Because collective cognition emerges from local signaling among group members, deciphering communication systems is crucial to understanding the underlying mechanisms. Alarm signals are widespread in the social insects and can elicit a variety of behavioral responses to danger, but the functional plasticity of these signals has not been well studied. Here we report an alarm pheromone in the ant Temnothorax rugatulus that elicits two different behaviors depending on context. When an ant was tethered inside an unfamiliar nest site and unable to move freely, she released a pheromone from her mandibular gland that signaled other ants to reject this nest as a potential new home, presumably to avoid potential danger. When the same pheromone was presented near the ants' home nest, they were instead attracted to it, presumably to respond to a threat to the colony. We used coupled gas chromatography/mass spectrometry to identify candidate compounds from the mandibular gland and tested each one in a nest choice bioassay. We found that 2,5-dimethylpyrazine was sufficient to induce rejection of a marked new nest and also to attract ants when released at the home nest. This is the first detailed investigation of chemical communication in the leptothoracine ants. We discuss the possibility that this pheromone's deterrent function can improve an emigrating colony's nest site selection performance.

In many taxa, from slime molds to humans, groups cooperatively process information to achieve collective cognition (Couzin, 2009; Marshall and Franks, 2009). By distributing the burden of cognition across many individuals, groups can assess their environment and make consensus decisions, oftentimes more rapidly and accurately than a solitary animal could do (Biro et al., 2006; Sasaki et al., 2013; Ward et al., 2011). Collective cognition emerges in non-obvious ways from a complex network of local interactions among group members. Understanding this process requires decoding the specialized signals that group members exchange in these interactions (Sumpter, 2010). Communication systems, and the group behavior they underlie, have reached especially great diversity and complexity in the eusocial insects (Hölldobler and Wilson, 2009; Seeley, 1989; Wheeler, 1912). Extensive study of the ants and bees has revealed much about the physical nature and information content of signals, and how they contribute to emergent colony properties (Franks, 1989; Hirsh and Gordon, 2001; Marshall et al., 2009; Passino and Seeley, 2006; Pratt, 2005; Seeley and Buhrman, 2001; Seeley, 1997; Visscher, 2007).

Most of this work has concerned recruitment signals used by successful foragers or nest site scouts, but another fundamental type of communication is alarm signaling. In social insects, defensive behavior is closely connected with alarm signals that either recruit nestmates to combat a potential danger or warn them to stay away (Blum, 1969; Crewe and Fletcher, 1974; Maschwitz, 1964). Besides an early report (Goetsch, 1953), the first thorough study of chemical alarm communication in ants was on the pharaoh ant, Monomorium pharaonis (Sudd, 1957). Workers of this species reacted with escape behavior when a nestmate was crushed nearby. The first experimental investigations of the anatomical origin and chemical nature of alarm communication by Wilson (Wilson, 1958) on the harvester ant Pogonomyrmex badius and by Butenandt et al. (Butenandt et al., 1959) on the leafcutter ant Atta sexdens further showed that the worker ants of these species discharge a strong-smelling substance from the mandibular gland when they perceive a threat. The pheromone of P. badius was identified as 4-methyl-3-heptanone (McGurk et al., 1966), which was also later identified as the active component in the alarm pheromone of A. sexdens (Blum, 1969). In numerous subsequent investigations, various exocrine glands have been determined to be the sources of alarm pheromones (Buschinger and Maschwitz, 1984) and many compounds have been identified (Blum, 1985; Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990; Parry and Morgan, 1979; Vander Meer and Alonso, 1998).

In some ant species, alarm pheromones have been recognized as multi-component signals, whereby individual constituents of the blend of glandular secretions have different diffusion rates and accordingly elicit different behavioral responses in receivers (Bradshaw et al., 1975; Bradshaw et al., 1979; Fujiwara-Tsujii et al., 2006; Hölldobler and Wilson, 2009). The response behavior can also vary in different groups and castes of societies, and in time and space (Hölldobler, 1977). Although this functional plasticity was first recognized 50 years ago (Maschwitz, 1964), little attention has been given to specifying how social and environmental contexts, particularly those associated with collective information processing, affect behavioral responses to alarm pheromones in ants.

The present study reports the first analysis of context-specific functions of a hitherto unknown alarm pheromone in the myrmicine ant Temnothorax rugatulus (Emery 1895). Ants of this genus form small colonies typically comprising 150–250 workers. They usually live in small cavities, such as acorns and rock crevices, whose fragility requires frequent emigrations to new homes. They organize these moves using recruitment by tandem running and carrying of nestmates (Möglich, 1978), and they show remarkable abilities to collectively choose a single optimal nest among multiple options (Franks et al., 2002; Mallon et al., 2001; Pratt and Sumpter, 2006; Pratt et al., 2002; Sasaki et al., 2013). The role of chemical communication in Temnothorax societies is poorly known, other than that tandem run leaders discharge secretions from the poison gland that function as a recruitment signal (Möglich et al., 1974). In addition, indirect evidence suggests that nest site scouts of T. albipennis may place distinctive marks on undesirable nests that enhance the ability of colonies to collectively choose the best available site (Franks et al., 2007; Stroeymeyt et al., 2011; Stroeymeyt et al., 2014). However, the nature and origin of any such negative signal remains unknown. In preliminary observations, we noted that T. rugatulus colonies seemed reluctant to move into candidate nest sites in which some of their nestmates had been tethered to the nest wall. We set out to test whether these tethered ants released a pheromone that discouraged other ants from choosing the site and, if so, to determine the signal's anatomical source and its chemical identity. We further examined whether and how this signal functions outside the context of collective nest site selection.

Experiment 1: tethered ants emit a deterrent signal

We tested whether tethering ants in an unfamiliar nest site caused them to release a pheromone that signals other ants to reject the nest during colony migration. Colonies were given a binary choice between a nest with five tethered ants and an empty nest. The results showed that colonies have a strong preference for the empty nest (two-tailed binomial test: P=0.008; Fig. 1A). This pattern remained consistent even when the tethered ants were removed from the nest before a migration started (two-tailed binomial test: P=0.016; Fig. 1B). These results suggested that tethered ants released a pheromone that signals to other ants to reject the nest site. Video recordings showed that the tethered ants repeatedly opened their mandibles very wide while facing toward the nest floor. This suggests that this behavior is associated with release of a pheromone from their mandibular glands (supplementary material Movie 1). The mandible opening can also indicate aggressive behavior. Based on our observational experience, however, the mandible flaring is typically faster and aimed at an ‘enemy’ target during aggressive behavior. In the context of marking, however, mandible gaping is often slow and widely opened and pointed to the ground. Obviously, releasing an aversive pheromone or an alarm pheromone are parts of the same behavioral syndrome closely related to aggressive behavior.

Experiment 2: the signal originates in the head

If the pheromone originates in the mandibular gland, we predicted that marking a nest with crushed heads, thus releasing the pheromone, would cause ants to reject it. When presented with a binary choice between a nest with five crushed heads and a nest with five crushed alitrunks, colonies showed a strong preference for the alitrunk nest (two-tailed binomial test: P<0.001; Fig. 2A). When gasters were used instead of alitrunks, the gaster nest was significantly preferred over the head nest (two-tailed binomial test: P<0.001; Fig. 2B).

These results suggest that the ants rejected the nest that contained heads, but it might instead be the case that they were attracted to alitrunks and gasters. To exclude this possibility, we also tested a binary choice between a nest with five alitrunks or five gasters and an empty nest. Colonies showed no preference for either alitrunks (eight in empty, four in alitrunk, three split decisions; two-tailed binomial test: P=0.38) or gasters (seven in empty, three in gaster, five split decisions; two-tailed binomial test: P=0.34).

Experiment 3: the signal is present in solvent extracts of the head

After the results of Experiment 2 indicated the head as the source of the signal, we next tested whether the same effect could be produced by chemical extracts of the heads. Given a binary choice between a nest with a hexane extract of the head and a nest treated with only hexane, colonies strongly preferred the hexane-treated nest (two-tailed binomial test: P<0.001; Fig. 3A). This pattern remained consistent even when migrations started 14 h after chemical compounds were applied to the papers (two-tailed binomial test: P=0.049; Fig. 3B). These results indicate that chemical compounds from heads signaled ants to reject the nest, and this effect persisted for at least 14 h.

Experiment 4: the mandibular gland contains multiple compounds

Coupled gas chromatography/mass spectrometry (GC/MS) was used to identify compounds in ant heads. The GC/MS analyses of volatile compounds collected from dissected mandibular glands by solid phase microextraction (SPME) revealed the presence of several substances. To distinguish glandular compounds from contaminants, we compared these results with parallel analyses of empty vials (Fig. 4) and found three compounds that were clearly derived from the mandibular glands: 2,5-dimethylpyrazine (DMP; peak 1), benzyl alcohol (peak 2) and 2-phenethyl alcohol (peak 4). Because it is extremely difficult to dissect the mandibular glands of these tiny ants without risking some contamination with secretions from the postpharyngeal gland or other sources, we cannot be certain whether several other compounds, such as nonanal (peak 3), undecanal (peak 7) and geranyl acetone (peak 8) are part of the mandibular gland secretions. We therefore also conducted either full bioassay series (for nonanal and decanal) or pilot tests (for geranyl acetone and undecanal) with these compounds. None of these compounds elicited any detectable behavioral responses from test ants, and so no extended bioassay series were carried out with these substances. It is also worth noting that some of these components such as the aldehydes are common contaminants (see Fig. 4), for example from human skin odors, although this is clearly not the case for the compounds DMP, benzyl alcohol and 2-phenethyl alcohol.

Experiment 5: DMP induces rejection of a nest site

We tested a series of binary choices between a nest with hexane solutions of one of eight compounds identified in Experiment 4 and a nest treated only with hexane. Ants were significantly more likely to choose the hexane-treated nest only when the other nest had DMP (two-tailed binomial test: P<0.01). They also tended to reject the nonanal nest (two-tailed binomial test: P=0.10). When the solutions of nonanal and DMP were diluted to 5 ppm, the effect disappeared for nonanal (two-tailed binomial test: P=1), but not for DMP (two-tailed binomial test: P<0.01). Surprisingly, ants rejected the nest with DMP even when it was as low as 0.5 ppm (~2.5 ng of DMP on each filter paper; two-tailed binomial test: P<0.01). However, because we were unable to measure the actual amount of DMP in the mandibular gland, it is uncertain whether this tiny dose is at or above the biologically relevant amount. Furthermore, the effect of 5 ppm DMP (~25 ng) seemed to persist even after 14 h (two-tailed binomial test: P<0.01), consistent with the results of extracted heads in Experiment 3. Table 1 shows the summary of these tests. The long-lasting effect of DMP (which is quite volatile) inside test nests is possibly due to the fact that these nests are relatively closed entities so that the applied DMP dissipates slowly, and residues of the compound can still be detected by the ants after 14 h.

We further tested whether the ants were sensitive to the dose of DMP by presenting a choice between a nest with 5 ppm and a nest with 0.5 ppm DMP. The results suggested that the ants rejected the nest with the higher dose of DMP (two-tailed binomial test: P=0.07) (Fig. 5) and thus could distinguish different DMP doses, at least between 5 and 0.5 ppm.

Experiment 6: the signal induces attraction to the entrance when released at the home nest

Once we identified DMP as the signal responsible for nest rejection, we tested whether it would elicit a different behavior in another context. When a head was crushed and presented near the home nest entrance, it attracted significantly more ants than did the controls (Fig. 6). Alternatively, when the head was presented to ants away from their home nest, it was more often rejected than the controls (Table 2). Our preliminary test showed that dissected mandibular glands elicited responses similar to those elicited by the head in both contexts. Furthermore, crushed heads from which the mandibular glands had been removed did not elicit these behaviors, indicating that the mandibular gland was the source of the pheromone.

Presentation of DMP elicited the same patterns of responses as the intact head: it attracted ants that were in a home nest (Fig. 6) but repelled them when they were away from home (Table 2), confirming that DMP is the semiochemical mediating these behaviors. Surprisingly, a very low dose of DMP (0.5 ppm) still elicited these behaviors (Fig. 6).

Chemical alarm signals are ubiquitous in the Formicidae. They are found even in phylogenetically less derived subfamilies, such as the Ponerinae and Myrmeciinae, which typically do not employ mass communication (Billen and Morgan, 1998; Duffield and Blum, 1973; Duffield et al., 1976; Hölldobler and Taylor, 1983; Longhurst et al., 1978; Wheeler and Blum, 1973). Nevertheless, for many ant species no records yet exist as to whether alarm pheromones are used. The closely related myrmicine genera Leptothorax and Temnothorax belong to this group. It has even been suggested that alarm pheromones might be absent in species such as those in these genera, which have very small colony sizes, because a massive group defense is unlikely (Maschwitz, 1964).

Our present study is the first demonstration and in-depth investigation of alarm communication in the genus Temnothorax (formerly Leptothorax). Chemical analyses combined with behavioral bioassays identified DMP as an alarm pheromone. Pyrazines have been previously reported as alarm pheromones in other ant species. For example, 2-ethyl-3,5-dimethylpyrazine and 2,5-dimethyl-3-isopentylpyrazine have been reported to be at least part of an alarm pheromone in the ponerine species Odontomachus brunneus and Odontomachus hastatus, respectively (Longhurst et al., 1978; Wheeler and Blum, 1973). Among the myrmicine ants, only the fire ant Solenopsis invicta has previously been shown to use a pyrazine as an alarm pheromone, specifically 2-ethyl-3,6-dimethylpyrazine originating in the mandibular glands of workers, males and female sexuals (Vander Meer et al., 2010). DMP, identified here as an alarm pheromone, is also known in other myrmicine species. However, it is typically used as a trail pheromone originating from the poison gland (Billen and Morgan, 1998). To our knowledge this is the first report of its function as an alarm pheromone originating in the mandibular gland.

Alarm pheromones may have different behavioral effects on different recipients. For example, in some ant species young workers respond to alarm pheromones by retreating into the nest, whereas older workers move out and exhibit aggressive behavior (Maschwitz, 1964; also see Hölldobler, 1977). Reactions may also vary among different species. In the harvester ant genus Pogonomyrmex, which have large colonies, old workers are attracted to low concentrations of their alarm pheromone, 4-methyl-3-heptanone. At high concentrations, they either show aggressive behavior or perform digging behavior in an attempt to rescue a buried nestmate (Wilson, 1958; Wilson and Bossert, 1963). Species with small colonies, in contrast, may react very differently. For example, workers of the ponerine ant Hypoponera opacior frantically evacuate the area when nestmates release the alarm signal 2,5-dimethyl-3-isopentylpyrazine from their mandibular glands (Duffield et al., 1976).

Although the diversity of behaviors elicited by alarm pheromones is well appreciated, little attention has been given to the context specificity of responses. In the first thorough research on this topic, Maschwitz (Maschwitz, 1964) showed that, for some hymenopteran species, alarm signals release aggressive behavior when discharged close to the nest, but escape behavior when emitted far from the nest. In the subsequent 50 years, there has been little further investigation of context-specific responses. Our findings are consistent with the pattern Maschwitz described. When Temnothorax workers perceived the alarm pheromone in the arena far from their nest, they exhibited escape behavior. In contrast, when the alarm signal was instead presented at the nest entrance, a large number of workers inside the nest moved towards the nest entrance. Video recordings of pilot tests suggest that these workers then attempted to close the nest entrance (supplementary material Movie 2), behavior that was not seen on exposure to a hexane control. This is consistent with previous findings that they use soil and debris to reduce entrance size or even to close it entirely for defensive purposes (Aleksiev et al., 2007). These observations are preliminary, and further investigation will be required to show whether the compound actually elicits entrance-closing behavior.

The importance of positive feedback to collective decision-making has been extensively investigated (Camazine et al., 2003; Jeanson et al., 2012; Sumpter, 2010; Sumpter and Pratt, 2009), but the role of negative feedback has, until recently, been less appreciated. Honeybee foragers have been found to use a form of vibrational communication – the stop signal – to suppress recruitment to a food source where they had been briefly trapped, perhaps to reduce the colony's exposure to dangerous areas (Nieh, 2010). Our study similarly showed that Temnothorax workers tethered within a site release a signal that induces their nestmates to avoid moving there. This effect can be considered altruistic because it does not lead to rescue of the signaler, but instead helps the colony as a whole to avoid danger (Blum, 1985).

Negative signals may also contribute to the speed or accuracy of a colony's collective decision-making. Many species rely on positive feedback from mass recruitment to concentrate foraging forces on the best available food source (Hölldobler and Wilson, 2009; Seeley, 1995; Sumpter, 2010). In a few species, evidence suggests that scouts apply repellent pheromones to deter nestmates from foraging in areas of low-quality food (Giurfa and Núñez, 1992; Robinson et al., 2005; Robinson et al., 2008; Stout et al., 1998). Theoretical models predict that such repellent signals can prevent the strong positive feedback of mass recruitment from locking a colony into a suboptimal choice (Giurfa and Núñez, 1992; Robinson et al., 2005; Robinson et al., 2008; Stout et al., 1998). However, none of these proposed pheromones have been identified. A much clearer example of negative signaling in the context of decision-making was recently found in honeybees (Seeley et al., 2012). The stop signal, noted above for its use by foragers, is also used by nest site scouts during a colony's collective choice of a new nest site. Successful scouts, in addition to recruiting to the site they have found, use stop signals to inhibit recruitment to competing sites. This may serve to speed the attainment of consensus on a single site, and may also enhance the colony's ability to optimize the trade-off between decision speed and accuracy. Indeed, the role of these signals in nest site choice is remarkably similar to inhibitory pathways in analogous decision-making systems in the primate brain (Hofstadter, 1999; Passino et al., 2008; Seeley and Buhrman, 2001; Visscher, 2007). In both systems, populations (of either neurons or ants) accumulate evidence for competing options; a decision is made for whichever population first crosses a threshold (of either neural activity or ant numbers). Models suggest that mutual inhibition between the populations allows them to make a statistically optimal trade-off between decision speed and accuracy (Marshall et al., 2009).

Emigrating Temnothorax colonies follow a remarkably similar nest choice strategy, but the potential role of inhibition for their decisions remains uncertain. Indirect evidence indicates that T. albipennis leave a deterrent signal in low-quality nests during emigrations (Franks et al., 2007; Stroeymeyt et al., 2014; Stroeymeyt et al., 2011). The nature of this signal has not been determined, but it may be the same as the alarm pheromone that we have identified in T. rugatulus. In both cases, unlike other reported negative pheromones (Giurfa and Núñez, 1992; Robinson et al., 2005; Robinson et al., 2008; Stout et al., 1998), the signal does not actually repel ants from entering a marked nest, but instead reduces the colony's probability of moving to the nest (Stroeymeyt et al., 2014; T.S. and B.H., personal observation). The signal could accomplish this by altering the behavior of a scout that enters a marked nest, perhaps causing her to refrain from recruiting other ants to the nest. We speculate that Temnothorax ants may use DMP as an integral part of their decision-making strategy. However, testing this idea must await detailed observations on whether and how scouts emit and respond to this signal during colony emigration.

Nest designs

We evaluated pheromone effects in the context of nest site selection experiments carried out in laboratory arenas. Each candidate nest was made from a balsa wood slat (2.4 mm thick) sandwiched between glass microscope slides (50×75 mm). A circular cavity (38 mm diameter) was cut through the middle of the slat, and a round entrance hole (Ø=2 mm) was drilled through the center of the glass roof (Fig. 7). The entrance of the home nest was either a hole (Ø=3.2 mm) on the center of the roof or a slit (2 mm) cut out of the side of the nest (Sasaki et al., 2013). Balsa slats were made fresh for each experiment and never reused. Glass slides were reused after washing in a commercial dishwasher. The walls of experimental arenas were coated with Fluon to prevent the ants from escaping. Before each experiment, the experimental arena was cleaned with ethanol to remove any chemical marks that the ants may have left.

Subjects

A total 126 colonies of Temnothorax rugatulus were used. Each colony was used only once in each experiment except for Experiment 5. Colonies were collected in the Pinal Mountains near Globe, Arizona. All had at least one queen, with worker populations ranging from 121 to 280 and brood populations ranging from 18 to ~300. Each colony was housed in a nest such as those described above. Nests were kept in a plastic box (11×11 cm) with Fluon-coated walls. Each box was provided with a water-filled plastic tube capped with cotton and an agar-based diet that was refreshed weekly (Bhatkar and Whitcomb, 1970; Sasaki et al., 2013).

Experiment 1: do tethered ants release a pheromone?

Ants were tethered with a string of silk (part no. 7.091, Louet North America, Prescott, ONT, Canada) tied around the petiole using a knot tyer (Haight, 2012). The length of each string was ~2 cm with one side fastened with adhesive tape between the floor glass and the balsa sheet. Five worker ants from the same colony were tethered in the same nest, equidistant from each other (Fig. 7).

Colonies were given a binary choice between a nest with tethered ants and a nest that had five strings but no ants. These two target nests were first placed adjacent to one another against one wall of the test arena (Fig. 8). The home nest containing the colony from which the tethered ants were taken was then placed against the center of the wall opposite to the location of the target nests. Finally, the roof of the home nest was removed to induce migration.

The colony's choice was assayed by recording the site occupied 12 h after inducing the migration. In every trial, all ants moved entirely from the home nest to one of the target sites. If one site contained more than 90% of colony members, including all queens and brood items, we designated that as the colony's choice. If this criterion was not achieved, the choice was recorded as a ‘split’ decision.

To exclude the possibility that ants avoided the nest as a result of direct contacts with the tethered ants, we also conducted another experiment, in which tethered ants were absent during the migration. The procedure was identical to the one described above except that the tethered ants were left in the nest for 3 h and then removed immediately before the migration was induced.

To closely observe the behavior of ants releasing pheromone, we additionally filmed tethered ants using a high-resolution camera (Canon EOS Rebel T2i; www.usa.canon.com) with a macro lens (Canon MP-E 65 mm f/2.8 1-5x macro lens). The ants were tethered in the same way described above.

Experiment 2: does the pheromone come from the head?

We freeze-killed five worker ants from the same colony and used fine forceps to separate each ant's head and gaster from its alitrunk. We then placed five heads in a nest, equidistant from one another, and crushed them with a wooden applicator stick to release any potential pheromones. We similarly crushed either five alitrunks or gasters in another nest. The colony from which the crushed ants were taken was then induced to choose between these nests, as in Experiment 1 (Fig. 8). To test whether the effect of alarm pheromone would persist over time, we repeated the experiment, except that the emigration was induced 14 h after crushing the body parts. The colony's choice was assayed by recording the site occupied 12 h after inducing the emigration using the same criteria as in Experiment 1.

Experiment 3: is the pheromone present in a chemical extract of heads?

Twenty heads from the same colony were placed in 100 μl hexane and crushed with a wooden applicator stick. After 3 h, we used a glass syringe (www.hamiltoncompany.com) to apply 5 μl of this solution to a small filter paper (~1×1 cm), which was then placed in a standard nest (Fig. 7). Another nest received a similar filter paper marked with 5 μl of pure hexane. The colony from which the ants were taken was then induced to choose between these nests, as in Experiment 1 (Fig. 8). The colony's choice was assayed by recording the site occupied 12 h after inducing the emigration using the same criteria as in Experiment 1.

Experiment 4: identification of substances in the mandibular gland

Ants were freeze-killed and shipped to the University of California, Riverside, on dry ice. After thawing, the ants were decapitated, and groups of approximately 50 heads were transferred to 1.5 ml glass vials. The heads were crushed with a flat-bottomed glass rod, and the top of the vial was tightly covered with aluminum foil. A polydimethylsiloxane SPME fiber was cleaned by thermal desorption in a GC injector port at 250°C for 5 min, and after cooling, the fiber was inserted into the covered vial and left exposed to the headspace volatiles for 45 min. The loaded fiber was then thermally desorbed in the injector port of the GC/MS for 30 s in splitless mode, with an injector temperature of 250°C. The GC was fitted with a 30 m×0.25 mm ID DB-5 column (J&W Scientific, Folsom, CA, USA), and was temperature programmed from 10°C for 1 min, then 10°C min−1 to 280°C, hold 20 min. Analyses were conducted with a 6890N GC interfaced to a 5975C mass selective detector (Agilent Technologies, Wilmington, DE, USA), with electron impact ionization (70 eV). Compounds were tentatively identified by matches with the NIS mass spectral database, and identifications were confirmed by matching mass spectra and retention times with those of authentic standards. Analogous analyses were conducted on the crushed bodies minus the heads. Authentic standards were purchased from Aldrich Chemical Co. (Milwaukee, WI, USA).

To confirm that compounds found in the volatiles from the crushed heads were from the mandibular glands, approximately 35 glands were dissected from the heads of freeze-killed workers (Fig. 9) and placed in a 1 ml tapered glass screw-cap vial with a Teflon septum. The septum was punctured with a needle, and the SPME fiber was inserted through the hole to collect volatiles. The volatiles were then analyzed as described above. The analyses were replicated with two sets of dissected glands.

Experiment 5: testing candidate chemical compounds

All eight compounds identified from the mandibular gland were first diluted to 50 ppm in hexane, or even lower if a 50 ppm dilution elicited an effect. As in Experiment 3, we applied 5 μl of one of these solutions to a small filter paper and placed it in the standard nest (Fig. 7). We also applied 5 μl of hexane to a filter paper and placed it in an identical nest. A colony was then induced to choose between these nests, as in Experiment 1 (Fig. 8).

The colony's choice was assayed by recording the site occupied 12 h after inducing the emigration using the same criteria as in Experiment 1. A total of 69 colonies were used, and all were used three or four times, but no colony experienced the same compound more than once. At least 10 days elapsed between experiments on a given colony, to avoid any influence of previous migrations on the current migration (Langridge et al., 2004; Langridge et al., 2008).

Experiment 6: does the pheromone elicit different behaviors in different contexts?

We crushed a head with a wooden applicator stick or applied DMP [2 μl of 5 ppm (10.0 ng), 0.5 ppm (1.0 ng), 0.1 ppm (200 pg) or 0.05 ppm (100 pg) solution] to a stick. The stick was then slowly presented near the ant's home nest. The reaction was measured by counting how many ants within the home nest moved towards the nest entrance (i.e. 1 cm mark from the entrance was placed on the computer screen and a number of ants who completely crossed this line was counted). We did not count ants that were already by the entrance when the stick was introduced. The order of the tests was randomized, and at least 45 min elapsed between tests. The DMP was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich (St Louis, MO, USA).

To confirm that the source of the pheromone was the mandibular gland, we also presented a dissected mandibular gland and a head from which the mandibular gland had been removed. Finally, we presented an untreated stick and a stick treated with hexane as controls.

We further tested how the ants responded to the same alarm pheromone when they were not in the home nest. Similar to the previous test, a head or DMP was first applied to a stick. We then slowly presented the stick to ants that were at least 10 cm away from their home nest. Their reaction was categorized as either avoidance (walking away from the stick) or attraction (walking towards the stick). The order of the tests was randomized, and each ant was tested only once.

Statistical analysis

We tested nest site preferences using a two-tailed binomial test in Experiments 1, 2, 3 and 5. Split colonies were not included in the analyses. A two-tailed t-test was used for investigating attraction released by DMP inside the home nest, and a χ2 test of partial independence was used for investigating avoidance of DMP away from the home nest in Experiment 6. The statistical package R (v. 2.9.0) was used for all analyses.

We thank Kevin Haight for teaching us how to make the knot tyer.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Science Foundation (award no. 1012029) and by funds of Arizona State University to B.H.

Aleksiev
A. S.
,
Sendova-Franks
A. B.
,
Franks
N. R.
(
2007
).
Nest ‘moulting’ in the ant Temnothorax albipennis
.
Anim. Behav.
74
,
567
-
575
.
Bhatkar
A.
,
Whitcomb
W. H.
(
1970
).
Artificial diet for rearing various species of ants
.
Fla. Entomol.
53
,
229
-
232
.
Billen
J.
,
Morgan
E. D.
(
1998
).
Pheromone communication in social insects: sources and secretions
. In
Pheromone Communication in Social Insects Ants, Wasps, Bees and Termites
(ed.
Vander Meer
R. K.
,
Breed
M. D.
,
Espelie
K. E.
,
Winston
M. L.
), pp.
3
-
33
.
Boulder, CO
:
Westview Press
.
Biro
D.
,
Sumpter
D. J. T.
,
Meade
J.
,
Guilford
T.
(
2006
).
From compromise to leadership in pigeon homing
.
Curr. Biol.
16
,
2123
-
2128
.
Blum
M. S.
(
1969
).
Alarm pheromones
.
Annu. Rev. Entomol.
14
,
57
-
80
.
Blum
M. S.
(
1985
).
Alarm pheromones
. In
Comprehensive Insect Physiology, Biochemistry and Pharmacology: Behaviour
, Vol.
9
(ed.
Kerkut
G. A.
,
Gilbert
L. I.
), pp.
193
-
224
.
New York, NY
:
Pergamon Press
.
Bradshaw
J. W. S.
,
Baker
R.
,
Howse
P. E.
(
1975
).
Multicomponent alarm pheromones of the weaver ant
.
Nature
258
,
230
-
231
.
Bradshaw
J. W. S.
,
Baker
R.
,
Howse
P. E.
(
1979
).
Multicomponent alarm pheromones in the mandibular glands of major workers of the African weaver ant, Oecophylla longinoda
.
Physiol. Entomol.
4
,
15
-
25
.
Buschinger
A.
,
Maschwitz
U.
(
1984
).
Defensive behavior and defensive mechanisms in ants
. In
Defensive Mechanisms in Social Insect
(ed.
Hermann
H. R.
), pp.
95
-
150
.
New York, NY
:
Praeger
.
Butenandt
A.
,
Linzen
B.
,
Lindauer
M.
(
1959
).
Über einen Duftstoff aus der Mandibeldrüse der Blattschneiderameise Atta sexdens rubropilosa Forel
.
Arch. Anat. Microsc. Morphol. Exp.
48
,
13
-
19
.
Camazine
S.
,
Deneubourg
J.
,
Franks
N.
,
Sneyd
J.
,
Theraulaz
G.
,
Bonabeau
E.
(
2003
).
Self-organization in Biological Systems
.
Princeton, NJ
:
Princeton University Press
.
Couzin
I. D.
(
2009
).
Collective cognition in animal groups
.
Trends Cogn. Sci.
13
,
36
-
43
.
Crewe
R. M.
,
Fletcher
D.
(
1974
).
Ponerine ant secretions: the mandibular gland secretion of Paltothyreus tarsatus Fabr
.
J. Entomol. Soc. South Africa
37
,
291
-
298
.
Duffield
R. M.
,
Blum
M. S.
(
1973
).
4-Methyl-3-heptanone: identification and function in Neoponera villosa (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)
.
Ann. Entomol. Soc. Am.
66
,
1357
.
Duffield
R. M.
,
Blum
M. S.
,
Wheeler
J. W.
(
1976
).
Alkylpyrazine alarm pheromones in primitive ants with small colonial units
.
Comp. Biochem. Physiol. B
54
,
439
-
440
.
Franks
N. R.
(
1989
).
Army ants: a collective intelligence
.
Am. Sci.
77
,
138
-
145
.
Franks
N. R.
,
Pratt
S. C.
,
Mallon
E. B.
,
Britton
N. F.
,
Sumpter
D. J.
(
2002
).
Information flow, opinion polling and collective intelligence in house-hunting social insects
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B
357
,
1567
-
1583
.
Franks
N. R.
,
Hooper
J. W.
,
Dornhaus
A.
,
Aukett
P. J.
,
Hayward
A. L.
,
Berghoff
S. M.
(
2007
).
Reconnaissance and latent learning in ants
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
274
,
1505
-
1509
.
Fujiwara-Tsujii
N.
,
Yamagata
N.
,
Takeda
T.
,
Mizunami
M.
,
Yamaoka
R.
(
2006
).
Behavioral responses to the alarm pheromone of the ant Camponotus obscuripes (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)
.
Zoolog. Sci.
23
,
353
-
358
.
Giurfa
M.
,
Núñez
J. A.
(
1992
).
Honeybees mark with scent and reject recently visited flowers
.
Oecologia
89
,
113
-
117
.
Goetsch
W.
(
1953
).
Vergleichende Biologie der Insektenstaaten
.
Leipzig
:
Geest und Portig
.
Haight
K. L.
(
2012
).
Patterns of venom production and temporal polyethism in workers of Jerdon's jumping ant, Harpegnathos saltator
.
J. Insect Physiol.
58
,
1568
-
1574
.
Hirsh
A.
,
Gordon
D.
(
2001
).
Distributed problem solving in social insects
.
Ann. Math. Artif. Intell.
31
,
199
-
221
.
Hofstadter
D. R.
(
1999
).
Gödel, Escher, Bach
.
New York, NY
:
Basic Books
.
Hölldobler
B.
(
1977
).
Communication in social Hymenoptera
. In
How Animals Communicate
(ed.
Sebeok
T. A.
), pp.
418
-
470
.
Bloomington, IN; London
:
Indiana University Press
.
Hölldobler
B.
,
Taylor
R. W.
(
1983
).
A behavioral study of the primitive ant Nothomyrmecia macrops Clark
.
Insectes Soc.
30
,
384
-
401
.
Hölldobler
B.
,
Wilson
E. O.
(
1990
).
The Ants
.
Cambridge, MA
:
Belknap Press of Harvard University
.
Hölldobler
B.
,
Wilson
E. O.
(
2009
).
The Superorganism
.
New York, NY
:
W. W. Norton & Company
.
Jeanson
R.
,
Dussutour
A.
,
Fourcassié
V.
(
2012
).
Key factors for the emergence of collective decisions in invertebrates
.
Front. Neurosci.
6
,
121
.
Langridge
E. A.
,
Franks
N. R.
,
Sendova-Franks
A. B.
(
2004
).
Improvement in collective performance with experience in ants
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
56
,
523
-
529
.
Langridge
E. A.
,
Sendova-Franks
A. B.
,
Franks
N. R.
(
2008
).
How experienced individuals contribute to an improvement in collective performance in ants
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
62
,
447
-
456
.
Longhurst
C.
,
Baker
R.
,
Howse
P. E.
,
Speed
W.
(
1978
).
Alkylpyrazines in ponerine ants: their presence in three genera, and caste specific behavioural responses to them in Odontomachus troglodytes
.
J. Insect Physiol.
24
,
833
-
837
.
Mallon
E. B.
,
Pratt
S. C.
,
Franks
N. R.
(
2001
).
Individual and collective decision-making during nest site selection by the ant Leptothorax albipennis
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
50
,
352
-
359
.
Marshall
J. A. R.
,
Franks
N. R.
(
2009
).
Colony-level cognition
.
Curr. Biol.
19
,
R395
-
R396
.
Marshall
J. A. R.
,
Bogacz
R.
,
Dornhaus
A.
,
Planqué
R.
,
Kovacs
T.
,
Franks
N. R.
(
2009
).
On optimal decision-making in brains and social insect colonies
.
J. R. Soc. Interface
6
,
1065
-
1074
.
Maschwitz
U.
(
1964
).
Gefahrenalarmstoffe und gefahrenalarmierung bei sozialen Hymenopteren
.
Z. Vgl. Physiol.
47
,
596
-
655
.
McGurk
D. J.
,
Frost
J.
,
Eisenbraun
E. J.
,
Vick
K.
,
Drew
W. A.
,
Young
J.
(
1966
).
Volatile compounds in ants: identification of 4-methyl-3-heptanone from Pogonomyrmex ants
.
J. Insect Physiol.
12
,
1435
-
1441
.
Möglich
M.
(
1978
).
Social organization of nest emigration in Leptothorax
.
Insectes Soc.
25
,
205
-
225
.
Möglich
M.
,
Maschwitz
U.
,
Hölldobler
B.
(
1974
).
Tandem calling: a new kind of signal in ant communication
.
Science
186
,
1046
-
1047
.
Nieh
J. C.
(
2010
).
A negative feedback signal that is triggered by peril curbs honey bee recruitment
.
Curr. Biol.
20
,
310
-
315
.
Parry
K.
,
Morgan
E. D.
(
1979
).
Pheromones of ants: a review
.
Physiol. Entomol.
4
,
161
-
189
.
Passino
K.
,
Seeley
T.
(
2006
).
Modeling and analysis of nest-site selection by honeybee swarms: the speed and accuracy trade-off
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
59
,
427
-
442
.
Passino
K. M.
,
Seeley
T. D.
,
Visscher
P. K.
(
2008
).
Swarm cognition in honey bees
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
62
,
401
-
414
.
Pratt
S.
(
2005
).
Behavioral mechanisms of collective nest-site choice by the ant Temnothorax curvispinosus
.
Insectes Soc.
52
,
383
-
392
.
Pratt
S. C.
,
Sumpter
D. J. T.
(
2006
).
A tunable algorithm for collective decision-making
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
103
,
15906
-
15910
.
Pratt
S.
,
Mallon
E.
,
Sumpter
D.
,
Franks
N.
(
2002
).
Quorum sensing, recruitment, and collective decision-making during colony emigration by the ant Leptothorax albipennis
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
52
,
117
-
127
.
Robinson
E. J. H.
,
Jackson
D. E.
,
Holcombe
M.
,
Ratnieks
F. L. W.
(
2005
).
Insect communication: ‘no entry’ signal in ant foraging
.
Nature
438
,
442
-
442
.
Robinson
E. J. H.
,
Ratnieks
F. L. W.
,
Holcombe
M.
(
2008
).
An agent-based model to investigate the roles of attractive and repellent pheromones in ant decision making during foraging
.
J. Theor. Biol.
255
,
250
-
258
.
Sasaki
T.
,
Granovskiy
B.
,
Mann
R. P.
,
Sumpter
D. J. T.
,
Pratt
S. C.
(
2013
).
Ant colonies outperform individuals when a sensory discrimination task is difficult but not when it is easy
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
110
,
13769
-
13773
.
Seeley
T.
(
1989
).
The honey bee colony as a superorganism
.
Am. Sci.
77
,
546
-
553
.
Seeley
T. D.
(
1995
).
The Wisdom of the Hive
.
Cambridge, MA
:
Harvard University Press
.
Seeley
T. D.
(
1997
).
Honey bee colonies are group-level adaptive units
.
Am. Nat.
150
Suppl. 1
,
S22
-
S41
.
Seeley
T.
,
Buhrman
S.
(
2001
).
Nest-site selection in honey bees: how well do swarms implement the ‘best-of-N’ decision rule
?
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
49
,
416
-
427
.
Seeley
T. D.
,
Visscher
P. K.
,
Schlegel
T.
,
Hogan
P. M.
,
Franks
N. R.
,
Marshall
J. A. R.
(
2012
).
Stop signals provide cross inhibition in collective decision-making by honeybee swarms
.
Science
335
,
108
-
111
.
Stout
J. C.
,
Goulson
D.
,
Allen
J. A.
(
1998
).
Repellent scent-marking of flowers by a guild of foraging bumblebees (Bombus spp.)
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
43
,
317
-
326
.
Stroeymeyt
N.
,
Franks
N. R.
,
Giurfa
M.
(
2011
).
Knowledgeable individuals lead collective decisions in ants
.
J. Exp. Biol.
214
,
3046
-
3054
.
Stroeymeyt
N.
,
Jordan
C.
,
Mayer
G.
,
Hovsepian
S.
,
Giurfa
M.
,
Franks
N. R.
(
2014
).
Seasonality in communication and collective decision-making in ants
.
Proc. Biol. Sci.
281
,
20133108
.
Sudd
J. H.
(
1957
).
A response of worker ants to dead ants of their own species
.
Nature
179
,
431
-
432
.
Sumpter
D. J. T.
(
2010
).
Collective Animal Behavior
.
Princeton, NJ
:
Princeton University Press
.
Sumpter
D. J. T.
,
Pratt
S. C.
(
2009
).
Quorum responses and consensus decision making
.
Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B
364
,
743
-
753
.
Vander Meer
R. K.
,
Alonso
L. E.
(
1998
).
Pheromone directed behavior in ants
. In
Pheromone Communication in Social Insects
(ed.
Vander Meer
R. K.
,
Breed
M. D.
,
Espelie
K. E.
,
Winston
M. L.
), pp.
166
-
174
.
Boulder, CO
:
Westview Press
.
Vander Meer
R. K.
,
Preston
C. A.
,
Choi
M.-Y.
(
2010
).
Isolation of a pyrazine alarm pheromone component from the fire ant, Solenopsis invicta
.
J. Chem. Ecol.
36
,
163
-
170
.
Visscher
P. K.
(
2007
).
Group decision making in nest-site selection among social insects
.
Annu. Rev. Entomol.
52
,
255
-
275
.
Ward
A. J. W.
,
Herbert-Read
J. E.
,
Sumpter
D. J. T.
,
Krause
J.
(
2011
).
Fast and accurate decisions through collective vigilance in fish shoals
.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
108
,
2312
-
2315
.
Wheeler
W.
(
1912
).
The ant-colony as an organism
.
J. Morphol.
22
,
307
-
325
.
Wheeler
J. W.
,
Blum
M. S.
(
1973
).
Alkylpyrazine alarm pheromones in ponerine ants
.
Science
182
,
501
-
503
.
Wilson
E. O.
(
1958
).
A chemical releaser of alarm and digging behavior in the ant Pogonomyrmex badius (Latreille)
.
Psyche
65
,
41
-
51
.
Wilson
E. O.
,
Bossert
W. H.
(
1963
).
Chemical communication among animals
.
Recent Prog. Horm. Res.
19
,
673
-
716
.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Supplementary information