(
2014
).
Projected near-future CO2 levels increase activity and alter defensive behaviours in the tropical squid Idiosepius pygmaeus
.
Biol. Open
3
,
1063
-
1070
.
Timmermann
,
A.
,
Oberhuber
,
J.
,
Bacher
,
A.
,
Esch
,
M.
,
Latif
,
M.
and
Roeckner
,
E.
(
1999
).
Increased El Nino frequency in a climate model forced by future greenhouse warming
.
Nature
398
,
694
-
697
.
Torres
,
R.
,
Turner
,
D. R.
,
Silva
,
N.
and
Rutllant
,
J.
(
1999
).
High short-term variability of CO2 fluxes during an upwelling event off the Chilean coast at 30 S
.
Deep Sea Res. Part I Oceanogr, Res, Pap,
46
,
1161
-
1179
.
Torres
,
R.
,
Turner
,
D. R.
,
Rutllant
,
J.
and
Lefèvre
,
N.
(
2003
).
Continued CO2 outgassing in an upwelling area off northern Chile during the development phase of El Niño 1997–1998 (July 1997)
.
J. Geophys. Res.
108
,
3336
.
Torres
,
R.
,
Manríquez
,
P. H.
,
Duarte
,
C.
,
Navarro
,
J. M.
,
Lagos
,
N. A.
,
Vargas
,
C. A.
and
Lardies
,
M. A.
(
2013
).
Evaluation of a semi-automatic system for long-term seawater carbonate chemistry manipulation
.
Rev. Chil. Hist. Nat.
86
,
443
-
451
.
Vallortigara
,
G.
and
Rogers
,
L. J.
(
2005
).
Survival with an asymmetrical brain: advantages and disadvantages of cerebral lateralization
.
Behav. Brain Sci.
28
,
575
-
589
.
von Schuckmann
,
K.
,
Palmer
,
M. D.
,
Trenberth
,
K. E.
,
Cazenave
,
A.
,
Chambers
,
D.
,
Champollion
,
N.
,
Hansen
,
J.
,
Josey
,
S. A.
,
Loeb
,
N.
,
Mathieu
,
P. P.
, et al. 
(
2016
).
An imperative to monitor Earth's energy imbalance
.
Nat. Clim. Change
6
,
138
-
144
.
Watson
,
S.-A.
,
Lefevre
,
S.
,
McCormick
,
M. I.
,
Domenici
,
P.
,
Nilsson
,
G. E.
and
Munday
,
P. L.
(
2014
).
Marine mollusc predator-escape behaviour altered by near-future carbon dioxide levels
.
Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci.
281
,
20132377
.
Wobbrock
,
J. O.
,
Findlater
,
L.
,
Gergle
,
D.
and
Higgins
,
J. J.
(
2011
).
The aligned rank transform for nonparametric factorial analyses using only ANOVA procedures
. In
Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems
, pp.
143
-
146
.
New York: ACM Press
.
Zar
,
J.
(
1984
).
Biostatistical Analysis
.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ
:
Prentice Hall
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing or financial interests.

Supplementary information