The influence of interactions between the antagonistic centre and surround areas of receptive fields on the electrical activities of medulla neurones was examined in the larval swallowtail butterfly Papilio xuthus. Weak signals from the surround had a significant depressive effect on the maintained discharge, which increased for on-centre cells or decreased for off-centre cells in response to illumination of the centre. Moderation of the maintained discharge suppressed saturation of the response and extended the graded response range of the neurones. Surround illumination also reduced irregular fluctuations in the membrane potentials and variability in the discharge rate of impulses. The results indicate that the spatial inhibitory mechanism is important for the larval visual system in order to increase the efficiency of signal processing.

Visual interneurones in both vertebrates and invertebrates have antagonistic areas within the receptive field; such areas in vertebrates usually have a concentric centre-surround structure. Antagonistic or inhibitory signals from the receptive field surround are important for the enhancement of contrast and flicker (Hartline et al. 1956; Ratliff et al. 1967) and for the adjustment of sensitivity or threshold (Barlow and Levick, 1976; Werblin, 1974; Werblin and Copenhagen, 1974; Thibos and Werblin, 1978). Lateral antagonism is also a fundamental neural mechanism for movement detection and is involved in colour coding (Barlow and Levick, 1965; Zrenner, 1983). In addition, a steady signal from the surround controls the maintained activities of spiking neurones; the level and variability of the spiking activities determine the response range and the signal-to-noise ratio of the neurones (Barlow and Levick, 1969a,b; Enroth-Cugell and Lennie, 1975). Insect visual systems contain interneurones with antagonistic receptive fields in the lamina (Arnett, 1971; Dubs, 1982; Laughlin, 1974; Menzel, 1974; Mimura, 1976; Zettler and Järvilehto, 1972) and in the medulla and lobula (Hertel, 1980; Honegger, 1980; Ichikawa, 1986; Kien and Menzel, 1977; Osorio, 1986, 1987), although in no case do the antagonistic receptive fields have a concentric form. Antagonistic signals from the surround area of the receptive field appear to be weak (e.g. Arnett, 1972; Ichikawa, 1986). A detailed analysis of the effects of such a weak signal on visual processing can be difficult because stray light from the test light shed on the surround affects the neighbouring central area of the receptive field, which dominates the response profiles of the neurones.

Butterfly larvae have an eye consisting of a small number of receptive units (six stemmata) with a large diameter, so we can stimulate individual units independently (Ichikawa, 1986). Although the number of optical units is greatly reduced, the larval visual system appears to be constructed according to the same architectural principles as the adult visual system with compound eyes (Ichikawa and Tateda, 1984; Toh and Iwasaki, 1982). These features make the larval visual system an interesting model for insect vision. The spatial and chromatic properties of the majority of neurones in the medulla of larval swallowtail butterflies have been characterized (Ichikawa, 1986, 1990, 1991). Many medulla neurones produced a strong, dominant response to stimulation of one particular stemma (centre) and a weak, antagonistic response to stimulation of other stemmata (surround). Because the receptive field of the stemmata as a whole almost covers a hemisphere of the visual space (Ichikawa and Tateda, 1982), a large area of the space forms the receptive field surround of these neurones. Stemmata located in close proximity to the centre stemma usually make a larger contribution to the surround response (Ichikawa, 1986). The receptive field centre is most important for colour coding, but the surround appears to play little or no role in colour coding (Ichikawa, 1986, 1990). In this paper, I report that illumination of the antagonistic surround affects the spiking activities of medulla neurones and increases the efficiency of signal coding in neurones. The present study demonstrates that the principles of lateral inhibition extend to the simple eye of the larval visual system.

The preparation and the methods of recording and stimulation have been described in previous papers (Ichikawa, 1990, 1991). Fifth-instar larvae of the swallowtail butterfly Papilio xuthus L. were reared in our laboratory. After the mouthparts had been plugged with a quick-drying glue, the head of the larva was fixed with beeswax to an experimental chamber. The chamber was then filled with a physiological saline (in mmol I−1 NaCl, 4; KC1, 40; MgCl2,18; CaCl2, 3; glucose, 150; pH 6.5 with 2.5 mmol l−1 KH2PO4-KHCO3) and all of the head except for the right lateral part bearing the stemmata was placed in this saline bath. The brain was exposed by removing the frontal part of the cuticle of the head capsule. Circumoesophageal connectives were cut to reduce the transmission of movement of the oesophagus to the brain. A pair of stainless-steel insect pins was placed under the brain to serve as a platform. The tip of a glass pipette containing 1% Pronase was placed on the brain for 20–30 s to facilitate penetration by a glass pipette microelectrode. The microelectrode, filled with Imoll−1 potassium acetate (30–50M Ω), was inserted into the somata region of the medulla neurones between two imaginai discs (Ichikawa and Tateda, 1984). An indifferent electrode was placed in the saline bath. Intracellular responses were amplified in the conventional manner and recorded simultaneously on magnetic tape and a chart recorder.

Each stemma was illuminated independently via a quartz optical fibre (150 μm in diameter), the tip of which was placed within 50μm of the surface of the corneal lens of the stemma. White light from a 500 W xenon arc lamp and monochromatic test light from a grating monochromator (Bausch & Lomb) equipped with a 150 W xenon arc lamp were introduced into each optical fibre with the aid of a small quartz half-mirror. Monochromatic light for the second test stimuli was obtained by interposing an interference filter with a transmission peak at 370, 450 or 580 nm into the pathway from the former light source. A rotary metal disc with various patterns of holes was placed in each light path to select the stemmata to be illuminated. The duration of illumination was controlled by a mechanical shutter. The intensity of the light was regulated with quartz neutral density filters and a quartz neutral density wedge. The light intensities were measured by a radiometer or a thermopile. The reference intensity of white light (logIw=0.0) corresponded to 3.2Wm−2, and that of monochromatic light (log Im=0.0) to 2.4×1013quanta cm−2 s−J. Contamination of adjacent stemmata by stray light scattered from a light path was determined in preliminary experiments by measuring photoreceptor responses and was less than −3 log units.

After successful penetration of a medulla neurone in the dark, six stemmata were separately illuminated with monochromatic test lights of a constant intensity (2.4×1011 quanta cm−2 s−1) in order to identify the neurone (Ichikawa, 1990). The maintained discharge of the cell was analyzed after waiting for the activity of the units to become practically constant for 10 min in the dark or for l min with background illumination. Intervals between successive impulses for off-centre cells were analyzed using an interspike-interval analyzer (Nihon Kohden, DAB-1100) with a time resolution of 0.1ms. For on-centre cells with relatively small spikes, a train of impulses was recorded on a chart recorder and the intervals were measured by eye.

Medulla neurones with an antagonistic receptive field have been classed into 16 spectral types (Ichikawa, 1990). Spectral types of neurones used for the present analyses were UV+bg+ (‘on-centre’ unit) and Gb+ (‘off-centre’ unit). Excitatory responses of the former were dominated by ultraviolet-sensitive photoreceptors, while inhibitory responses of the latter were dominated by green-sensitive photoreceptors (Ichikawa, 1986). These neurones were frequently encountered and showed a consistent antagonistic response. Fig. 1A shows typical responses from an on-centre cell exhibiting excitation in response to illumination of stemma V (centre) and inhibition in response to illumination of the other stemmata (surround). Fig. IB shows an off-centre cell that exhibits an inhibitory response to stimulation of stemma I (centre) and an excitatory response to stimulation of the other stemmata (surround). Fig. 2 shows the response of the on-centre unit to a 1 s stimulus presented to the surround in the presence of a 3s centre illumination. In order to demonstrate clearly the effect of illumination of the receptive field surround on the activity of the unit, the intensity of light at the centre was decreased to evoke an approximately half-maximal response and the intensity of light to the surround was increased so that it was 2 log units greater than that for the centre. The light shed on the surround reduced both the discharge rate of impulses and the amplitude of slow depolarization. It also reduced the fluctuation (noise) of the membrane potentials that increased as a result of the illumination of the centre. The predominance of the centre was apparent from the incomplete suppression of the excitatory centre response by surround stimulation. Fig. 3 shows results of similar experiments obtained by varying the intensity of surround stimulation in the presence of centre illumination of a constant intensity. The numbers of impulses during a Is surround stimulation decreased linearly with increasing intensity of the surround stimuli. As the three curves are almost parallel, there is probably a subtractive interaction between the centre and surround mechanisms. When the intensity of illumination of the centre was −4.1 log units, at which point the cell showed about half-maximal response to excitation, the surround required stronger illumination, approximately 3 log units higher, to cancel the centre response. At higher intensities of centre illumination (logI=−3.7, −3.1), the strongest stimulus applied to the surround did not extinguish responses from the centre.

The rate of spike discharge of medulla neurones during prolonged background illumination was usually maintained at a constant level and depended on the intensity of the background illumination. Fig. 4 shows the rates of the maintained discharge of the on-and off-centre cells, at various intensities of background illumination, on the centre alone or on the whole receptive field (centre plus surround). The maintained discharge when illumination was on the centre alone increased or decreased faster than when the whole receptive field was illuminated.

The difference between the curves represents the contribution of the surround. The antagonistic surround mechanism has an effect equivalent to that of shifting the maintained discharge curve along the abscissa by 1 or 2 log units to a higher intensity. The responsiveness of an on-centre unit at different background configurations is shown in Fig. 5. In this experiment Is monochromatic stimuli (370 nm) were presented to the receptive field centre to test the sensitivity of the cell. It was apparent that background illumination of the surround acted to release the cell from a compression of the graded response range that results from an elevation of the maintained discharge level caused by illumination of the centre. Similar results were obtained from off-centre cells (data not shown). Strong background illumination shed on the centre of on-centre cells often made the maintained discharge intermittent; the intensity-response functions were saturated at a lower level and declined with further increases in the stimulus intensity (Fig. 5). This abnormal behaviour of the cell suggests an important role for surround antagonism in the maintenance of the normal activity of medulla neurones. It is possible that surround illumination changes the response threshold, as was noted in mudpuppy retinal bipolar and ganglion cells (Thibos and Werblin, 1978). Brief flashes (50 ms in duration) at an intensity near threshold were repeatedly shone on the centre in the presence or absence of steady background illumination on the surround. Summed responses obtained at different background configurations showed no significant differences.

Surround illumination reduced irregular fluctuations in the membrane potential and caused the firing of the spikes to become fairly regular. Variability of the maintained discharge was examined by measuring intervals between successive spikes in the presence of a steady background illumination on the centre alone or on both the centre and the surround. Fig. 6 shows histograms of such interspike intervals in the two types of cells. The surround illumination increased the mean of the intervals for the on-centre unit and decreased that for the off-centre unit. The standard deviation of the distribution of interspike intervals (S.D.) for both cells decreased. The relative variability or coefficient of variation, which was defined as S.D./mean, was also decreased by surround illumination. Fig. 7 shows the coefficient of variation as a function of background illumination. As illumination of the centre increased, the coefficient of variation for both cells first increased, reaching a maximum at illumination levels of −5 or −4 log units, and then declined to the level seen in the dark-adapted state. The depressive effect of the surround on the coefficient of variation was insignificant or weak at lower levels of background illumination (<−4.01ogunits), but was evident at higher background levels (>−3 log units). Similar results were obtained from three on- and four off-cells. Surround illumination at -2.0 log units decreased the coefficient of variation by 0.09±0.03 (on-centre cells) or 0.12±0.05 (off-centre cells).

A class of visual interneurones in the fly lamina has antagonistic areas in the receptive field, an on-region (centre) and an off-region (surround) (Arnett, 1971). The off-region of the lamina neurone needs 1 log unit brighter illumination than does the on-region in order to extinguish completely the on-discharge elicited by stimulation of the on-region (Arnett, 1972). The surround of the receptive field of butterfly larval medulla neurones needed much more intense illumination, that is 3 log units or more, to produce a similar result (Fig. 3). Although such a weak signal from the surround appeared to be trivial, it had a functionally significant influence on the rate and the variability of the maintained discharge (Figs 4, 6, 7). The rate of the maintained discharge limits the response range of the neurones (Fig. 5), while the variability affects the sensitivity or threshold of the cells (Barlow and Levick, 1969a).

Depression of the maintained discharge by the receptive field surround at various levels of background luminance has been examined in cat retinal ganglion cells (Enroth-Cugell and Lennie, 1975; Enroth-Cugell et al. 1975). The maintained discharge rate of the cells could not usually be related to the background luminance by a monotonic function and the contribution of the surround to the maintained discharge varied from one cell (type) to another. The surround for some cells (the X-cells) had a relatively consistent depressing influence on the maintained discharge, either at a wide or a limited range of background luminance; for some cells (the Y-cells) the surround effects were weak or inconsistent (Enroth-Cugell and Lennie, 1975). These complex centre-surround antagonisms may be due to lateral interactions that occur at multiple layers of the retina (Werblin, 1974; Werblin and Copenhagen, 1974). The maintained discharge of the larval medulla neurones changed monotonically with the background illumination and the surround had a consistent, subtractive effect on the maintained discharge of the neurones at intensities above λ6.0 log units (Fig. 4). Thus, the centre-surround antagonism may be fairly constant over a wide range of illumination and possibly occurs at a single stage of the visual pathway in the optic lobe.

The principal source of variations in the interspike interval of medulla neurones may be the fluctuations in generator potential of photoreceptors, as suggested from work in an eccentric cell in the lateral eye of Limulus polyphemus (Ratliff et al. 1968; Shapley, 1971a). Fig. 8 shows voltage fluctuations of the generator potential in the soma of a photoreceptor cell in the butterfly larval eye under conditions of different background illumination. Small discrete potentials are seen in the dark (Ichikawa and Tateda, 1980). The amplitudes of the irregular fluctuation of membrane potentials caused by random absorption of photons increased with an increase in the intensity of illumination, became maximal at −6 or −5 log units and then declined. The change in the variability of photoreceptor membrane potential with background intensity appears to be similar to that for the coefficient of variation of interspike intervals of medulla neurones (Fig. 7). However, fluctuations of photoreceptor membrane potential differ at −7 log units and at −2 log units, where the medulla neurones show similar coefficients of variation. Thus, in addition to the fluctuation in membrane potential of photoreceptors, other factors, such as intrinsic noise generated during neural transmission and filtering properties of visual pathways to the medulla neurones, may affect variability in the discharge of neurones.

Lateral inhibition has been observed in a variety of visual systems and fulfils a number of roles in signal processing (see Introduction). The present study extends this generalization to the larval visual system with stemmata. The mechanisms of the inhibition have been intensively studied in the eccentric cells of the lateral eye of Limulus. The inhibition of these cells is mediated by axon collaterals (Hartline et al. 1956). Although the inhibitory mechanism reduced the mean rate of maintained discharge of the cells, it usually increased the variability or the relative variability of the spike discharge rate (Shapley, 1971b). This occurs because the inhibitory signals, in the form of spikes, add low-frequency fluctuations to the membrane potentials of a postsynaptic cell. The surround of larval medulla neurones invariably reduced the relative variability of the maintained discharge rate (Fig. 7). Antagonistic signals from the surround may be produced by summing weak graded signals from many photoreceptors in the surround stemmata. The neural mechanisms responsible for the antagonistic interactions in the larval optic lobe remain to be examined (Toh and Iwasaki, 1982).

This work was supported in part by grant from the Ministry of Education, Science and Culture. I thank M. Ohara for reading the manuscript.

Arnett
,
D. W.
(
1971
).
Receptive field organization of units in the first optic ganglion of Diptera
.
Science
173
,
929
931
.
Arnett
,
D. W.
(
1972
).
Spatial and temporal integration properties of units in first optic ganglion of Dipterans
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
35
,
429
444
.
Barlow
,
H. B.
and
Levick
,
W. R.
(
1965
).
The mechanism of directionally selective units in the rabbit’s retina
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
178
,
477
504
.
Barlow
,
H. B.
and
Levick
,
W. R.
(
1969a
).
Three factors limiting the reliable detection of light by retinal ganglion cells of the cat
.
J. Physiol., Load
.
200
,
1
24
.
Barlow
,
H. B.
and
Levick
,
W. R.
(
1969b
).
Changes in the maintained discharge with adaptation level in the cat retina
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
202
,
699
718
.
Barlow
,
H. B.
and
Levick
,
W. R.
(
1976
).
Threshold setting by the surround of cat retinal ganglion cells
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
259
,
737
757
.
Dubs
,
A.
(
1982
).
The spatial integration of signals in the retina and lamina of the fly compound eye under different conditions of luminance
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
146
,
321
343
.
Enroth-Cugell
,
C.
and
Lennie
,
P.
(
1975
).
The control of retinal ganglion cell discharge by receptive field surrounds
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
247
,
551
578
.
Enroth-Cugell
,
C.
,
Lennie
,
P.
and
Shapley
,
R. M.
(
1975
).
Surround contribution to light adaptation in cat retinal ganglion cells
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
247
,
579
588
.
Hartline
,
H. K.
,
Wagner
,
H. G.
and
Ratliff
,
F.
(
1956
).
Inhibition in the eye of Limulus
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
39
,
651
673
.
Hertel
,
H.
(
1980
).
Chromatic properties of identified interneurons in optic lobes of the bee
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
137
,
215
231
.
Honegger
,
H. W.
(
1980
).
Receptive fields of sustained medulla neurones in crickets
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
136
,
191
201
.
Ichikawa
,
T.
(
1986
).
Color and spatial antagonism in the visual system of the larval swallowtail butterfly
.
Brain Res
.
397
,
381
385
.
Ichikawa
,
T.
(
1990
).
Spectral sensitivities of elementary color-coded neurones in butterfly larva
.
J. Neurophysiol
.
64
,
1861
1872
.
Ichikawa
,
T.
(
1991
).
Integration of color signals in the medulla of swallowtail butterfly larva
.
J. exp. Biol
.
155
,
127
145
.
Ichikawa
,
T.
and
Tateda
,
H.
(
1980
).
Cellular patterns and spectral sensitivity of larval ocelli in the swallowtail butterfly Papilio
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
139
,
41
47
.
Ichikawa
,
T.
and
Tateda
,
H.
(
1982
).
Receptive field of the stemmata in the swallowtail butterfly Papilio
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
146
,
191
199
.
Ichikawa
,
T.
and
Tateda
,
H.
(
1984
).
Termination profiles of photoreceptor cells in the larval eye of the swallowtail butterfly
.
J. Neurocytol
.
13
,
227
238
.
Kien
,
J.
and
Menzel
,
R.
(
1977
).
Chromatic properties of interneurones in the optic lobes of the bee. II. Narrow band and colour opponent neurons
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
113
,
35
53
.
Laughlin
,
S. B.
(
1974
).
Neural integration in the first optic neuropile of dragonflies. III. The transfer of angular information
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
92
,
377
396
.
Menzel
,
R.
(
1974
).
Spectral sensitivity of monopolar cells in the bee lamina
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
93
,
337
346
.
Mimura
,
K.
(
1976
).
Some spatial properties in the first optic ganglion of the fly
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
105
,
65
82
.
Osorio
,
D.
(
1986
).
Ultraviolet sensitivity and spectral opponency in the locust
.
J. exp. Biol
.
122
,
193
208
.
Osorio
,
D.
(
1987
).
Temporal and spectral properties of sustaining cells in the medulla of the locust
.
J. comp. Physiol
.
161
,
441
448
.
Ratliff
,
F.
,
Hartline
,
H. K.
and
Lange
,
D.
(
1968
).
Variability of interspike intervals in optic nerve fibers of Limulus: effect of light and dark adaptation
.
Proc. natn. Acad. Sci. U.S.A
.
60
,
464
469
.
Ratliff
,
F.
,
Knight
,
B. W.
,
Toyoda
,
J.
and
Hartline
,
H. K.
(
1967
).
Enhancement of flicker by lateral inhibition
.
Science
158
,
392
393
.
Shapley
,
R.
(
1971a
).
Fluctuations of the impulse rate in Limulus eccentric cells
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
57
,
539
556
.
Shapley
,
R.
(
1971b
).
Effects of lateral inhibition on fluctuations of the impulse rate
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
57
,
557
575
.
Thibos
,
L. N.
and
Werblin
,
F. S.
(
1978
).
The response properties of the steady antagonistic surround in the mudpuppy retina
.
J. Physiol., Lond
.
278
,
79
99
.
Toh
,
Y.
and
Iwasaki
,
M.
(
1982
).
Ocellar system of the swallowtail butterfly larva. II. Projection of retinular axons in the brain
.
J. Ultrastruct. Res
.
78
,
120
135
.
Werblin
,
F. S.
(
1974
).
Control of retinal sensitivity. II. Lateral interactions at the outer plexiform layer
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
63
,
62
87
.
Werblin
,
F. S.
and
Copenhagen
,
D. R.
(
1974
).
Control of retinal sensitivity. III. lateral interactions at the inner plexiform layer
.
J. gen. Physiol
.
63
,
88
110
.
Zettler
,
F.
and
Järvilehto
,
M.
(
1972
).
Lateral inhibition in an insect eye
.
Z. vergl. Physiol
.
76
,
233
244
.
Zrenner
,
E.
(
1983
).
Neurophysiological Aspects of Color Vision in Primates
.
Berlin
:
SpringerVerlag
.