The genetic basis, mode of inheritance and recent evolutionary changes of migratory directions in birds are discussed on the basis of published and new experimental evidence. Displacement experiments with wild-caught migrants and orientation tests with hand-reared passerines illustrate that inexperienced young birds possess genetic information about the direction and approximate distance of migration, but not about the geographic location of the winter quarters. Hand-raised blackcaps Sylvia atricapilla from east and west of the Central European migratory divide, when tested under identical conditions, exibited population-specific migratory directions in orientation cages. Cross-breeding of birds from these two populations demonstrated an intermediate mode of inheritance of this behavioural character. New data on the orientation of an F2 generation suggest that the directional information is encoded by only a few major genes. Migratory adaptations may have evolved recently, in some cases rapidly, as is illustrated by the establishment of a new migration route of central European blackcaps to winter quarters in the British Isles. This new route is shown (in a captive breeding experiment) to be based on a novel, genetically programmed westnorthwesterly migratory direction. It must have spread from almost zero to 7–11 % frequency in parts of central Europe within only three decades. The novel direction is also inherited phenotypically intermediately; its rapid evolution may be mediated by assortative mating based on differential arrival times at the breeding grounds. The evolutionary flexibility of migratory adaptations is discussed in relation to changes in the environment, both natural and accelerated by man.

One of the fundamental questions of bird migration research is how young birds that initiate migration for the first time know where to fly. The question is especially relevant to species in which young birds migrate independently of experienced adults, i.e. the majority of nocturnally migrating passerines. From a conceptual point of view, we need to distinguish between a bird’s ‘knowledge’ of a migratory direction and the compass mechanisms employed to locate that intended flight direction in space. Compass mechanisms have been studied in detail and, although it is not entirely clear how magnetic and celestial mechanisms interact during ontogeny, it is well established that under most environmental circumstances birds can determine compass directions reliably. This review will be concerned only with the genetic basis of directional information, the mode of inheritance of this character and the ways in which it might change during evolution.

Two types of experiments have clearly demonstrated that birds possess innate information about their migratory direction.

Displacement experiments

In a classic field experiment, several thousand European starlings Sturnus vulgaris were displaced from autumn stop-over sites in Holland perpendicular to the expected migration route (Perdeck, 1958). The results have been comprehensively illustrated by Emlen (1975). Juvenile starlings, ringed and released singly after an 800 km aeroplane flight, continued to migrate from the release site in approximately the same direction as they had flown prior to displacement, i.e. westsouthwest. Adults, however, compensated for the displacement by migrating northwest towards the winter quarters in northern France and southern England, where they had spent at least one previous winter.

An earlier displacement experiment with white storks Ciconia ciconia (Schüz, 1951; reviewed by Helbig, 1993) had yielded similar results: nestlings of a southeast-migrating population (eastern Europe) were transferred to the breeding grounds of a southwest-migrating population (western Germany) and reared in captivity. Released after all the local storks had departed, these birds started to migrate towards the southeast, i.e. in the appropriate direction for their population of origin, but clearly on a different heading from that of the local breeding population.

Directional tendencies of hand-raised passerines in orientation cages

From the 1960s onwards, several studies of migratory orientation were conducted with passerine migrants hand-raised from a very early age (e.g. Emlen, 1969; Gwinner and Wiltschko, 1978). When tested in round orientation cages during the migration season, these birds on average hopped or fluttered in a direction that corresponded quite well to the migration direction known from recoveries of ringed birds of the respective species and population (Helbig, 1991a).

The conclusion that such spontaneous directional choices do indeed reflect genetically based migratory orientation was most clearly demonstrated by a comparison of two blackcap populations with very different autumn migratory directions (Helbig et al. 1989): nestlings from both populations were hand-reared and their orientation was later tested at the same site and under identical conditions. The directional choices of the test birds during the first half of the season differed by about 90 ° between the two groups, corresponding to the difference between the actual flight paths (known from ringing) of free-living individuals of these populations. Furthermore, birds from the eastern population showed a directional shift about midway through the migration season, whereas birds from the western population did not (Fig. 1). Both the sign (clockwise) and the amount of the shift (approximately 45 °) were in agreement with findings derived from recoveries of ringed blackcaps: birds from the eastern population were found to fly around the eastern edge of the Mediterranean Sea and they must alter flight direction southwards to reach their winter quarters in eastern Africa. The western population, in contrast, is known to follow a fairly straight course to winter quarters in the western Mediterranean region.

From both types of experiments, one must conclude that young birds, even in social species such as storks and starlings which normally migrate in flocks, do possess genetic information about the direction in which to migrate. They inherit from their parents not only a general starting direction but also a fairly detailed time-direction programme allowing for programmed changes of direction to circumvent potential barriers. To terminate migration after approximately the right distance, young birds also need a measure of distance to be flown: this may be partly encoded as the duration of endogenously produced migratory activity per season, a quantity that has been measured extensively in cage experiments and correlates roughly with the migration distances observed in the field (Gwinner, 1968; Berthold, 1973).

Taking together all the available evidence on innate components of migration, it is appropriate to term this type of behaviour vector orientation: direction and distance are encoded genetically, but there is no evidence that young birds possess genetic information about the geographic location of their winter quarters. Only after having spent some time in an appropriate wintering area are young birds able to navigate towards this specific goal area from an unknown location. The cues on which such ‘site imprinting’ is based and the processes by which it is established are so far poorly understood.

If imprinting on a wintering site is prevented by keeping hand-raised blackcaps in captivity throughout their first and second calendar year, seasonally appropriate migratory orientation can still be demonstrated during the second autumn (Fig. 2; Helbig, 1992a). Thus, even in birds unable to gain any migratory experience, the endogenous directional information is not lost.

A divergence between populations migrating towards different directions is a fairly common phenomenon in many bird species breeding in the Palearctic and other parts of the world. Detailed analyses of ringing recoveries (e.g. Zink, 1973–1985) have revealed such ‘migratory divides’ to be fairly sharp, not only in white stork and blackcap, but also in white wagtail Motacilla alba, bluethroat Luscinia svecica, willow warbler Phylloscopus trochilus and many others. Selection pressures leading to such behavioural differentiation are determined by the costs of migration (in terms of distance and the need to cross topographical and ecological barriers) and the geographical distribution of appropriate wintering habitats (Lundberg and Alerstam, 1986). To European long-distance migrants, the Alps, the Mediterranean Sea and the Sahara Desert constitute significant topographical and ecological barriers, making it advantageous to deviate either towards the east or towards the west from the great circle route to the intended winter quarters.

The blackcap is an appropriate model organism with which to study the inheritance of such differentiated migratory directions. Two groups of hand-raised blackcaps from east and west of the European migratory divide, whose orientation had already been tested in autumn (see Fig. 1), were cross-bred in outdoor aviaries during their second and third year of life (Helbig, 1991b). From 35 mixed pairs of southwest and southeast migrants, 68 F1 offspring were raised, whose orientation was again tested during the autumn and compared with that of the two parental groups (Fig. 3). The F1 generation oriented towards directions intermediate between those of the parental groups with no difference in the degree of scatter. Mean directions were significantly different among all three groups in October, and between F1 birds and the southwest-migrating parents in November, when the eastern parents had shifted their orientation to southsouthwest.

These results not only demonstrated directly the genetic basis of migratory directions through modification by cross-breeding, but also revealed a phenotypically intermediate mode of inheritance of this behavioural character. Some idea about the approximate number of genes involved in determining migratory directions can be gained from studying the expression of this trait in the F2 generation. In collaboration with P. Berthold and his coworkers at the Vogelwarte Radolfzell, we managed to breed in captivity 26 F2 offspring from seven pairs of F1 blackcaps. Subsequent orientation tests revealed a significant increase in scatter among the F2 individuals compared with the F1 individuals, all of which had been reared and tested under identical conditions (Fig. 4; A. J. Helbig and P. Berthold, unpublished data). Surprisingly, the range of directions displayed by the F2 group surpassed that of both parental groups from which the F1 group had been bred. This finding strongly indicates that only very few major genes are involved in determining innate migratory directions in this species. In fact, the results are hard to distinguish from what would be expected if only a single locus were responsible for the expression of this trait.

As with all inherited behaviour patterns, there is always an interaction between the genetically determined migratory direction and environmental factors influencing the bird’s actual migration course. The extent to which an individual will follow its genetic programme will depend on the degree of its exposure and individual susceptibility to environmental factors such as social influences (of flock members), topography, wind drift, etc. Experimental studies with hand-raised, caged migrants simplify the situation by subjecting all experimental birds to an approximately uniform and rather limited set of environmental influences, thus providing more conclusive information on the underlying genetic programme.

Blackcaps are distributed continuously across the central European migratory divide so that mixed matings between southeast-and southwest-migrating birds must be expected to occur. Offspring of such pairs would probably try to cross the Alps and Mediterranean Sea and might end up in very arid parts of northern Africa. Both the increased costs of such a migration and the lowered chances of surviving the winter compared with traditional winter quarters should act as a constant selection pressure against such a behaviour. Some evidence for the behaviour of birds breeding in the suspected contact zone between the two migratory populations north of the Alps is available from ringing recoveries and tests with hand-raised birds (Helbig, 1991c). If pure phenotypes mix frequently, a broad spectrum of flight directions between southeast and southwest would be expected. However, few recoveries point directly southwards, and hand-raised birds from the Linz area in Austria oriented predominantly westwards, suggesting that on a regional scale subpopulations have evolved migratory directions that allow them to avoid the mountain crossing. It thus appears that mixing of pure phenotypes, as simulated in the cross-breeding experiment described above, is relatively rare. This is confirmed by the observation that blackcaps, compared with their relative breeding abundance (the species is among the commonest forest and woodland breeding birds in Europe), are decidedly underrepresented among migrants crossing high alpine passes (Jenni and Naef-Daenzer, 1986). So far it is unclear at what geographic scale migratory adaptations are differentiated among blackcap and other bird populations. Gene flow should counteract such adaptation, but assortative mating based on different arrival times on the breeding grounds is a distinct possibility that would restrict gene flow between populations using different winter quarters (such as East Africa versus the western Mediterranean area). Unfortunately, no markers for birds of different winter origin are yet available that would allow us to test this hypothesis.

A new migration route of central European blackcaps to winter quarters in Britain and Ireland (as opposed to the western Mediterranean area) was discovered during the 1960s and 1970s from ringing recoveries (Zink, 1962; Schlenker, 1981). Prior to 1960, blackcaps were rare in winter in Britain, but in the late 1980s thousands of individuals wintered regularly, as was revealed by the nationwide surveys for the atlas of birds wintering in Britain (Lack, 1986). So far, there have been no ringing recoveries indicating a British or Scandinavian breeding origin of these wintering birds. However, many blackcaps ringed during the breeding season in Belgium, southern Germany and the western half of Austria have been recovered in a west to northwest direction, some of them in Britain and Ireland (Berthold and Schlenker, 1991); others even flew north to Scandinavia (Franssen and Stolt, 1993). For the latter, there is good evidence that they represent reverse migration (as suggested by Busse, 1987): blackcaps from east of the migratory divide (southeast migrants) mostly flew northwest, those from west of the divide (southwest migrants) mostly flew northeast, i.e. the opposite of their normal autumn migration direction (Franssen and Stolt, 1993). This explanation does not apply, however, to west and northwest migrants recovered in the British Isles, because almost all of them originated from areas in central Europe where predominantly southwest migrants breed.

It therefore appeared that a fraction of the originally southwest-migrating population had shifted its migration course towards the west and northwest and that the majority of British-wintering blackcaps originated from central Europe. To find out whether this new migration route had a genetic basis (rather than being due to increased wind drift or other environmental factors), it was necessary to breed offspring from blackcaps that had wintered in Britain. The autumn orientation of such birds by itself would be inconclusive, because it might reflect the birds’ ability to navigate towards winter quarters with which they had previous experience. We therefore caught a sample of 40 wintering blackcaps in southwest England, transferred them to Germany and bred them in captivity to test the migratory direction that their offspring had inherited (Berthold et al. 1992). The autumn orientation of 41 offspring thus produced was tested together with that of their parents and a control group from southwest Germany (expected to orient southwest). Both the parental birds that had wintered in southwest England and their captive-bred offspring did indeed orient towards the westnorthwest, i.e. in a significantly different direction from the southwest-orienting German control group (Fig. 5). Tracing the origin of British-wintering blackcaps by reversal of the autumn orientation they had inherited to their offspring indicates a breeding origin somewhere between Belgium and central Germany. These are areas where indeed the percentage of west to northwest migrants among blackcaps has increased from almost zero before 1960 to 7–11 % in local populations (Helbig, 1992b).

These data support the idea that part of the central European blackcap population has evolved a new migration route within only a few decades. This microevolutionary process is probably a geographically restricted frequency shift (of westnorthwest migrants) within the range of pre-existing genetic variation for migratory directions. The selection pressures that may favour wintering of continental blackcaps in Britain rather than in the western Mediterranean region may include the following. (1) Factors acting in the new wintering area such as progressively milder winters in recent decades, reduced competition compared with Mediterranean habitats and improved food supply during cold spells at bird tables provided by man, an important resource exploited by wintering blackcaps (Hardy, 1978), and at berry bushes planted in coastal areas and gardens. (2) Factors related to the geographic location of the new winter quarters: the migration distance is shorter by up to 1500 km and, owing to the earlier increase in daylength, British-wintering birds would be expected to return earlier to the breeding grounds (Berthold and Terrill, 1988). There is some experimental evidence supporting the latter proposition (Terrill and Berthold, 1990), and this has important implications for the understanding of such rapid microevolution: an earlier return to the breeding grounds of British versus Mediterranean wintering blackcaps should enable them to pair preferentially among each other (assortative mating based on differential arrival times), to occupy the best territories and to produce most offspring.

Apart from the rapid rate of evolution of this new migration route, it is surprising that British-breeding blackcaps apparently have not yet become partially resident. This is probably because abandoning migration altogether is a much more complex evolutionary process than just changing migratory direction and may require more time. Eventually, however, British blackcaps may evolve into a partially migratory or resident population, and a balance of competition between wintering migrants from the continent and residents may establish itself.

How can the rapid rate of evolution of the new migratory direction in some central European blackcaps be explained? Do we need to invoke a different genetic mechanism, perhaps involving a dominant allele that overrides the effect of other genes regulating migratory direction? It has also been suggested that the increase in the number of west to northwest migrants could be due to non-genetic factors. Myers (1992) argued that ‘xenobiotic oestrogens’ transmitted through the egg might lead to aberrant development of migratory orientation, which – although normally deleterious – in the case of westward migration to Britain happened to be viable. The novel migration behaviour would thus not reflect rapid microevolution, but rather the result of (in general harmful) environmental contamination. To exclude any such maternal transmission effect, sex-linked inheritance must be tested for.

To address these questions, we performed another cross-breeding experiment involving blackcaps from both fractions of the population, i.e. those migrating towards the southwest (the majority of the southern German population) and those migrating westnorthwest (those wintering in southern England), and tested the orientation of the offspring. Again the orientation of the F1 group was intermediate between, and significantly different from, the migratory directions of the two populations from which their parents originated (Helbig et al. 1994), thus confirming the result of the previous cross-breeding experiment between southwest-and southeast-migrating individuals (see above). F1 individuals were then divided into two groups depending on whether their mother was of German or British origin. The mean orientation of the two groups did not differ (mean directions 254 ° versus 252 °; P0.05, Hotelling two-sample test). The inheritance of orientation was therefore independent of whether an individual’s parents were a German female and a British-wintering male or vice versa.

The evidence shows that the west/northwest migration taking central European blackcaps to new winter quarters on the British Isles can be explained without invoking any novel genetic mechanism of inheritance. Neither can the rapid increase in the frequency of this behaviour be explained by the non-genetic influence of environmental contaminants transmitted maternally. Although some behavioural effects of contaminants have been demonstrated (Colborn and Clement, 1992), our results clearly reject this possibility as an explanation for the westward migration of central European blackcaps.

The findings reviewed here show that, in an evolutionary sense, migratory directions of birds are highly dynamic and, in principle, able to adapt to environmental changes in a relatively short period. However, whether a species is able to adapt and how quickly it does will depend on the population size, the amount of available genetic variation, the severity of selection pressures and, most of all, on the rapidity with which such pressures change. Having demonstrated a rapid evolutionary response and the underlying genetic basis in one species, one cannot assume that all other species will adapt as quickly or even adapt at all to the drastic, partly man-induced, environmental changes we are currently witnessing. We therefore need to investigate what population genetic mechanisms allow for such rapid evolution and what prerequisites must be fulfilled for a bird population to adapt successfully to changing conditions.

Financial support by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft through grants to P. Berthold and a research scholarship to the author is gratefully acknowledged. Many people helped with breeding and raising blackcaps. I particularly wish to thank P. Berthold, G. Mohr and U. Querner for their long-standing cooperation. K. Able, E. Gwinner, W. Harvey and M. Lehrer all provided valuable comments on an earlier version of this paper.

Berthold
,
P.
(
1973
).
Relationships between migratory restlessness and migration distance in six Sylvia species
.
Ibis
115
,
594
599
.
Berthold
,
P.
,
Helbig
,
A. J.
,
Mohr
,
G.
and
Querner
,
U.
(
1992
).
Rapid microevolution of migratory behaviour in a wild bird species
.
Nature
360
,
668
670
.
Berthold
,
P.
and
Schlenker
,
R.
(
1991
).
Sylvia atricapilla – Mönchsgrasmücke
. In
Handbuch der Vögel Mitteleuropas
, vol.
12/II
(ed. U. N.
Glutz
Von Blotzheim
and
K. M.
Bauer
), pp.
949
1020
. Wiesbaden: Aula-Verlag.
Berthold
,
P.
and
Terrill
,
S. B.
(
1988
).
Migratory behaviour and population growth of Blackcaps wintering in Britain and Ireland: some hypotheses
.
Ringing Migration
9
,
153
159
.
Busse
,
P.
(
1987
).
Migration patterns of European passerines
.
Sitta
1
,
18
36
.
Colborn
,
T.
and
Clement
,
C.
(
1992
).
(ed
.).
Chemically Induced Alterations in Sexual and Functional Development: The Wildlife/Human Connection
.
Princeton, NJ
:
Princeton Science Publishing Company
.
Emlen
,
S. T.
(
1969
).
The development of migratory orientation in young Indigo Buntings
.
Living Bird
8
,
113
126
.
Emlen
,
S. T.
(
1975
).
Migration: Orientation and navigation
. In
Avian Biology
, vol.
5
(ed.
D. S.
Farner
,
J. R.
King
and
K. C.
Parkes
), pp.
129
219
.
New York
:
Academic Press
.
Franssen
,
T.
and
Stolt
,
B.-O.
(
1993
).
Is there an autumn migration of continental Blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla) into northern Europe?
Vogelwarte
37
,
89
95
.
Gwinner
,
E.
(
1968
).
Artspezifische Muster der Zugunruhe bei Laubsängern und ihre mögliche Bedeutung für die Beendigung des Zuges im Winterquartier
.
Z. Tierpsychol.
25
,
843
853
.
Gwinner
,
E.
and
Wiltschko
,
W.
(
1978
).
Endogenously controlled changes in migratory direction of the Garden Warbler, Sylvia borin
.
J. comp. Physiol.
125
,
267
273
.
Hardy
,
E.
(
1978
).
Winter foods of Blackcaps in Britain
.
Bird Study
25
,
60
61
.
Helbig
,
A. J.
(
1991a
).
Experimental and analytical techniques used in bird orientation research
. In
Orientation in Birds
(ed.
P.
Berthold
), pp.
270
306
. Basel: Birkhäuser Verlag.
Helbig
,
A. J.
(
1991b
).
Inheritance of migratory direction in a bird species: a cross-breeding experiment with SE-and SW-migrating blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla)
.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.
28
,
9
12
.
Helbig
,
A. J.
(
1991c
).
SE-and SW-migrating Blackcap (Sylvia atricapilla) populations in Central Europe: Orientation of birds in the contact zone
.
J. evol. Biol
.
4
,
657
670
.
Helbig
,
A. J.
(
1992a
).
Ontogenetic stability of inherited migratory directions in a nocturnal bird migrant: comparison between the first and second year of life
.
Ethol. Ecol. Evol
.
4
,
375
388
.
Helbig
,
A. J.
(
1992b
).
Population differentiation of migratory directions in birds: comparison between ringing results and orientation behaviour of hand-raised migrants
.
Oecologia
90
,
483
488
.
Helbig
,
A. J.
(
1993
).
What do we know about the genetic basis of bird orientation?
J. Navig.
46
,
376
382
.
Helbig
,
A. J.
(
1994
).
Genetic basis and evolutionary change of migratory directions in a European passerine migrant Sylvia atricapilla
.
Ostrich
65
,
151
159
.
Helbig
,
A. J.
,
Berthold
,
P.
,
Mohr
,
G.
and
Querner
,
U.
(
1994
).
Inheritance of a novel migratory direction in central European Blackcaps (Aves: Sylvia atricapilla)
.
Naturwissenschaften
81
,
184
186
.
Helbig
,
A. J.
,
Berthold
,
P.
and
Wiltschko
,
W.
(
1989
).
Migratory orientation of Blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla): population-specific shifts of direction during the autumn
.
Ethology
82
,
307
315
.
Jenni
,
L.
and
Naef-Daenzer
,
B.
(
1986
).
Vergleich der Fanghäufigkeiten von Zugvögeln auf dem Alpenpass Col de Bretolet mit Brutbeständen im Herkunftsgebiet
.
Orn. Beob
.
83
,
95
110
.
Lack
,
P.
(
1986
).
The Atlas of Wintering Birds in Britain and Ireland
.
Calton
:
Poyser
.
Lundberg
,
S.
and
Alerstam
,
T.
(
1986
).
Bird migration patterns: conditions for stable geographical population segregation
.
J. theor. Biol.
123
,
403
414
.
Myers
,
J. P.
(
1992
).
Facts, inferences and shameless speculations
.
Am. Birds
46
,
1082
1083
.
Perdeck
,
A. C.
(
1958
).
Two types of orientation in migrating Starlings, Sturnus vulgaris L. and Chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs L., as revealed by displacement experiments
.
Ardea
46
,
1
37
.
Schlenker
,
R.
(
1981
).
Verlagerung der Zugwege von Teilen der südwestdeutschen und österreichischen Mönchsgrasmücken (Sylvia atricapilla) – Population
.
Ökol. Vögel
3
,
314
318
.
Schüz
,
E.
(
1951
).
Überblick über die Orientierungsversuche der Vogelwarte Rossitten
.
Proceedings of the Xth International Ornithological Congress, Uppsala, 1950, pp. 249–268
.
Terrill
,
S. B.
and
Berthold
,
P.
(
1990
).
Ecophysiological aspects of rapid population growth in a novel migratory Blackcap (Sylvia atricapilla) population: an experimental approach
.
Oecologia
85
,
266
270
.
Zink
,
G.
(
1962
).
Eine Mönchsgrasmücke (Sylvia atricapilla) zieht im Herbst von Oberösterreich nach Irland
.
Vogelwarte
21
,
222
223
.
Zink
,
G.
(
1973–1985
).
Der Zug europäischer Singvögel. Ein Atlas der Wiederfunde beringter Vögel, vols 1–4. Möggingen: Vogelzug-Verlag
.